Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:45:57.895Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From ocean to emotion: a pilot study exploring acute mood effects following consumption of a DHA-rich powder compared with placebo in middle-aged Australian men

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 December 2024

Jeffery M. Reddan*
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Sarah Gauci
Affiliation:
Food & Mood Centre, The Institute for Mental and Physical Health and Clinical Translation (IMPACT), School of Medicine, Deakin University, Geelong, VIC, Australia
Lauren M. Young
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Greg Kennedy
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Renee Rowsell
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
Anne Marie Minihane
Affiliation:
Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
Andrew Scholey
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia Department of Nutrition, Dietetics and Food, Monash University, Notting Hill, VIC, Australia
Andrew Pipingas
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences, Swinburne University of Technology, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
*
Corresponding author: Jeffery M. Reddan; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

While there is evidence that long-chain n-3 PUFA supplementation benefits mood, the extent to which a single high dose of n-3 PUFA can induce acute mood effects has not been examined. The present study investigated whether a single dose of a DHA-rich powder affects self-reported mood in middle-aged males during elevated cognitive demand. In a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial with a balanced crossover design, twenty-nine healthy males (age M = 52.8 years, sd = 5.3) were administered a powder (in a meal) containing 4·74 g n-3 PUFA (DHA 4020 mg; EPA 720 mg) or placebo in random order on two different testing days separated by a washout period of 7 ± 3 d. Participants completed mood assessments before and after completing two cognitive test batteries at baseline and again 3·5–4·0 h following the consumption of the active treatment or placebo. While completion of the cognitive test batteries increased negative mood, differential effects for alertness (P = 0·008) and stress (P = 0·04) followed consumption of the DHA-rich powder compared with placebo. Although alertness declined when completing the cognitive batteries, it was higher following consumption of the DHA-rich powder compared with placebo (P = 0·006). Conversely, stress was lower following consumption of the DHA-rich powder relative to placebo, though this difference only approached significance (P = 0·05). Overall, results from this pilot study demonstrate that a single high dose of n-3 PUFA may deliver acute mood benefits following elevated cognitive demand in healthy middle-aged males.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Mood is a subjective emotional state that plays an important role in maintaining well-being, which in many people is clinically disordered. Mood disorders such as depression are among the leading causes of disease burden worldwide, accounting for 125·3 million disability-adjusted life years(1) and a point prevalence of 12·9 % within the general population(Reference Lim, Tam and Lu2). However, this does not account for the burden of ‘subthreshold’ disordered mood within the general population, such as subthreshold depression (approximately 11 % prevalence(Reference Zhang, Peng and Song3)) and generalised anxiety (approximately 4·4 % prevalence(Reference Haller, Cramer and Lauche4)). Interestingly, one commonly cited reason for the use of complementary medicines, such as dietary supplements, in the general population is to help manage chronic diseases(Reference Reid, Steel and Wardle5), including mental health issues such as disordered mood(Reference Lavretsky6,Reference Ravindran and da Silva7) . Research from the field of nutritional psychiatry suggests that nutrition plays an important role in maintaining mood and mental well-being(Reference Marx, Lane and Hockey8). There is increasing evidence that adherence to more healthful dietary patterns, such as a Mediterranean-style diet, is associated with a reduced risk of developing poor mental health(Reference Firth, Marx and Dash9); conversely, evidence indicates that adherence to Western diets high in ultra-processed foods are associated with an increased risk of poor mental health(Reference Lane, Gamage and Travica10). In addition to whole dietary patterns, greater chronic intake of specific nutrients, such as B vitamins, vitamin D and long-chain n-3 PUFA (ω-3 PUFA), have been found to be related to a reduced risk of mood disorders(Reference Akpınar and Karadağ11Reference Grosso, Galvano and Marventano14). This suggests a role for dietary interventions, including specific dietary supplements, to benefit mood.

The ω-3 PUFAs – DHA and EPA – are essential for healthy brain function(Reference Denis, Potier and Vancassel15,Reference Shahidi and Ambigaipalan16) . Brain tissue is highly enriched in DHA relative to most other organs, with well-defined roles including maintaining cell membrane integrity and function, neuronal plasticity, neurogenesis and the regulation of neuroinflammation and β-amyloid clearance(Reference Pontifex, Vauzour and Minihane17). EPA has also been shown to be particularly important in glial cell functionality(Reference Bazinet, Metherel and Chen18). However, because endogenous biosynthesis of these fatty acids is low, it is recommended that they be obtained from external dietary sources. Studies exploring dietary sources of ω-3 PUFA have shown improved mood outcomes and reduced risk of developing mental health disorders with greater consumption(Reference Larrieu and Layé19) but also that supplementation benefits clinically disordered mood(Reference Kelaiditis, Gibson and Dyall20Reference Su, Tseng and Lin22). However, the heterogeneity between clinical trials demonstrates the need for more high-quality randomised controlled trials(Reference Kelaiditis, Gibson and Dyall20,Reference Liao, Xie and Zhang21) .

As indicated above, there is increasing interest regarding the extent to which longer-term dietary behaviours are associated with healthier mood. However, there is also growing interest as to whether dietary interventions are capable of delivering benefits to subjective mood in the hours after ingestion. Indeed, there are data indicating that a single serving/dose of specific foods or dietary bioactives can induce immediate effects on biological processes such as cardiovascular function(Reference Boon, Croft and Shinde23Reference McManus, Tejera and Awwad25), cerebral blood flow(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey26,Reference Gratton, Weaver and Burley27) and inflammation(Reference Davis, Tallent and Navalta28,Reference Schell, Betts and Lyons29) . It may be that acute effects upon these or other biological processes mediate subsequent acute effects upon subjective mood following a single dose of various dietary extracts or compounds. Examples of dietary extracts shown to benefit mood in the hours following consumption include coffeeberry(Reference Jackson, Haskell-Ramsay and Forster30), apple(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey26), saffron(Reference Pouchieu, Pourtau and Brossaud31) and blackcurrant(Reference Watson, Haskell-Ramsay and Kennedy32) extracts. Acute mood effects have also been reported following single doses of cocoa flavanols(Reference Boolani, Lindheimer and Loy33Reference Scholey, French and Morris35), flavonoid-rich orange juice(Reference Alharbi, Lamport and Dodd36), wild berry drink(Reference Khalid, Barfoot and May37), decaffeinated coffeeberry(Reference Reed, Mitchell and Saunders38), green coffee(Reference Camfield, Silber and Scholey39,Reference Cropley, Croft and Silber40) and tryptophan-rich hydrolysed protein(Reference Firk and Markus41). Various nootropic formulations have also been shown to benefit mood in the hours after a single dose(Reference Medrano, Molina-Hidalgo and Alcantara42Reference Scholey, Downey and Ciorciari45). Importantly, many of these studies specifically report that these dietary bioactives exerted a mitigating effect upon negative mood change occurring in response to elevated cognitive demand(Reference Massee, Ried and Pase34,Reference Scholey, French and Morris35,Reference Philip, Sagaspe and Taillard46) . In a study of 50- to 75-year-old women, a single dose of a multivitamin, mineral and herbal supplement was found to improve overall mood rating as scored by the DASS (depression anxiety stress scale), reduce ratings of stress and increase the rating of calmness measured using a visual analogue scale(Reference Macpherson, Rowsell and Cox47). However, other dietary supplement studies have found no such effects upon acute mood following a single dose(Reference Kennedy, Bonnländer and Lang48Reference Jackson, Kenney and Forster50).

To date, there are no published studies examining the acute impact of a single dose of ω-3 PUFA on mood outcomes in the hours following consumption. The paucity of such studies is intriguing, especially given prior evidence that acute mood effects are possible following consumption of a single dose of other nutritional products, but also evidence that ω-3 PUFA supplementation can support healthier mood(Reference Larrieu and Layé19Reference Su, Tseng and Lin22,Reference White, de Klerk and Woods44,Reference Scholey, Downey and Ciorciari45) . Therefore, the present study aimed to examine whether a single high dose of ω-3 PUFA, specifically a DHA-rich powder incorporated into a meal, can affect mood in response to elevated cognitive demand in healthy middle-aged males.

Methods

This pilot trial utilised a double-blind, placebo-controlled balanced crossover design. This was classified as a pilot trial as this is the first to assess the acute impact of a single dose of ω-3 PUFA on mood outcomes; therefore, the sample size and dosage were exploratory in nature. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in the study. The study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Swinburne University Human Research Ethics Committee (2018/160). The trial (‘Post-prandial cognitive and vascular effects of a DHA-rich n-3 powder in healthy middle-aged males’) was registered with the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry (ACTRN12618001160224; www.anzctr.org.au/Trial/Registration/TrialReview.aspx?ACTRN = 12618001160224) and was conducted from July 2018 to April 2021. The primary outcomes from this trial have been previously reported(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24). In summary, no significant differential treatment effects were observed for cognitive performance. However, a significant treatment by time interaction was apparent for aortic systolic blood pressure (F = 5·95, P = 0·022), demonstrating that a reduction in aortic systolic blood pressure (pre-dose to post-dose) was apparent following consumption of the ω-3 PUFA treatment (mean difference = –4·11 mmHg, P = 0·004) but not placebo (mean difference = –1·39 mmHg, P = 0·12). Furthermore, we observed the expected increase in plasma DHA following the consumption of the DHA-rich powder. Both these findings indicate that acute physiological effects were achievable in the hours after consuming the DHA-rich powder.

Participants

Male participants aged between 40 and 60 years were recruited from the local community. As this was a pilot study, with cognitive function as the primary outcome(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24), recruitment was limited to males, controlling for sex differences to achieve a more homogenous study cohort(Reference Kheloui, Brouillard and Rossi51,Reference Lejbak, Crossley and Vrbancic52) . Eligibility criteria included being free from CVD or uncontrolled hypertension (systolic > 160 mmHg and/or diastolic > 90 mmHg), type 1 diabetes or pharmaceutically managed type 2 diabetes, recent head trauma, dementia or cognitive impairment (Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score < 24)(Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh53), a history of neurological conditions (e.g. stroke) and mood or psychiatric disorders, as well as any gastrointestinal, endocrine or bleeding disorders. Participants were also excluded if they were current smokers or taking nicotine in other forms (including the use of nicotine-containing products such as patches, gum or ‘vapes’) or using any dietary supplements or medications expected to influence cognitive function. Consumption of more than one serving of oily fish/seafood per week was also an exclusion criterion (participants were also instructed to avoid consuming fish/seafood the night prior to experimental testing).

Overall, 110 possible participants underwent telephone screening, with thirty-six completing subsequent in-person screening at the Centre for Human Psychopharmacology (now Centre for Mental Health and Brain Sciences) lab at Swinburne University of Technology (Melbourne, Australia). Following in-person screening, thirty-two participants were randomised and completed the trial. Randomisation of treatment order was performed by an employee in our research centre not involved with the present study using a computer random number generator. The randomisation key was not accessed by study staff until the formal analysis was complete. Due to missing or incomplete data for three participants, only twenty-nine participants were included in the final analysis. Figure 1 provides an overview of the crossover design of the trial and reports on the flow of participants through the trial.

Figure 1. Overview of trial design, participant recruitment and retention.

Investigational product

The experimental treatment was a microencapsulated tuna oil powder (Driphorm® HiDHA®360), manufactured and supplied by Nu-Mega Ingredients Limited. Each 12 g serving provided 4·74 g ω-3 PUFA (4020 mg DHA and 720 mg EPA)(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24). A spray-dried powder containing sunflower oil, matched in appearance to the experimental treatment, was also utilised as a placebo. The powders were added to a single serving (140 g) of vanilla-flavoured Greek yogurt, which was then mixed thoroughly to ensure textural consistency. A single drop of fish oil was added to the placebo to replicate a similar taste and smell as the experimental treatment. Trial staff immediately involved with data collection and analysis did not prepare treatments and were not present during treatment preparation to maintain blinding.

Inducing cognitive demand

Two cognitive batteries were administered consecutively during pre-dose (baseline) and post-dose (acute) testing in order to assess cognitive function. Mood was assessed immediately before and after the completion of these cognitive batteries using visual analogue scales and computerised Likert scales (see Fig. 2).

Figure 2. Overview of schedule on experimental testing days.

DASS-21, 21-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; SFC-VAMS, Stress, Fatigue and Concentration Visual Analogue Mood Scales; STAI-S, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – State Index; SUCCAB, Swinburne University Computerised Cognitive Assessment Battery; CDB, cognitive demand battery.

The first battery, the Swinburne University Computerised Cognitive Assessment Battery (SUCCAB)(Reference Pipingas, Harris and Tournier54), assessed the primary outcome (i.e. cognitive response time), the results of which have been reported previously(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24). The second battery, the cognitive demand battery (CDB)(Reference Scholey, French and Morris35), immediately following SUCCAB, is comprised of three tasks (serial 3 subtraction, serial 7 subtraction and rapid visual information processing tasks), which participants completed three times sequentially over a period of approximately 30 min. The CDB was included in the trial to induce workload stress via an increased cognitive load, facilitating a shift towards negative mood (i.e. reduced alertness, elevated stress and increased fatigue)(Reference Massee, Ried and Pase34,Reference Scholey, French and Morris35,Reference Reed, Mitchell and Saunders38) . While the SUCCAB was not specifically designed to negatively affect participant mood (e.g. alertness, stress or fatigue), the battery has been shown in earlier acute mood studies to mentally tax participants(Reference Massee, Ried and Pase34).

Subjective state mood assessments

Participants were asked to report their current (i.e. ‘right now’) mood states before and after the two cognitive batteries using visual analogue scales, specifically, the Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scales and Stress, Fatigue and Concentration Visual Analogue Mood Scales. The Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scales and Stress, Fatigue and Concentration Visual Analogue Mood Scales, or similar, have been used previously to assess changes in mood following the completion of cognitive tasks and are sensitive to different treatment effects of nutritional interventions(Reference Watson, Haskell-Ramsay and Kennedy32,Reference Massee, Ried and Pase34,Reference Camfield, Silber and Scholey39,Reference Jackson, Kenney and Forster50) . The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – State Index (STAI-S) was also completed.

The Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scales(Reference Bond and Lader55) are comprised of sixteen 100 mm lines anchored at either end by antonyms (e.g. ‘sad’ and ‘happy’). Participants were required to mark the line between these antonyms to indicate their current mood (i.e. are they closer to sad or happy?). The score for each item was determined via the distance in millimetres between the participant’s mark and the negative antonym. Higher scores for each item reflect a greater positive mood. Scores for different items can be combined to describe three factors: alertness, calmness and contentedness. Higher scores for each factor reflect greater alertness, calmness or contentedness.

The Stress, Fatigue and Concentration Visual Analogue Mood Scales consist of four 100 mm lines anchored at either end with the labels ‘not at all’ and ‘very much so’. Participants were instructed to mark each line to reflect the extent to which they felt stressed, mentally or physically fatigued or able to concentrate. Scores for each item were determined by the distance in millimetres between the participant’s mark and the ‘not at all’ label. As such, higher scores reflect greater feelings of stress or fatigue. For the concentration scale, higher scores reflect a greater ability to concentrate.

The STAI(Reference Spielberger, Gorsuch and Lushene56) was designed to measure general, stable levels of anxiety (trait), as well as fluctuating levels of anxiety (state). The STAI-S consists of twenty items. Participants responded to each item by indicating along a four-point Likert scale the extent to which that item applied to them at that precise moment. Possible responses ranged from ‘not at all’ to ‘very much so’, with the range of possible scores for the STAI-S being 20–80. Higher STAI-S scores reflect greater state anxiety.

Demographics and participant characterisation

Demographic and anthropomorphic data were collected in person during the initial screening and familiarisation visit (V0, see Fig. 1). Participants were also asked to complete the MMSE(Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh53) to characterise global cognitive function and the 21-item Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale(Reference Lovibond and Lovibond57) to characterise mental health at baseline.

Procedure

Participants were initially screened for eligibility via telephone. Individuals deemed eligible were invited to attend the Centre of Human Psychopharmacology at Swinburne University of Technology to provide written informed consent and finalise eligibility screening. Participant eligibility was confirmed with the completion of additional screening measures (e.g. MMSE) as well as a medical history taken by a trained research nurse. All study procedures were then explained to eligible participants before completing a practice session with the cognitive tasks.

All experimental testing occurred on two separate days. The first was scheduled to occur within 14 d of the in-person screening/familiarisation visit, while the second was scheduled to occur 7 d (± 3 d) after the first testing visit. This was to ensure an appropriate washout for the initial treatment. However, due to changes in participant availability, some participants (a total of seven) had their second testing visit scheduled outside this time (ranging from 11 to 26 d). Importantly, no participants completed the second testing day earlier than specified ensuring adequate washout. All testing was performed in a dedicated human laboratory, which was free from outside distractions. All cognitive and mood assessments were presented to participants using the same size LCD computer monitor (this avoids variance in stimulus size, which for mood assessment would likely impact the magnitude of responses made by participants). All participants arrived at our lab in the morning having been fasting since 22.00 hours the night before (with participants requested to avoid fish/seafood or other foods high in ω-3 fatty acids for dinner the night before, as well as vigorous physical activity and alcohol for at least 12 h prior to the start of testing. Compliance with these criteria was confirmed at the start of each study visit). Following an initial blood sample and cardiovascular assessment(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24), participants were provided with a standardised breakfast – a choice of wholemeal toast with jam/vegemite/peanut butter or cereal. Participants were provided the same breakfast the following visit. A standardised meal was also provided 75 min after breakfast (i.e. salad roll with or without ham or chicken) with the study treatment (i.e. DHA-rich powder or placebo mixed in a single 140 g serving of Greek yogurt). The same foods were provided to participants on the second experimental testing day. Participants did not consume any other food or drinks except for plain water on the experimental testing days. Additional details about the testing procedure have been previously reported(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24). See Fig. 2 for an overview of experimental testing.

Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using International Business Machines SPSS Statistics for Windows version 26 (IBM Corp.). Baseline data are presented as the mean, standard deviation and range of scores for continuous variables and percentages for categorical variables (see Table 1). Univariate outliers were determined using the lower/upper bound (i.e. 25th and 75th percentile) method(Reference Hoaglin and Iglewicz58). The assumption of normality was assessed by examining the skewness/skewness error Z-score for each measure, with a Z-score less than 1·96 indicating a normal distribution(Reference Kim59). If distributional normality was not apparent, commonly employed data transformations (e.g. square root, Log10) were applied, sometimes after the addition of a small constant. Treatment effects upon subjective state mood were examined using a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA. All analyses were performed using data in its original form and repeated following the exclusion of outliers and transformations required for meeting the distributional assumptions of repeated-measures ANOVA. As the current examination is a secondary analysis of measures not designated as the primary outcome of the pilot study from which they were derived(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24), no formal power analysis was performed. Statistical significance was set at P < 0·05 (two-tailed).

Table 1. Participant demographics (Mean values and standard deviations; numbers and percentages)

MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; DASS-21, 21-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scale. n 29.

Results

Demographics

An overview of the demographic information of the participants (n 29) is provided in Table 1. In brief, participants were aged between 42 and 60 years (M = 52·8, sd = 5·3). Participant education attainment ranged from 11 to 21 years, with most (n 27) having completed tertiary or postgraduate education. Most were working full time (n 18) or part-time/casual (n 3). The mean participant MMSE score was 29·3 (sd = 0·9). None of the participants had a current history of disordered mood or was taking medications for treating such conditions. Further, the three-factor scores (i.e. depression, anxiety and stress) from the 21-item Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (assessed during pre-dose testing at visit 1) were all in the normal range (depression 0–9; anxiety 0–7; stress 0–14). Finally, erythrocyte ω-3 PUFA status of participants in this study is comparable to data from earlier studies with Australian samples (relative % of EPA = 0·43–1·03, DHA = 2·30–5·40, EPA + DHA = 2·91–6·28)(Reference Stark, Van Elswyk and Higgins60).

Change in mood with elevated cognitive demand during pre-dose testing

The purpose of analysing mood data prior to participants having received their scheduled treatment (‘pre-dose’ testing in Fig. 2) was to establish that the completion of the cognitive testing batteries does negatively influence mood. As expected, the completion of the SUCCAB and CDB altered participants’ subjective mood. Specifically, there were significant main effects of time for each mood measure (see Table 2). Alertness, calmness, contentedness and the ability to concentrate were all significantly lower after cognitive testing, while stress and mental and physical fatigue were significantly elevated. Similarly, there was a significant main effect of time for state anxiety (STAI-S), indicating that anxiety increased as a result of completing the cognitive batteries (see Table 2). As expected, prior to participants being administered the DHA-rich powder or placebo, there were no differences in mood changes in response to increased cognitive demand between the two groups (for all interaction effects P > 0·05). The results were unchanged following the removal of outliers and/or transforming data to achieve normality. As such, the results presented in Table 2 are those using data in its original form.

Table 2. Change in mood with elevated cognitive demand during pre-dose testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

All results are from analyses using data in its original form and n 29, except state anxiety, n 27 due to missing data. Analyses repeated following outlier removal and/or data transformation to achieve normality did not alter the direction of effects or significance of findings reported in this table.

Differential change in mood with cognitive demand during post-dose (4 h) testing

Outliers were identified for alertness, contentedness, stress and physical fatigue. Sample sizes following outlier removal are reported in Table 3 alongside summary statistics for each measure. In addition, transformations were required to normalise all measures except for alertness and contentedness (see Table 4).

Table 3. Summary statistics for mood during post-dose (4 h) testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

All data are in original form. Note that for all measures, the original n was 29, except for physical fatigue where initially n was 28 (one participant had missing data).

Table 4. Differential change in mood with cognitive demand during post-dose (4 h) testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

aTransformations required for calmness (X2, negative skew), stress (Log10, positive skew), mental fatigue (√, positive skew), physical fatigue (√, positive skew), concentration (−√(max-x), negative skew) and state anxiety (−1/√, positive skew). A constant of 10 was added to stress, mental fatigue, physical fatigue and concentration ability prior to transformation to aid in achieving a normal distribution. bNote that due to the nature of the transformation used, greater values are represented as being closer to 0. Sample size for each analysis indicated in Table 3.

Consistent with changes in mood observed during pre-dose testing, there was a significant main effect of time for each of the mood measures following increased cognitive demand regardless of the treatment participants had consumed on the day: alertness (F(1,26) = 27·08, P < 0·001), contentedness (F(1,25) = 29·07, P < 0·001), calmness (transformed; F(1,28) = 47·19, P < 0·001), stress (transformed; F(1,27) = 53·42, P < 0·001), mental fatigue (transformed; F(1,28) = 43·57, P < 0·001), physical fatigue (transformed; F(1,24) = 24·55, P < 0·001), ability to concentrate (transformed; F(1,28) = 31·54, P < 0·001) and state anxiety (transformed; F(1,28) = 21·94, P < 0·001). As indicated in Table 4, each of these measures significantly changed as a result of the increased cognitive demand facilitated by the testing batteries regardless of the treatment received (note that due to the nature of the transformations applied to the ability to concentrate and state anxiety data, values that were originally larger are represented as being closer to 0).

Importantly, a significant differential treatment effect (i.e. interaction) was only apparent for alertness and stress (transformed), while it approached significance for physical fatigue (transformed) (see Table 4). No significant differences in alertness, stress (transformed) or physical fatigue (transformed) were identified across treatment conditions (lowest P = 0·71) prior to completing the cognitive batteries and inducing elevated cognitive demand. However, following the completion of the cognitive batteries, alertness was significantly greater for the DHA-rich powder compared with placebo (P = 0·006; see Fig. 3(a)). Conversely, while stress (transformed) in response to elevated cognitive demand was lower for the DHA-rich powder, the difference to placebo only approached significance (P = 0·05; see Fig. 3(b)). Physical fatigue (transformed) was also significantly lower after completing the cognitive batteries for the DHA-rich powder compared with placebo (P = 0·009; see Fig. 3(c)).

Figure 3. Differential treatment effects on (a) alertness, (b) stress and (c) physical fatigue during post-dose (4 h) testing.

Discussion

The present study examined whether a single dose of a DHA-rich powder, incorporated into a meal, provided acute benefits to mood in middle-aged males following elevated cognitive demand. As expected, the completion of the cognitive batteries significantly altered mood, resulting in reduced self-rated alertness, calmness, contentedness and ability to concentrate, coupled with increased stress and mental and physical fatigue, as well as state anxiety. Significant interaction effects were observed for alertness and stress during post-dose testing, demonstrating that the consumption of the DHA-rich powder altered mood by supporting alertness and buffering the increase in stress in response to elevated cognitive demand. While there was a trend towards a differential treatment effect for physical fatigue, no significant interaction effects were observed for contentedness, calmness, mental fatigue, the ability to concentrate or state anxiety. Thus, data from the present study suggest that the consumption of a high dose of ω-3 PUFA, specifically a high dose of DHA, may represent a viable ‘pre-intervention’ or prophylaxis for mitigating negative mood (i.e. elevated stress, reduced alertness) following enhanced cognitive demand. This may have tangible implications for promoting healthier mental well-being in response to acute periods of elevated cognitive demand such as that experienced in the workplace.

Consistent with results reported here of the acute effects of ω-3 PUFA on alertness, earlier studies have revealed that the consumption of certain food extracts, nutrients and nootropic formulations can benefit subjective alertness following a single dose. For example, in a sample of thirty-two young adults (n 6 males), Jackson et al. (2020) reported that a single serving of a multi-extract (i.e. beetroot, ginseng and sage) beverage with added apple polyphenols resulted in greater subjective ratings of alertness and reduced fatigue compared with placebo after successive rounds of the CDB (completed 60-, 180- and 360-min post-dose)(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey26). Other studies investigating acute effects on alertness in the hours following consumption of flavonoids from oranges(Reference Alharbi, Lamport and Dodd36) or blackcurrants(Reference Watson, Haskell-Ramsay and Kennedy32) have similarly demonstrated a mitigating effect of flavonoids on reduced alertness following elevated cognitive demand. Similar effects have been reported following ingestion of caffeinated coffee in middle-aged and older adults(Reference Cropley, Croft and Silber40), though such effects were also observed following ingestion of decaffeinated coffee delivering a high dose (521 mg) of chlorogenic acid relative to decaffeinated coffee with a lower dose (224 mg). Similar benefits are also reported for subjective stress. One recent crossover study involving healthy young (18–25 years) males demonstrated that a single dose of saffron extract significantly mitigated the rise in subjective stress, as well as anxiety, elicited in response to the Maastricht acute stress test compared with placebo(Reference Pouchieu, Pourtau and Brossaud31). Exploring the acute effects of a single 200 mg dose of l-theanine in healthy young (18–40 years) adults, White et al. (2016) observed that participants treated with l-theanine exhibited a significantly smaller stress response (i.e. change in mood from start to end of cognitive testing) than placebo, when testing was repeated 1 h post-dose, though no differences were evident 3 h post-dose(Reference White, de Klerk and Woods44). Acute reductions in stress have also been reported 2 h after a single dose of epigallocatechin gallate (a flavonoid from green tea) in healthy young adults(Reference Scholey, Downey and Ciorciari45). Thus, the present study builds upon an expanding literature demonstrating that a single dose of a nutritional product (i.e. extract or nootropic formulation) is capable of delivering acute mood affects (e.g. alertness, stress) in the hours following consumption, particularly during times of elevated cognitive demand.

While enhanced intake (resulting in a higher status) of ω-3 PUFA over periods of weeks or months has been shown to benefit mood(Reference Liao, Xie and Zhang21,Reference Su, Tseng and Lin22,Reference Giles, Mahoney and Urry61,Reference Ginty and Conklin62) , to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the acute effects of ω-3 PUFA on mood. Due to the limited data on the acute effects of ω-3 PUFA, the results should be interpreted with caution until further evidence is produced. Although this pilot study did not explicitly seek to delineate the mechanisms underlying such effects, it is worth considering possible aetiological mechanisms that could be explored in follow-on randomised clinical trials.

One potential mechanism is an effect on endothelial and overall cardiovascular function. Benefits to central blood pressures following a single dose of the DHA-rich powder in this sample have already been reported(Reference Pipingas, Reddan and Gauci24). There is also evidence from other studies indicating that a meal enriched in ω-3 PUFA (combined DHA + EPA typically 0·90 g to 5·40 g; DHA doses ranged from 0·36 g to 3·24 g; EPA doses ranged from 0·54 g to 2·16 g) may benefit endothelial function in the hours after consumption(Reference McManus, Tejera and Awwad25,Reference Fahs, Yan and Ranadive63Reference Armah, Jackson and Doman65) and significantly reduced arterial stiffness has been reported 4 h post-dose (significant following 4·16 g DHA but borderline significant following 4·16 g EPA) in a sample of males aged between 35 and 55 years(Reference McManus, Tejera and Awwad25). Indeed, in their acute polyphenol study, Jackson et al. (2020) reported elevated total Hb using near-infrared spectroscopy, indicative of enhanced cerebral blood flow, following consumption of apple polyphenols, a treatment which also appeared to benefit alertness(Reference Jackson, Wightman and Veasey26). Jackson et al. (2020) suggested that altered cerebral blood flow may have contributed to their findings on subjective mood. Also of relevance is a flavonoid study by Gratton et al. (2020), wherein it was reported that a single high dose of cocoa flavanols was capable of benefiting cognitive function, though such effects were only apparent in those who similarly demonstrated enhanced cerebrovascular reactivity and blood flow following the treatment(Reference Gratton, Weaver and Burley27). Importantly, an earlier study by Jackson et al. indicated that 12 weeks of intake of DHA-rich (1 g DHA) oil, but not EPA-rich (1 g EPA) oil, facilitated enhanced cerebral blood flow (assessed using near-infrared spectroscopy) in young adults while completing cognitive tasks(Reference Jackson, Reay and Scholey66). Conversely, another study by Jackson and colleagues failed to identify any significant effect of 6 months of daily intake of 2 g DHA-rich (∼ 0·90 g DHA and 0·13 g EPA) fish oil alone or in combination with a multi-nutrient (containing phosphatidylserine, Ginkgo biloba, folic acid and vitamin B12) upon cerebral blood flow in older adults(Reference Patan, Kennedy and Husberg67). As the above data appear to demonstrate that endothelial and cardiovascular function is modifiable following high dose of ω-3 PUFA intake and that such changes are detectable in the hours following a single dose, it is plausible that a beneficial effect upon vascular function may have contributed to the effect upon subjective mood reported here. This potential mechanism should be explored further in future research.

Another potential mechanism by which ω-3 PUFA consumption could benefit mood, particularly stress, is via the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, as indicated via changes in cortisol levels. Noreen et al. (2010) reported a ‘tendency for reduced salivary cortisol’ in young and middle-aged (i.e. 18–55 years) adults after 6 weeks of daily supplementation with ω-3 PUFA (1·6 g EPA and 0·8 g DHA) compared with placebo(Reference Noreen, Sass and Crowe68). Others have also explored the effects of ω-3 PUFA supplementation on cortisol concentrations in response to enhanced cognitive stress/demand. Delarue et al. (2003) demonstrated that an acute mental stressor task enhanced plasma cortisol in healthy young males but that daily ω-3 PUFA supplementation (1·1 g EPA and 0·7 g DHA) mitigated the subsequent rise in plasma cortisol when assessed 3 weeks later(Reference Delarue, Matzinger and Binnert69). Likewise, Barbadoro et al. (2013) observed that 3 weeks of daily ω-3 PUFA supplementation (60 mg EPA and 252 mg DHA) in males with alcohol use disorder (participating in a residential rehabilitation programme) significantly reduced daytime salivary cortisol levels (assessed approximately every 4 h) compared with baseline – an effect not observed in those taking the placebo(Reference Barbadoro, Annino and Ponzio70). The ω-3 PUFA group also reported a reduction in perceived stress after the 21 d supplementation. Importantly, the authors also assessed changes in salivary cortisol in response to completing a cognitive stressor (i.e. the Trier Social Stress Test) but failed to observe any group differences in the ‘AUC’ or peak cortisol, thereby suggesting no differential treatment effects upon hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responsiveness. Giles et al. (2015) also reported no significant differences in salivary cortisol change after completing a non-stressful social task and the Trier Social Stress Test in a sample of young adults who either supplemented with ω-3 PUFA or placebo for 3 weeks(Reference Giles, Mahoney and Urry61). However, all of the above studies involved longer-term ω-3 PUFA supplementation, not a once-off dose, as in the present work. Therefore, while it has been shown that the cortisol response in response to stressful stimuli is modifiable following a single dose of a nutritional product(Reference Pouchieu, Pourtau and Brossaud31,Reference White, de Klerk and Woods44) , future research is required to determine whether similar changes to the cortisol response are apparent after a single dose of ω-3 PUFA, in particular a high dose of DHA as in the present study. As cortisol was not assessed in the present study, future replication is required to delineate whether a single high dose of ω-3 PUFA is capable of influencing hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal activity as indicated via changes in cortisol and whether such changes underpin alterations in acute subjective stress.

A particular strength of this study was the use of the SUCCAB and CDB cognitive challenge to induce mood changes, thereby greatly increasing the sensitivity of the assessment. Furthermore, the use of a dedicated human laboratory free from outside distractions provided a controlled environment that may otherwise influence mood (via disengagement with the cognitive tasks). Moreover, mood was assessed using the visual analogue scales immediately before and after the cognitive tasks, which may make them more likely to capture more immediate mood effects than if they were just completed at regular intervals over the testing day.

Nonetheless, the project has several limitations that should be considered when interpreting results. First, the present study sample was relatively small, and it is likely that this reduced the statistical power for detecting significant effects in other measures, such as physical fatigue. A larger sample would no doubt improve statistical power and potentially lead to broader more robust subjective mood effects. Another factor that may have contributed to the limited number of mood measures demonstrating effects in the present study is the restriction of the study sample to males only. Other studies(Reference Massee, Ried and Pase34,Reference Scholey, French and Morris35,Reference Reed, Mitchell and Saunders38) that have reported differential effects following a single dose of cocoa flavanols upon mental fatigue included male and female participants. It may be that there are sex or gender differences in the extent to which elevated cognitive demand negatively influences mood, and, therefore, the likelihood that acute effects associated with a dietary intervention are detectable. As such, it would be necessary to repeat the present study with a larger representative sample involving males and females to not only assess this possibility but also facilitate greater generalisability of any detected effects.

Future research should investigate whether high dose of ω-3 PUFA can provide acute benefits to mood in clinical populations (e.g. clinical depression or anxiety), who may be more sensitive to the negative influences of cognitive demand and to the benefits of mitigating interventions. While the dosage used in the present study is similar to the ω-3 PUFA profile in a serving of salmon (2·5/100 g)(Reference Cladis, Kleiner and Freiser71), future research should examine whether similar effects are attainable using different dosages as well as fatty acid ratios resembling those attainable from dietary sources. For example, additional research in both clinical and non-clinical samples could assess whether similar mood effects are observed in response to an equivalent high-dose EPA-rich supplement and whether there are differential effects to high-dose DHA. Indeed, a recent meta-analysis(Reference Bazinet, Metherel and Chen18) reported that treatment with EPA (alone or as the predominant ω-3 PUFA) facilitated greater mood effects than when DHA (alone or as predominant ω-3 PUFA) was administered over longer periods. Considering that the present work is the first to assess the acute effects of any dose of ω-3 PUFA upon subjective mood in response to elevated cognitive demand, it is unknown whether a higher dose of EPA relative to DHA likewise provides benefits to mood. As such, it is possible that the limited effects reported here may become more expansive, or possibly of greater magnitude, if a higher dose of EPA relative to DHA is administered. Therefore, it may be worth replicating the present study with a single high dose of EPA (alone or as the predominant ω-3 PUFA) in order to determine whether similar if not greater effects upon subjective mood are attainable. Future replication directly contrasting higher relative doses of EPA to DHA and vice versa with placebo would also be beneficial for elucidating potentially different mechanisms facilitating acute effects upon subjective mood. Further exploration is needed to determine whether similar effects upon subjective mood are attainable at lower doses, as a lower dose may be more feasible for long-term use.

Conclusion

The present study revealed that a single high dose of a DHA-rich powder could mitigate the effect of elevated cognitive demand on alertness and stress in healthy middle-aged Australian men. However, further work is required to delineate the potential mechanisms via which a single high dose of ω-3 PUFA may support healthier mood in response to cognitively demanding stimuli and to determine whether acute benefits are similarly achievable in middle-aged females and those with clinical mood disorders.

Acknowledgements

Special thanks to Naomi Perry and Nikola Stupar for their assistance in trial management, as well as to Samaneh Fard (Nu-Mega Ingredients) for assisting in developing an appropriate food vehicle for administering the investigational product. We also thank our participants for their time and dedication to the study.

The study was sponsored by Nu-Mega Ingredients Limited, but they did not have any role in the data analysis or interpretation of results.

J. M. R. and S. G. were involved in conducting the study, analysing data and interpreting study findings. J. M. R. and S. G. are co-first authors, having equally contributed to writing the initial manuscript draft and preparing the manuscript for publication. L. M. Y. was involved in conducting the study and analysing data. G. K. was involved in data collection, interpreting findings and preparing the manuscript for publication. R. R., A. M. M., A. S. and A. P. were involved in formulating the research questions, designing the study and preparing the manuscript for publication.

A. S. and A. P. are named inventors on a patent ‘Uses of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids’ (EP4009965A1, 15 June 2022). Nu-Mega Ingredients provided partial funding for the study and supplied investigational products. J. M. R., S. G., L. M. Y., G. K., R. R. and A. M. M. do not report any conflicts.

Footnotes

Joint first authors/These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

GBD 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators (2022) Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry 9, 137150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, GY, Tam, WW, Lu, Y, et al. (2018) Prevalence of depression in the community from 30 countries between 1994 and 2014. Sci Rep 8, 2861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, R, Peng, X, Song, X, et al. (2023) The prevalence and risk of developing major depression among individuals with subthreshold depression in the general population. Psychol Med 53, 36113620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haller, H, Cramer, H, Lauche, R, et al. (2014) The prevalence and burden of subthreshold generalized anxiety disorder: a systematic review. BMC Psychiatry 14, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reid, R, Steel, A, Wardle, J, et al. (2016) Complementary medicine use by the Australian population: a critical mixed studies systematic review of utilisation, perceptions and factors associated with use. BMC Complement Altern Med 16, 176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavretsky, H (2009) Complementary and alternative medicine use for treatment and prevention of late-life mood and cognitive disorders. Aging Health 5, 6178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ravindran, AV & da Silva, TL (2013) Complementary and alternative therapies as add-on to pharmacotherapy for mood and anxiety disorders: a systematic review. J Affect Disord 150, 707719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marx, W, Lane, M, Hockey, M, et al. (2021) Diet and depression: exploring the biological mechanisms of action. Mol Psychiatry 26, 134150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firth, J, Marx, W, Dash, S, et al. (2019) The effects of dietary improvement on symptoms of depression and anxiety: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med 81, 265280.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, MM, Gamage, E, Travica, N, et al. (2022) Ultra-processed food consumption and mental health: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Nutrients 14, 2568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akpınar, Ş & Karadağ, MG (2022) Is vitamin D important in anxiety or depression? What is the truth? Curr Nutr Rep 11, 675681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarris, J, Murphy, J, Mischoulon, D, et al. (2016) Adjunctive nutraceuticals for depression: a systematic review and meta-analyses. Am J Psychiatry 173, 575587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, LM, Pipingas, A, White, DJ, et al. (2019) A systematic review and meta-analysis of B vitamin supplementation on depressive symptoms, anxiety, and stress: effects on healthy and ‘at-risk’ individuals. Nutrients 11, 2232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosso, G, Galvano, F, Marventano, S, et al. (2014) n-3 fatty acids and depression: scientific evidence and biological mechanisms. Oxid Med Cell Longev 2014, 313570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denis, I, Potier, B, Vancassel, S, et al. (2013) n-3 fatty acids and brain resistance to ageing and stress: body of evidence and possible mechanisms. Ageing Res Rev 12, 579594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shahidi, F & Ambigaipalan, P (2018) n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and their health benefits. Annu Rev Food Sci Technol 9, 345381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pontifex, M, Vauzour, D & Minihane, A-M (2018) The effect of APOE genotype on Alzheimer’s disease risk is influenced by sex and docosahexaenoic acid status. Neurobiol Aging 69, 209220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bazinet, RP, Metherel, AH, Chen, CT, et al. (2020) Brain eicosapentaenoic acid metabolism as a lead for novel therapeutics in major depression. Brain Behav Immun 85, 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larrieu, T & Layé, S (2018) Food for mood: relevance of nutritional n-3 fatty acids for depression and anxiety. Front Physiol 9, 1047.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelaiditis, CF, Gibson, EL & Dyall, SC (2023) Effects of long-chain n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on reducing anxiety and/or depression in adults; a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 192, 102572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liao, Y, Xie, B, Zhang, H, et al. (2019) Efficacy of n-3 PUFAs in depression: a meta-analysis. Transl Psychiatry 9, 190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Su, KP, Tseng, PT, Lin, PY, et al. (2018) Association of use of n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids with changes in severity of anxiety symptoms: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Netw Open 1, e182327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boon, EAJ, Croft, KD, Shinde, S, et al. (2017) The acute effect of coffee on endothelial function and glucose metabolism following a glucose load in healthy human volunteers. Food Funct 8, 33663373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pipingas, A, Reddan, JM, Gauci, S, et al. (2023) Post-prandial cognitive and blood pressure effects of a DHA-rich n-3 powder in middle-aged males: a pilot study. Nutrients 15, 2198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McManus, S, Tejera, N, Awwad, K, et al. (2016) Differential effects of EPA v. DHA on postprandial vascular function and the plasma oxylipin profile in men. J Lipid Res 57, 17201727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, PA, Wightman, EL, Veasey, R, et al. (2020) A randomized, crossover study of the acute cognitive and cerebral blood flow effects of phenolic, nitrate and botanical beverages in young, healthy humans. Nutrients 12, 2254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G, Weaver, SR, Burley, CV, et al. (2020) Dietary flavanols improve cerebral cortical oxygenation and cognition in healthy adults. Sci Rep 10, 19409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, DW, Tallent, R, Navalta, JW, et al. (2020) Effects of acute cocoa supplementation on postprandial apolipoproteins, lipoprotein subclasses, and inflammatory biomarkers in adults with type 2 diabetes after a high-fat meal. Nutrients 12, 1902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schell, J, Betts, NM, Lyons, TJ, et al. (2019) Raspberries improve postprandial glucose and acute and chronic inflammation in adults with type 2 diabetes. Ann Nutr Metab 74, 165174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, PA, Haskell-Ramsay, C, Forster, J, et al. (2022) Acute cognitive performance and mood effects of coffee berry and apple extracts: a randomised, double blind, placebo controlled crossover study in healthy humans. Nutr Neurosci 25, 23352343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pouchieu, C, Pourtau, L, Brossaud, J, et al. (2023) Acute effect of a saffron extract (Safr’InsideTM) and its main volatile compound on the stress response in healthy young men: a randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Nutrients 15, 2921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, AW, Haskell-Ramsay, CF, Kennedy, DO, et al. (2015) Acute supplementation with blackcurrant extracts modulates cognitive functioning and inhibits monoamine oxidase-B in healthy young adults. J Funct Foods 17, 524539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boolani, A, Lindheimer, JB, Loy, BD, et al. (2017) Acute effects of brewed cocoa consumption on attention, motivation to perform cognitive work and feelings of anxiety, energy and fatigue: a randomized, placebo-controlled crossover experiment. BMC Nutr 3, 8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Massee, LA, Ried, K, Pase, M, et al. (2015) The acute and sub-chronic effects of cocoa flavanols on mood, cognitive and cardiovascular health in young healthy adults: a randomized, controlled trial. Front Pharmacol 6, 93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholey, AB, French, SJ, Morris, PJ, et al. (2010) Consumption of cocoa flavanols results in acute improvements in mood and cognitive performance during sustained mental effort. J Psychopharmacol 24, 15051514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alharbi, MH, Lamport, DJ, Dodd, GF, et al. (2016) Flavonoid-rich orange juice is associated with acute improvements in cognitive function in healthy middle-aged males. Eur J Nutr 55, 20212029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalid, S, Barfoot, KL, May, G, et al. (2017) Effects of acute blueberry flavonoids on mood in children and young adults. Nutrients 9, 158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, RA, Mitchell, ES, Saunders, C, et al. (2019) Acute low and moderate doses of a caffeine-free polyphenol-rich coffeeberry extract improve feelings of alertness and fatigue resulting from the performance of fatiguing cognitive tasks. J Cogn Enhanc 3, 193206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camfield, DA, Silber, BY, Scholey, AB, et al. (2013) A randomised placebo-controlled trial to differentiate the acute cognitive and mood effects of chlorogenic acid from decaffeinated coffee. PLoS One 8, e82897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cropley, V, Croft, R, Silber, B, et al. (2012) Does coffee enriched with chlorogenic acids improve mood and cognition after acute administration in healthy elderly? A pilot study. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 219, 737749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Firk, C & Markus, CR (2009) Mood and cortisol responses following tryptophan-rich hydrolyzed protein and acute stress in healthy subjects with high and low cognitive reactivity to depression. Clin Nutr 28, 266271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medrano, M, Molina-Hidalgo, C, Alcantara, JMA, et al. (2022) Acute effect of a dietary multi-ingredient nootropic as a cognitive enhancer in young healthy adults: a randomized, triple-blinded, placebo-controlled, crossover trial. Front Nutr 9, 858910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholey, A, Bauer, I, Neale, C, et al. (2013) Acute effects of different multivitamin mineral preparations with and without guaraná on mood, cognitive performance and functional brain activation. Nutrients 5, 35893604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, DJ, de Klerk, S, Woods, W, et al. (2016) Anti-stress, behavioural and magnetoencephalography effects of an l-theanine-based nutrient drink: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover trial. Nutrients 8, 53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholey, A, Downey, LA, Ciorciari, J, et al. (2012) Acute neurocognitive effects of epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG). Appetite 58, 767770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philip, P, Sagaspe, P, Taillard, J, et al. (2019) Acute intake of a grape and blueberry polyphenol-rich extract ameliorates cognitive performance in healthy young adults during a sustained cognitive effort. Antioxidants 8, 650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macpherson, H, Rowsell, R, Cox, KHM, et al. (2015) Acute mood but not cognitive improvements following administration of a single multivitamin and mineral supplement in healthy women aged 50 and above: a randomised controlled trial. Age 37, 9782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, DO, Bonnländer, B, Lang, SC, et al. (2020) Acute and chronic effects of green oat (Avena sativa) extract on cognitive function and mood during a laboratory stressor in healthy adults: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in healthy humans. Nutrients 12, 1598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wightman, EL, Jackson, PA, Forster, J, et al. (2020) Acute effects of a polyphenol-rich leaf extract of Mangifera indica L. (zynamite) on cognitive function in healthy adults: a double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover study. Nutrients 12, 2194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, PA, Kenney, C, Forster, J, et al. (2023) Acute cognitive performance and mood effects of coffeeberry extract: a randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled crossover study in healthy humans. Nutrients 15, 2418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kheloui, S, Brouillard, A, Rossi, M, et al. (2021) Exploring the sex and gender correlates of cognitive sex differences. Acta Psychol (Amst) 221, 103452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejbak, L, Crossley, M & Vrbancic, M (2011) A male advantage for spatial and object but not verbal working memory using the n-back task. Brain Cogn 76, 191196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folstein, MF, Folstein, SE & McHugh, PR (1975) ‘Mini-Mental State’: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pipingas, A, Harris, E, Tournier, E, et al. (2010) Assessing the efficacy of nutraceutical interventions on cognitive functioning in the elderly. Curr Top Nutraceutical Res 8, 7987.Google Scholar
Bond, A & Lader, M (1974) The use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. BJMP 47, 211218.Google Scholar
Spielberger, C, Gorsuch, R, Lushene, R, et al. (1983) Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Lovibond, S & Lovibond, PF (1995) Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, 2nd ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoaglin, DC & Iglewicz, B (1987) Fine-tuning some resistant rules for outlier labeling. J Am Stat Assoc 82, 11471149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H-Y (2013) Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dent Endod 38, 52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, KD, Van Elswyk, ME, Higgins, MR, et al. (2016) Global survey of the n-3 fatty acids, docosahexaenoic acid and eicosapentaenoic acid in the blood stream of healthy adults. Prog Lipid Res 63, 132152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giles, GE, Mahoney, CR, Urry, HL, et al. (2015) n-3 fatty acids and stress-induced changes to mood and cognition in healthy individuals. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 132, 1019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginty, AT & Conklin, SM (2015) Short-term supplementation of acute long-chain n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids may alter depression status and decrease symptomology among young adults with depression: a preliminary randomized and placebo controlled trial. Psychiatry Res 229, 485489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahs, CA, Yan, H, Ranadive, S, et al. (2010) The effect of acute fish-oil supplementation on endothelial function and arterial stiffness following a high-fat meal. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 35, 294302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, KG, Armah, CK, Doman, I, et al. (2009) The impact of age on the postprandial vascular response to a fish oil-enriched meal. Br J Nutr 102, 14141419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Armah, CK, Jackson, KG, Doman, I, et al. (2008) Fish oil fatty acids improve postprandial vascular reactivity in healthy men. Clin Sci 114, 679686.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, PA, Reay, JL, Scholey, AB, et al. (2012) DHA-rich oil modulates the cerebral haemodynamic response to cognitive tasks in healthy young adults: a near IR spectroscopy pilot study. Br J Nutr 107, 10931098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patan, MJ, Kennedy, DO, Husberg, C, et al. (2021) Supplementation with oil rich in eicosapentaenoic acid, but not in docosahexaenoic acid, improves global cognitive function in healthy, young adults: results from randomized controlled trials. Am J Clin Nutr 114, 914924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noreen, EE, Sass, MJ, Crowe, ML, et al. (2010) Effects of supplemental fish oil on resting metabolic rate, body composition, and salivary cortisol in healthy adults. J Int Soc Sports Nutr 7, 31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delarue, J, Matzinger, O, Binnert, C, et al. (2003) Fish oil prevents the adrenal activation elicited by mental stress in healthy men. Diabetes Metab 29, 289295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barbadoro, P, Annino, I, Ponzio, E, et al. (2013) Fish oil supplementation reduces cortisol basal levels and perceived stress: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial in abstinent alcoholics. Mol Nutr Food Res 57, 11101114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cladis, DP, Kleiner, AC, Freiser, HH, et al. (2014) Fatty acid profiles of commercially available finfish fillets in the United States. Lipids 49, 10051018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Overview of trial design, participant recruitment and retention.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Overview of schedule on experimental testing days.DASS-21, 21-item Depression Anxiety Stress Scale; SFC-VAMS, Stress, Fatigue and Concentration Visual Analogue Mood Scales; STAI-S, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory – State Index; SUCCAB, Swinburne University Computerised Cognitive Assessment Battery; CDB, cognitive demand battery.

Figure 2

Table 1. Participant demographics (Mean values and standard deviations; numbers and percentages)

Figure 3

Table 2. Change in mood with elevated cognitive demand during pre-dose testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 4

Table 3. Summary statistics for mood during post-dose (4 h) testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 5

Table 4. Differential change in mood with cognitive demand during post-dose (4 h) testing (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 6

Figure 3. Differential treatment effects on (a) alertness, (b) stress and (c) physical fatigue during post-dose (4 h) testing.