Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T17:27:27.043Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Correspondence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 July 2007

AUDREY NOËL
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
PEGGY QUINETTE
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
BÉRENGÈRE GUILLERY-GIRARD
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
JACQUES DAYAN
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
STÉPHANIE KATIS
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
PASCALE PIOLINO
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
PASCALE ABADIE
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
VINCENT DE LA SAYETTE
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
SOPHIE MARQUIS
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
FAUSTO VIADER
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
BÉATRICE DESGRANGES
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
FRANCIS EUSTACHE*
Affiliation:
Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, GIP Cyceron, CHU Côte de Nacre, Caen, France
*
Address correspondence to: Professor Francis Eustache, Inserm – EPHE – Université de Caen Basse-Normandie, Unité E0218, Laboratoire de Neuropsychologie, CHU Côte de Nacre, 14033 Caen Cedex, France (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Correspondence
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2007

Research Letter

How psychopathological factors affect both the onset of and recovery from transient global amnesia

In a previous study (Quinette et al. Reference Quinette, Guillery-Girard, Dayan, de la Sayette, Marquis, Viader, Desgranges and Eustache2006) we highlighted four subgroups of transient global amnesia (TGA) linked with different risk factors, one of which was the presence of personality disorders. We went deeper into this classification by collecting additional measures of psychopathological factors (anxiety and depression), in 38 patients (28 included in our preceding study and 10 new patients). Four subgroups of patients were also distinguished, one of whom was characterized by both emotional precipitants and previous or current anxious/depressive disorders (Fig. 1). This latent psychological fragility associated with an intense emotional experience could help to trigger a TGA (Inzitari et al. Reference Inzitari, Pantoni, Lamassa, Pallanti, Pracucci and Marini1997). The aim of the present research was to study the influence of these psychopathological factors on early recovery process (namely the day after the episode). Thus, we investigated the mood congruency effect in order to gain a better understanding of the influence of anxiety/depression on both the acquisition and retrieval of emotional episodic memories.

Fig. 1. A hierarchical cluster analysis was carried out with same clinical variables than those used in the study of Quinette et al. (Reference Quinette, Guillery-Girard, Dayan, de la Sayette, Marquis, Viader, Desgranges and Eustache2006). Two new measures were added: level of anxiety (with a cut-off point of 55 in the Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory; Spielberger, Reference Spielberger1983) and depression (with a cut-off point of 8, in the Beck Depression Inventory; Beck et al. Reference Beck, Rial and Rickets1974). The results shows four clusters: (1) transient global amnesia (TGA) preceded by physical precipitants, (2) TGA preceded by emotional episodes accompanied by psychopathological factors, (3) patients with vascular risk factors, (4) TGA with no particular characteristics. The cluster in boldface includes all the emotional characteristics.

Method

We reported data collected in 9 out of 10 new patients included in the cluster analysis (Fig. 1) and we divided this group of patients into two subgroups (‘emotional’ and ‘non-emotional’), according to their level of anxiety/depression as measured with the same scales and cut-off points as in the cluster analysis. The ‘emotional’ (mean score of anxiety/depression: 63/10·5) and ‘non-emotional’ groups (mean score of anxiety/depression: 50·6/2·6) included four and five patients, respectively. These groups were similar in age and duration of TGA episode. Control group included 13 healthy subjects (mean score of anxiety/depression: 46·9/2·5). All patients gave their informed consent to the study, which was performed in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Anterograde component of episodic memory

During the study session, the participants had to learn (intentional encoding) a list of 45 words (15 positive, 15 negative and 15 neutral items). Retrieval was assessed by means of a dual forced recognition task. An accuracy score corresponding to A′ (Gardiner et al. Reference Gardiner, Ramponi and Richardson-Klavehn2002) was obtained for each word valence.

Retrograde component of episodic memory

An autobiographical memory task, derived from Piolino et al.'s semi-structured questionnaire (Reference Piolino, Desgranges, Belliard, Matuszewski, Lalevée, de la Sayette and Eustache2003), allowed us to gauge the ability to recall specific events across three time periods and to control both the intensity and valence of memories.

Inter-group comparisons were made by means of the Mann–Whitney test. However, since only two patients in the ‘emotional group’ carried out the autobiographical memory task, their performances were compared with normal controls according to the z score method.

Results

Anterograde component of episodic memory

A group effect (p=0·04) was found for the accuracy score of negative words between controls (A′=0·88) and the ‘emotional group’ (A′=0·76). These patients had greater difficulty than controls distinguishing negative hits from lures. There was no statistical difference between controls and the ‘non-emotional’ group (A′=0·85). No significant difference was found for positive (A′=0·85 for ‘emotional group, A′=0·84 for ‘non-emotional’ group, A′=0·86 for control group) and neutral words (A′=0·87 for ‘non-emotional’ group, A′=0·86 for ‘emotional group’ and control group).

Retrograde component of episodic memory (Table 1)

The two patients of the ‘emotional group’ scored like controls for the two most recent periods, but their most remote memories were less specific and less intense than those of controls. Moreover, valence scores for this period bordered on the significance threshold for z scores, meaning that they tended to provide more negative memories than controls. No statistical difference was found between the scores of the ‘non-emotional’ group and the control group. In order to compare these groups more effectively, we also calculated z scores for the ‘non-emotional’ group. No z scores were pathological (data not shown).

Table 1. Performances of transient global amnesia (TGA) patients and normal controls on the autobiographical task

s.d., Standard deviation; P04/P08 corresponds to code of each patient.

Two specific memories (specificity score, Piolino et al. Reference Piolino, Desgranges, Belliard, Matuszewski, Lalevée, de la Sayette and Eustache2003) were requested for each lifetime period (18–30 years old, last 10 years except the last 12 months, and last 12 months). After each production, patients were asked to rate valence and intensity of memory. The mean scores of control and non-emotional group are presented. The scores of TGA patients belonging to the emotional group are reported individually.

* The pathological z score (calculated according to the number of controls and a threshold of 0·05) was 1·78.

Discussion

Some studies (Guillery-Girard et al. Reference Guillery-Girard, Desgranges, Urban, Piolino, de la Sayette and Eustache2005) highlighted several profiles of recovery after a TGA. Our study confirms this result and shows that only TGA patients with an anxious and/or depressive profile had persistent cognitive deficits in episodic memory. On the task assessing the anterograde component of episodic memory, these patients had difficulty distinguishing targets from lures, and only did so in the case of negative items. This ‘semantic cohesiveness’ phenomenon, already observed in depression (Danion et al. Reference Danion, Kauffmann-Muller, Grange, Zimmermann and Greth1995), is explained by the semantic closeness of emotional words which then act as ‘related lures’ and lead to a high proportion of false alarms. This effect limited to negative words reflects a phenomenon of mood congruency (Tarsia et al. Reference Tarsia, Power and Sanavio2003). Patients with an anxious or depressive vulnerability produced less specific, less intense and even more negative memories for the most distant period, which corresponds to the ‘reminiscence bump’, usually characterized by detailed and happy memories (Berntsen & Rubin, Reference Berntsen and Rubin2002; Piolino et al. Reference Piolino, Desgranges, Clarys, Guillery-Girard, Taconnat, Isingrini and Eustache2006). Furthermore, it has been shown (D'Argembeau et al. Reference D'Argembeau, Comblain and Van der Linden2003) that negative memories are less episodic than positive ones. Thus, our results suggest that anxiety and depression might slow the recovery process and even prevent a full recovery.

While not questioning the organic nature of TGA, the emotional state of patients and the events surrounding the episode suggest that in some patients, psychogenic factors may intervene (Lucchelli & Spinnler, Reference Lucchelli and Spinnler2002). Kopelman (Reference Kopelman2000) has proposed a revised model of amnesia, in which all types of amnesia take place along the same continuum according to the contribution of organic and psychogenic factors to their aetiology. Given this hypothesis, the two subgroups of TGA patients (with or without psychopathological disorders) would be situated at different points of the continuum. Whereas some TGA would be closely linked to the concept of psychogenic amnesia, i.e. triggered by psychopathological factors, other kinds of TGA would be entirely due to organic factors.

In conclusion, our study shows that psychopathological factors are a central issue in the onset of and recovery from TGA. However, further studies combining psychopathological, neuropsychological and neuroimaging approaches need to be performed to confirm the influence of psychopathological factors in TGA.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by the Caen University Hospital as a project of clinical research. The authors thank Patrice Clochon for his advice on the statistical analysis, Estelle Peuziat for her help in conducting the patients' neuropsychological examination, and the staff of the emergency unit at Caen University Hospital for their invaluable assistance.

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Beck, A. T., Rial, W. Y. & Rickets, K. (1974). Short form of Depression Inventory: cross validation. Psychological Reports 34, 11841186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berntsen, D. & Rubin, D. C. (2002). Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: the recall of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories. Psychology and Aging 17, 636652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Argembeau, A., Comblain, C. & Van der Linden, M. (2003). Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for positive, negative and neutral events. Applied Cognitive Psychology 17, 281294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danion, J. M., Kauffmann-Muller, F., Grange, D., Zimmermann, M. A. & Greth, P. (1995). Affective valence of words, explicit and implicit memory in clinical depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 34, 227234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardiner, J. M., Ramponi, C. & Richardson-Klavehn, A. (2002). Recognition memory and decision processes: a meta-analysis of remember, know, and guess responses. Memory 10, 8398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillery-Girard, B., Desgranges, B., Urban, C., Piolino, P., de la Sayette, V. & Eustache, F. (2005). The dynamic time course of memory recovery in transient global amnesia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 75, 15321540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inzitari, D., Pantoni, L., Lamassa, M., Pallanti, S., Pracucci, G. & Marini, P. (1997). Emotional arousal and phobia in transient global amnesia. Archives of Neurology 54, 866873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopelman, M. D. (2000). Focal retrograde amnesia and the attribution of causality: An exceptionally critical review. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17, 585621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucchelli, F. & Spinnler, H. (2002). The ‘psychogenic’ versus ‘organic’ conundrum of pure retrograde amnesia: is it still worth pursuing? Cortex 38, 665669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Belliard, S., Matuszewski, V., Lalevée, C., de la Sayette, V. & Eustache, F. (2003). Autobiographical memory and autonoetic consciousness: triple dissociation in neurodegenerative diseases. Brain 126, 22032219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Clarys, D., Guillery-Girard, B., Taconnat, L., Isingrini, M. & Eustache, F. (2006). Autobiographical memory, autonoetic consciousness, and self-perspective in aging. Psychology and Aging 21, 510525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quinette, P., Guillery-Girard, B., Dayan, J., de la Sayette, V., Marquis, S., Viader, F., Desgranges, B. & Eustache, F. (2006). What does transient global amnesia really mean? Review of the literature and thorough study of 142 cases. Brain 129, 16401658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety. Consulting Psychologist Press Inc.: Palo Alto.Google Scholar
Tarsia, M., Power, M. J. & Sanavio, E. (2003). Implicit and explicit memory biases in mixed anxiety-depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 77, 213225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. A hierarchical cluster analysis was carried out with same clinical variables than those used in the study of Quinette et al. (2006). Two new measures were added: level of anxiety (with a cut-off point of 55 in the Spielberger Trait Anxiety Inventory; Spielberger, 1983) and depression (with a cut-off point of 8, in the Beck Depression Inventory; Beck et al.1974). The results shows four clusters: (1) transient global amnesia (TGA) preceded by physical precipitants, (2) TGA preceded by emotional episodes accompanied by psychopathological factors, (3) patients with vascular risk factors, (4) TGA with no particular characteristics. The cluster in boldface includes all the emotional characteristics.

Figure 1

Table 1. Performances of transient global amnesia (TGA) patients and normal controls on the autobiographical task