Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T23:02:39.968Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between physical home environmental factors and vegetable consumption among Norwegian 3–5-year-olds: the BRA-study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2016

Anne Lene Kristiansen*
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, PO Box 1046 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
Mona Bjelland
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, PO Box 1046 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
Anne Himberg-Sundet
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, PO Box 1046 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
Nanna Lien
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, PO Box 1046 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
Lene Frost Andersen
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, Institute of Basic Medical Sciences, University of Oslo, PO Box 1046 Blindern, 0316 Oslo, Norway
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

First, to explore item pools developed to measure the physical home environment of pre-school children and assess the psychometric properties of these item pools; second, to explore associations between this environment and vegetable consumption among Norwegian 3–5-year-olds.

Design

Data were collected in three steps: (i) a parental web-based questionnaire assessing the child’s vegetable intake and factors potentially influencing the child’s vegetable consumption; (ii) direct observation of the children’s fruit, berry and vegetable intakes at two meals in one day in the kindergarten; and (iii) a parental web-based 24 h recall.

Setting

The target group for this study was pre-school children born in 2010 and 2011, attending public or private kindergartens in the counties of Vestfold and Buskerud, Norway.

Subjects

A total of 633 children participated.

Results

Principal component analysis on the thirteen-item pool assessing availability/accessibility resulted in two factors labelled ‘availability at home’ and ‘accessibility at home’, while the eight-item pool assessing barriers resulted in two factors labelled ‘serving barriers’ and ‘purchase barriers’. The psychometric properties of these factors were satisfactory. Linear regression of the associations between vegetable intake and the factors showed generally positive associations with ‘availability at home’ and ‘accessibility at home’ and negative associations with ‘serving barriers’.

Conclusions

This age group has so far been understudied and there is a need for comparable studies. Our findings highlight the importance of targeting the physical home environment of pre-school children in future interventions as there are important modifiable factors that both promote and hinder vegetable consumption in this environment.

Type
Research Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2016 

Worldwide, non-communicable diseases such as CVD, cancers, chronic respiratory diseases and diabetes type 2 cause almost 70 % of all deaths( 1 ). Non-communicable diseases develop slowly over many years; hence promoting a healthy diet early in life may contribute to reduced incidence of these diseases in the long run. Adequate consumption of fruits and vegetables is linked to a reduced risk of some non-communicable diseases( Reference Hu, Huang and Wang 2 Reference Wang, Ouyang and Liu 4 ). Despite the health benefits of diets rich in fruits and vegetables, many countries face a challenge with regard to their population’s intake of these food groups( Reference Micha, Khatibzadeh and Shi 5 ). This is also the case in Norway, where the latest national dietary surveys among adults, schoolchildren and pre-school children( Reference Kristiansen, Lande and Andersen 6 Reference Øverby, Kristiansen and Andersen 10 ) all report low consumption of vegetables. Reasons for inadequate consumption of fruits and vegetables are complex. For example, food preferences play an important role in children’s food choices and it is suggested that food preferences are formed by a combination of genetic and environmental factors( Reference Scaglioni, Arrizza and Vecchi 11 , Reference Wardle and Cooke 12 ). However, most children can learn to accept foods that are made available to them( Reference Savage, Fisher and Birch 13 ) even though they are not preferred, especially through repeated taste exposure( Reference Wardle and Cooke 12 , Reference Cooke 14 ).

There is evidence of the tracking of dietary intake from childhood to adulthood( Reference Bjelland, Brantsaeter and Haugen 15 Reference Totland, Gebremariam and Lien 18 ); consequently early childhood represents a critical period for the establishment of dietary habits( Reference Bjelland, Brantsaeter and Haugen 15 ). In a life course perspective, the home environment is acknowledged as the first environment to shape dietary habits and food preferences( Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski 19 ).

According to Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski( Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski 19 ), factors within the home environment can be characterized as physical factors, sociocultural factors and political/economic factors. A repeated finding across most reviews including school-aged children is that home physical availability and accessibility of fruits and vegetables are strong positive factors for consumption( Reference Blanchette and Brug 20 Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ). Availability implies that food items are present in the home, while accessibility implies that food items are in a ‘ready to eat’ form that facilitates their consumption( Reference Cullen, Baranowski and Owens 26 ). Furthermore, parental intake, parental modelling and parental encouragement act as important positive sociocultural factors for consumption of fruits and vegetables in that age group( Reference Blanchette and Brug 20 , Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ). Finally, as an economic factor, parental socio-economic position is of significance, where low socio-economic position is associated with low fruit and vegetable intake( Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 , Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 , Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ).

There are several distinctions between fruits and vegetables that play an important role when deciding to eat them; for example, sweet-tasting foods such as fruits are generally accepted more easily in childhood than foods containing bitter components such as vegetables( Reference Savage, Fisher and Birch 13 ). Therefore, Glasson et al.( Reference Glasson, Chapman and James 27 ) recommend treating fruits and vegetables as separate food groups when planning interventions. However, home environmental factors associated with fruit and vegetable consumption are often reported together in previous reviews among school-aged children( Reference Blanchette and Brug 20 , Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 , Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 , Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ).

Greater understanding of factors that promote or hinder vegetable consumption among young children is desirable. Although there is substantial research in the area of the relationship between childhood diet and parental food practices( Reference de Lauzon-Guillain, Oliveira and Charles 28 Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher 30 ) (the sociocultural environment), other aspects of the home environment of pre-school children are relatively unexplored( Reference Spurrier, Magarey and Golley 31 ). The present study aimed to address this gap, first by exploring item pools developed to measure the physical home environment of Norwegian pre-school children and assessing the psychometric properties of these item pools, and second by exploring the associations between the physical home environment and vegetable consumption among these children.

Methods

Study design and participants

The BRA-study (an acronym for the Norwegian words Barnehage (kindergarten), gRønnsaker (vegetables) and fAmilie (family)) is an intervention study with an overall aim to improve vegetable intake among pre-school children (aged 3–5 years at baseline) through changing the food environment and dietary practices in the kindergarten and the home. More specifically, the aims are to increase the daily frequency of vegetable intake, to increase the variety of vegetables eaten over a month and to increase the daily amount of vegetables consumed. The target group for the BRA-study is pre-school children born in 2010 and 2011, attending public or private kindergartens in the counties of Vestfold and Buskerud, Norway. All 479 public and private kindergartens in these two counties were invited by letter to participate in the BRA-study. Seventy-three kindergartens chose to participate (response rate 15·2 %). Parents of 1631 children born in 2010–2011 in the seventy-three kindergartens were invited by letter to participate. Parental consent was obtained for 633 children (response rate 38·8 %). Families were allowed to participate with more than one child (forty-five children in the total sample were siblings). The number of participating children in each kindergarten varied from no children up to twenty-three children. For the present study, only data from the baseline surveys among parents and the baseline observation of the children were included.

Design and methods

At baseline, data about the child and the home environment were collected in three steps (Fig. 1) as follows.

  1. 1. A parental web-based questionnaire assessing frequency and variety of the child’s vegetable intake, as well as factors potentially influencing the child’s vegetable consumption was filled in for 439 children (69 % of the 633).

  2. 2. Among a sub-sample (n 411) of the participating children (65 % of the 633), a direct observation of the children’s fruit, berry and vegetable intakes at two meals in one day in the kindergarten was conducted.

  3. 3. A parental web-based 24 h recall for assessing the child’s intakes of fruits, berries and vegetables was filled in for 470 children (74 % of the 633).

Fig. 1 Flowchart showing recruitment of and data collection from invited and participating 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study, Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway, in total and according to methods used in relation to the home environment at baseline

The number of children having data from all three steps was 246.

Step 1: parental web-based questionnaire

In March 2015, all parents of participating children in the BRA-study (n 633) received a link to a web-based questionnaire by email. If the family participated with more than one child, parents were instructed to answer separately for each child. One email reminder was sent out to non-responders about 3 weeks after the first email.

The questionnaire was tested in a pilot study with ten mothers. The questionnaire was administered in the same way and under similar conditions as for the main survey; however, the mothers were requested to give feedback to the researchers on questions and items that were not clear to them or felt too sensitive/personal. After the pilot test, six items were revised to increase clarity, one item was deleted due to being too equal to another item and three item pools of totally fifteen items were deleted owing to considerations of the time used to fill in the questionnaire.

The final questionnaire included fifty-three questions, divided in two parts. The first part of the questionnaire primarily aimed to measure the usual vegetable intake. Parents were asked to think about the last couple of months, and the frequency and variety of eighteen different types of vegetables were assessed with the question: ‘How often does your child eat the following vegetables?’ Response alternatives were (times/d in parenthesis) ‘1–3 times a month’ (0·07), ‘1 time per week’ (0·14), ‘2 times per week’ (0·29), ‘3 times per week’ (0·43), ‘4 times per week’ (0·57), ‘5 times per week’ (0·71), ‘6 times per week’ (0·86), ‘every day’ (1·0) and ‘2 or more times per day’ (2·0). This question was taken from the national dietary survey among Norwegian 2-year-olds( Reference Kristiansen, Lande and Andersen 6 ) mapping total dietary intake. A validation study has been undertaken for that survey, but not for the modified question used in the present study. Pickled and preserved vegetables and potatoes were not included as vegetables in the present study.

The second part of the questionnaire aimed to measure potential factors within the home environment assumed to be related to vegetable intake. In the present paper, only item pools related to the physical factors within the home environment, i.e. the items assessing availability/accessibility and barriers, are presented. The item pool assessing availability/accessibility consisted of thirteen items while the item pool assessing barriers consisted of eight items. The item pools used were composed of modified versions of statements and questions used in previous Norwegian and international studies among children( Reference Baranowski, Beltran and Chen 32 Reference Zeinstra, Koelen and Kok 35 ). Translation and back-translation of statements and questions available in English were conducted by fluent speakers of the English and Norwegian languages. Responses were given on a 5-point scale ranging from ‘totally disagree’ (=1) to ‘totally agree’ (=5), with a neutral midpoint.

The questionnaire also requested information like child gender, child birth year, number of siblings in the household, age of the respondent, respondent’s relationship to the child, cohabitant status of the respondent, nationality of the respondent and nationality of the mother/father of the child.

Step 2: direct observation

As part of the baseline data collection, the research team members visited all seventy-three kindergartens in April to June 2015. Direct observation was conducted if the kindergarten had three or more participating children in a department. Each researcher observed one to four children simultaneously, with a maximum of eight children in total from each kindergarten being observed. Preferably, children of mothers with a low educational level were observed and otherwise children were chosen for observation at random. During observations, the researchers were standing close to tables where the children sat and recorded intakes of fruits, berries and vegetables on an observational form. These foods could be provided by the kindergarten, by the parents or by both the kindergarten and the parents. As children in kindergartens often are offered sliced fruits and vegetables rather than whole pieces, a coloured picture sheet accompanied the observational form to assist researchers in deciding the portion size. The picture sheet contained pictures of nine different shapes like a slice of cucumber, stick of sweet pepper, section of orange, piece of banana, etc. In general, most kindergartens in Norway serve a hot meal for lunch once per week( 36 ). Therefore, two photograph series with four different portion sizes of a vegetable soup and a vegetable stew were included. After the data collection, the observed portion sizes were converted to grams based on reference weights. The primary source for reference weights was the Norwegian guide on Weights, Measures and Portion Sizes for Foods ( 37 ). However, when no reference weight was available, the research team developed a secondary reference by weighing samples of foods according to the shapes of foods observed. In this secondary reference, the number of samples for each shape of food varied; however, a mean weight of three up to thirteen samples for each shape of food was used as standard.

The researchers gave all participating children (n 633) a card to take home, which informed about the types of fruits, berries and vegetables served in the child’s department on the actual day. Moreover, the card informed parents that they would receive an email from the research team about the 24 h recall (see step 3).

All researchers were trained in observation and all procedures and measurements were conducted according to a standardized protocol.

During data collection there were occasionally options for the researchers to observe the same child/children to assess the inter-rater reliability between pairs of observers. Inter-rater reliability was assessed in sixteen kindergartens, with the first pair of observers observing sixty-six children simultaneously, while the second pair of observers observed twelve children simultaneously. The level of agreement between pairs of observers was estimated by calculating the intra-class correlation. The intra-class correlation for the first pair of observers was 0·96, while for the second pair it was 0·97.

Step 3: parental web-based 24 h recall

In the evening (after 21.00 hours) on the day of the direct observation (step 2), all parents received a link to a web-based 24 h recall by email. If the family participated with more than one child, parents were instructed to answer separately for each child. One email reminder was sent out to non-responders 2 d after the first email.

The 24 h recall was designed to measure the participating child’s intakes of fruits, berries and vegetables on the preceding day, with an extra focus on vegetable intake. To ease the reporting, the 24 h was divided in to six eating occasions: breakfast, snack meal 1 (intake after breakfast, but before lunch), lunch, snack meal 2 (intake after lunch but before dinner), dinner and snack meal 3 (intake after dinner and until the child went to bed).

For breakfast, snack meal 1 and snack meal 3, frequencies and amounts of six different types of vegetables were reported. For lunch, snack meal 2 and dinner, frequencies and amounts of six additional types of vegetables were reported. To assist parents in reporting amount of vegetables eaten, a photograph series with four different portion sizes was used. However, photographs of the amount of vegetable were displayed only if the vegetable was reported used. The focus was on everyday intake of vegetables; therefore 24 h recalls reporting on vegetable intake during a weekend day were excluded.

Parents who completed both web-based questionnaires (step 1 and step 3) were entered into a lottery with rewards; two of about 535 € and one of about 1070 €.

Classification of intake of vegetables

The web-based questionnaire (step 1) provided information about frequency and variety of vegetable intake. A vegetable was defined as not used only if the frequency was reported to be ‘never’. Due to asking about eighteen different vegetables in the questionnaire, 18 was the maximum variation a child could have in vegetable intake over a month.

Parental ability to recall their child’s diet when the child is in childcare may be a limiting factor when using dietary recall methodology among pre-school children( Reference Andersen, Lioret and Brants 38 , Reference Baranowski, Sprague and Baranowski 39 ). Therefore, the amount of vegetables consumed was calculated based on data from the 24 h recall (when at home, step 3) and the direct observation (when in kindergarten, step 2). Hence, only children with data from both step 2 and step 3 were eligible to be included in analyses considering amount of vegetables. In the 24 h recall, the parental reported intake of vegetables at lunch and at snack meal 2 was replaced by the observed vegetable intake in the kindergarten. However, if a child had been picked up from the kindergarten before snack meal 2 (n 15), only the lunch meal was replaced in the 24 h recall.

Data analysis

The number of participants included in data analysis varied for the different methods used. For the principal component analysis (PCA), participants included were those with data from the parental web-based questionnaire (step 1, n 439). This sample was also eligible to be included in the linear regression analyses; however, due to missing data for covariates (primarily maternal education), the number of participants in the linear regression analyses for variety and frequency was 395. For the analysis regarding amount of vegetables consumed, participants had to have data from both step 2 and step 3. There were 246 participants with such data; however, twenty-seven were lost due to missing data on covariates (primarily maternal education) and twenty-two were lost due to reporting vegetable intake of a weekend day in the 24 h recall, leaving 197 participants to be included in the analysis concerning amount of vegetables consumed.

PCA was used for exploring factors assumed to be related to vegetable intake. All items asked about in an item pool were entered into PCA. The number of factors retained from the PCA was chosen on the basis of the eigenvalue (explained variance), with the decision criterion of ≥1·0, and the interpretability of the factors. For availability/accessibility, the first two factors were chosen for further analysis. The remaining factors all had eigenvalue less than 1·0, and thus individually explained only a small fraction of the overall variance in the data. To improve the interpretation of the data, the two-factor solution was rotated by varimax rotation. The same pattern as for availability/accessibility was seen for barriers; hence a two-factor solution was chosen for further analysis of this item pool as well.

Items were considered to load on a factor if they had factor loadings >0·3( Reference Kline 40 ). Items not loading on a factor (factor loading <0·3) were eliminated. Items with high loading on more than one factor were included in the factor where they had the highest loading.

To assess the psychometric properties of the factors derived from the PCA, the internal reliability of the factors was calculated by corrected item–total correlation and Cronbach’s alpha (α). Corrected item–total correlation values >0·30 were considered good and values <0·15 were considered unreliable since that would indicate lack of homogeneity of the items within an item pool( Reference Nunnally and Bernstein 41 ). Cronbach’s α of 0·7 or 0·8 is often classified as an acceptable value( Reference Field 42 ).

Clustering effects due to kindergartens being the unit of recruitment were checked using the linear mixed-model procedure( Reference Heck, Thomas and Tabata 43 ). The unexplained variance in frequency, variation and amount of vegetables at the kindergarten level was 0 to 1·6 %, which is considered so low that this was not taken into account in the analysis.

Linear regression was applied to study the relationship between variation, frequency and amount of vegetable intake among the 3–5-year-olds and the four new factors derived from the PCA (‘availability at home’, ‘accessibility at home’, ‘serving barriers’ and ‘purchase barriers’). For every child a composite score for each of the four factors was created, and this score was further used in linear regression analysis. To be included in the linear regression analysis, participants had to have response on four out of five items within the factor ‘availability at home’ and likewise for the factor ‘accessibility at home’. To be included in the linear regression analysis according to ‘serving barriers’, participants had to have response on four out of five items, while for ‘purchase barriers’ participants had to have response on two out of three items.

All models were adjusted for child gender, child birth year and maternal educational level. Maternal and paternal educational level was assessed in the consent form. The educational level was assessed by four pre-coded categories, which were combined into two categories in the analysis: low education (upper secondary school or less) and high education (university college/university). Maternal education was used as an indicator of the socio-economic position of the family as maternal education was reported with fewer missing values compared with paternal education.

Interactions between maternal education and each of the four factors were tested if the associations in the linear regression analysis were significant.

All P values are two-sided, with values <0·05 considered statistically significant. All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical software package IBM® SPSS® Statistics Version 22.0.

Results

Table 1 presents selected characteristics of the children and their parents analysed in PCA. Boys and girls were equally represented, as was child year of birth. Parental respondents were aged between 24 and 60 years, with a mean age of 35·4 (sd 5·1) years. Most of the respondents were the mother of the child, most parents were Norwegians or from other European countries and most had higher education. The sample used in the regression analysis (n 395 and n 197) had the same distribution in characteristics as those presented in Table 1. Descriptive statistics for vegetable intake are also presented in Table 1. According to the reported variation and frequency of vegetable intake, less than 2 % of the children reported consumption to be ‘never’. According to observations and the 24 h recall data, results showed that less than 6 % of the children did not consume vegetables at all on the day when observations and recalls were done (data not shown).

Table 1 Characteristics of 3–5-year-old children and their parents in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

* Total number of children, n 197.

Exploration of item pools developed to measure the physical home environment

For the thirteen availability/accessibility items, the two factors derived from PCA were labelled ‘availability at home’ (five items) and ‘accessibility at home’ (five items; Table 2), explaining approximately 42 % of the total variance. Three items were not included in the final factor structure as the factor loadings for these items were low (<0·3). These were ‘During a meal, I remind my child several times to eat vegetables’, ‘I serve vegetables I know my child does not like several times a month’ and ‘I usually serve my child with vegetables’. For the eight barrier items, the two factors derived from PCA explained approximately 57 % of the total variance. The factors were labelled as ‘serving barriers’ (five items) and ‘purchase barriers’ (three items). Most items within each of the four new factors had high factor loadings (i.e. ≥0·6).

Table 2 Items and factor loadingsFootnote * for the factors derived from the principal component analysis reported by parents of 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

* Only items with factor loadings >0·3 are displayed.

Table 3 presents the mean values and the psychometric properties of the four factors derived from the PCA. The mean score for ‘availability at home’ and ‘accessibility at home’ was high, 4·13 and 3·70 respectively, indicating that most parents partly agreed with the statements included in these factors. The mean score for ‘purchase barriers’ and ‘serving barriers’ was low, indicating that most parents partly disagreed with the statements included in these factors.

Table 3 Mean value, standard deviation, corrected item–total correlation (CITC) and Cronbach’s alpha (α) for the factors derived from the principal component analysis reported by parents of 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

* Responses were given on 5-point scales ranging from ‘totally disagree’ (=1) to ‘totally agree’ (=5), with a neutral midpoint.

All values of corrected item–total correlation were 0·36 or above, and all values of Cronbach’s α were above 0·7.

Associations between factors of the physical home environment and vegetable consumption

For the factor ‘availability at home’, there was a significant interaction with maternal education and variation in vegetable intake (P=0·001), frequency of vegetable intake (P=0·015) and amount of vegetables consumed (P<0·001). Every unit increase in the factor ‘availability at home’ was associated with a higher increase in variation of vegetable intake among children of highly educated mothers compared with children of low-educated mothers (2·7 types/month v. 1·2 types/month, respectively; Table 4). The same pattern was observed for frequency of vegetable intake, where children of higher educated mothers had a higher frequency of intake compared with those with lower educated mothers (1·3 times/d v. 0·8 times/d, respectively). In opposition, a significantly higher amount of vegetables consumed was observed among those where maternal education was low compared with those where maternal education was high (79·7 g/d v. 28·9 g/d, respectively).

Table 4 Adjusted bivariate associationsFootnote * between physical home environmental factors and vegetable consumption among 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study, Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

* B is adjusted for maternal education, child gender and child birth year.

Total number of children, n 395.

Total number of children, n 197.

§ B is adjusted for child gender and child birth year.

Total number of children, n 116.

Total number of children, n 279.

** Total number of children, n 62.

†† Total number of children, n 135.

Every unit increase in the factor ‘accessibility at home’ was significantly associated with an increased frequency of vegetable intake, approximately an increase of 2–3 times/week (P<0·001), and with an average increase in the amount of vegetables consumed by roughly 17 g/d (P=0·018).

Significant associations were also seen for the factor ‘serving barriers’. Every unit increase in this factor was associated with a reduction in vegetable variety of about 2 types/month, a reduction in frequency of vegetable intake of about 1 time/d and an average reduction in amount of vegetables consumed of more than 38 g/d (all P<0·001). Finally, no significant associations were observed for the factor ‘purchase barriers’ and vegetable consumption.

Discussion

Presented in the current paper are four distinct factors of the physical home environment of Norwegian 3–5-year-olds, which were labelled ‘availability at home’, ‘accessibility at home’, ‘serving barriers’ and ‘purchase barriers’. The psychometric properties of the factors were satisfactory. Associations between factors and vegetable consumption indicated that within the home environment of these Norwegian pre-school children, there are important physical factors that both promote and hinder vegetable consumption.

Psychometric properties of factors

Most of the items used in the present study were composed of modified versions of statements and questions used in previous Norwegian and international studies among pre-school and school-aged children( Reference Baranowski, Beltran and Chen 32 Reference Zeinstra, Koelen and Kok 35 ), resulting in unknown validity and reliability. The psychometric properties of the factors in the present study were satisfactory, probably indicating suitability for use among children in general. Yet, more studies are needed to verify this. All items correlated with the factor to a good degree (with the lowest correlation being 0·36)( Reference Field 42 ). Additionally, all four factors had acceptable internal consistency, with Cronbach’s α values above 0·7. Cronbach’s α is sensitive to the number of items included in a scale, with increasing numbers of items in a scale possibly resulting in an increased value( Reference Field 42 ). None of the factors in the present study included more than five items, so this has probably not affected the results. Appropriate sample size when conducting PCA has been debated, but it seems reasonable to include a minimum of 300 participants( Reference Field 42 ). PCA was conducted on a sample consisting of more than 400 participants; hence the criterion of a large sample size seems fulfilled.

Physical home environment factors positively associated with vegetable consumption

The present study shows that home availability and accessibility are positively associated with vegetable consumption among the 3–5-year-olds. Other studies among pre-school children have also reported positive associations between vegetable intake and availability and accessibility. For example, a study among Australian 4–5-year-olds observed a strong positive association between home availability of vegetables and child intake( Reference Spurrier, Magarey and Golley 31 ). In another study focusing on Australian 3–5-year-olds( Reference Wyse, Campbell and Nathan 44 ), a positive association between fruit and vegetable consumption and home availability and accessibility was found. However, results for the two food groups were reported together and vegetable consumption included intake of potatoes and sweet potatoes. In a study among English 2–5-year-old children( Reference McGowan, Croker and Wardle 45 ) no significant association between home availability and vegetable intake was observed. In that study, vegetable intake and availability were both assessed with one question. The authors commented that there was a limited variation in parental response for the question assessing vegetable availability, which may have reduced the explanatory power of that result.

Comparing results across studies is challenging. Associations reported may for example depend on how vegetable intake is estimated, as studies have shown that questionnaires including longer vegetable lists result in higher estimates compared with shorter vegetable lists( Reference Krebs-Smith, Heimendinger and Subar 46 , Reference Roark and Niederhauser 47 ). Further, intake data may represent different time frames as in the present study where the 24 h recall reflects one random weekday, while frequency and variation reflect usual vegetable intake over the last couple of months.

Our results indicate that there are differences in associations according to vegetable consumption when it comes to availability and accessibility, supporting that the two factors should be treated separately. Yet, the item ‘I cut up vegetables that my child can eat between meals’ had the highest factor loading for the factor ‘availability at home’ (0·48), while theoretically this item should be included in the factor ‘accessibility at home’. However, the factor loading for this item on that factor was only 0·23 (data not shown).

We observed differences in the strength of associations for availability and accessibility according to how vegetable consumption was characterized; this also supports the view that variation, frequency and amount of vegetables are different aspects of the behaviour of vegetable intake and that these aspects might be associated with different factors. As Rasmussen et al.( Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 ) point out, increasing variation of vegetables in the home may lead to increased amount consumed, but may be unrelated to increased frequency.

The association between low parental education level and low intake of fruits and vegetables among children is well known( Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 , Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 , Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ). This was also found in the present paper, where the association between ‘availability at home’ and variation and frequency of vegetable intake was higher among children of highly educated mothers compared with children of low-educated mothers. However, for the association according to amount of vegetables, unexpectedly opposite associations were found. It is difficult to point out likely reasons for this finding; one explanation could be differences in the reporting in the 24 h recall between the two educational groups, where the low-educated mothers particularly reported higher intake of vegetables for breakfast compared with high-educated mothers. Such interactions need to be investigated further.

Physical home environment factors negatively associated with vegetable consumption

In the present study every unit increase in the factor ‘serving barriers’ (e.g. ‘It is difficult to use vegetables in the daily cooking’) was significantly associated with a reduction in variation, frequency and amount of vegetables consumed, while ‘purchase barriers’ was not. It might be that purchase barriers included too few items to assess actual purchase barriers, as the PCA resulted in only three items loading on this factor. Moreover, as grocery stores in Norway generally hold high quality on fresh foods such as vegetables, items included might be items that these participants do not worry about. Finally, as this sample consisted mostly of well-educated two-parent families, vegetable prices might not be an issue.

Across studies, scales to measure barriers are unalike and items included may be valued differently according to age, socio-economic position, culture and so on. Among adults, a review of factors associated with fruit and vegetable intake found that six out of nine studies reported a negative association between barriers and fruit and vegetable intake( Reference Shaikh, Yaroch and Nebeling 48 ). In the review by Rasmussen et al.( Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 ), one out of three studies reported a negative association between barriers and fruit and vegetable intake among 6–18-year-old children. Another review of factors associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in the same age group, including only qualitative studies, found that lack of convenience and price were the most discussed barriers( Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 ). One study focusing on 5–6-year-old Australian pre-school children found that the factor assessing barriers (‘high cost/low preference for fruits and vegetables’) was unrelated to vegetable consumption( Reference Campbell, Crawford and Ball 49 ). The authors found this surprising, and reported probable causes for this: ‘It may be that by combining two separate constructs, for two separate groups of foods, that is perceptions of cost (for fruit and vegetables) and perceptions of family liking (for fruits and vegetables), into one factor (as suggested by factor analysis), has limited the capacity to describe associations’ (p. 1278).

As barriers were assessed by several items and reporting vegetable intake in three different ways, we have extended current knowledge by demonstrating that ‘serving barriers’ is strongly and negatively associated with vegetable consumption in this sample.

Finally, as discussed earlier, differences between studies may be attributable to methodological differences, ways of assessing factors and ways of assessing vegetable consumption, in addition to subjective decisions made by researches when analysing data.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of the present study are first of all the age group, which so far has been understudied. Further, as vegetable consumption was assessed in three ways, the associations with factors of the physical home environment present a broad picture of potential relationships. Parental ability to recall their child’s diet when the child is in childcare may be a limiting factor( Reference Andersen, Lioret and Brants 38 , Reference Baranowski, Sprague and Baranowski 39 ). This was also supported by the parents in the present study, as more than forty responders of the 24 h recall (step 3) gave feedback regarding difficulties in reporting intake when child was in kindergarten (data not shown). Therefore, to collect a more precise picture of vegetable intake, parental reports were combined with observations by the research staff( Reference Foster and Adamson 50 ).

Interpretation of findings should also take some limitations into account. First, the data are cross-sectional and although cross-sectional data can demonstrate associations, they cannot be used to determine cause and effect. Further, the participation rate among parents in the present study is lower than participation rates in national dietary surveys among Norwegian pre-school children( Reference Kristiansen, Lande and Andersen 6 , 7 , Reference Øverby, Kristiansen and Andersen 10 ); this might have led to a biased sample of those most interested in vegetables. As discussed earlier, low parental education is associated with low intake of fruits and vegetables( Reference Krolner, Rasmussen and Brug 22 , Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp 24 , Reference van der Horst, Oenema and Ferreira 25 ). The level of parental education in our sample of participants was higher compared with the national educational level in Norway and this might also have influenced the findings. In Norway, 39 % of men and 55 % of women aged 35–39 years had a high educational level in 2015( 51 ), compared with 54 and 71 %, respectively, in the present study. In addition, to be included in the analyses of amount of vegetables consumed, participants had to have data from two questionnaires and the observations, which might further have led to a biased sample. The significant interaction between ‘availability at home’ and amount of vegetables consumed surprisingly showed lower amount of vegetables consumed among children of highly educated mothers compared with children of low-educated mothers, and such interactions need to be investigated further.

Conclusion

Exploring item pools developed to measure the physical home environment of Norwegian pre-school children resulted in four factors with satisfactory psychometric properties. The factors ‘home availability’ and ‘home accessibility’ were both strongly and positively associated with vegetable consumption. Further, ‘serving barriers’ was negatively associated with variation, frequency and amount of vegetables consumed, while ‘purchase barriers’ was not.

Our findings highlight the importance of targeting the physical home environment of pre-school children in future interventions as there are important modifiable factors that both promote and hinder vegetable consumption in this environment. This age group has so far been understudied and there is a need for comparable studies; hence further research should target these physical home environmental factors when aiming to increase vegetable consumption among pre-school children.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors are grateful to all children, their parents and the kindergarten staff who participated in the BRA-study. Financial support: The present study was supported by the Research Council of Norway (project number 228452) with supplementary funds from the Throne Holst Nutrition Research Foundation, University of Oslo. The funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest. Authorship: A.L.K. conducted the statistical analyses and wrote the draft of the manuscript. A.L.K., M.B., N.L. and L.F.A. participated in designing the BRA-study. All authors participated in project planning and data collection. All authors have critically revised the manuscript, and read and approved the final version of the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving human subjects/patients were approved by the Norwegian Social Science Data Services. Written informed consent for the child was obtained from all parents who agreed to participate (n 633).

References

1. World Health Organization (2014) Global Status Report on Noncommunicable Diseases 2014. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
2. Hu, D, Huang, J, Wang, Y et al. (2014) Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Stroke 45, 16131619.Google Scholar
3. Li, M, Fan, Y, Zhang, X et al. (2014) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus: meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ Open 4, e005497.Google Scholar
4. Wang, X, Ouyang, Y, Liu, J et al. (2014) Fruit and vegetable consumption and mortality from all causes, cardiovascular disease, and cancer: systematic review and dose–response meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. BMJ 349, g4490.Google Scholar
5. Micha, R, Khatibzadeh, S, Shi, P et al. (2015) Global, regional and national consumption of major food groups in 1990 and 2010: a systematic analysis including 266 country-specific nutrition surveys worldwide. BMJ Open 5, e008705.Google Scholar
6. Kristiansen, AL, Lande, B & Andersen, LF (2009) Småbarnskost 2007 – Dietary Habits Among 2-Year-Old Children in Norway. Report no. IS-1731. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
7. Norwegian Directorate of Health (2002) Ungkost – Dietary Habits Among 4-Year-Olds. Report no. IS-1067. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
8. Norwegian Directorate of Health (2012) Norkost 3 – Dietary Habits Among Adults in Norway in 201011. Report no. IS-2000. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
9. Norwegian Institute of Public Health (2016) Ungkost 3 – Dietary Intake Among 9 and 13-Year Olds in 2015. Oslo: Norwegian Institute of Public Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
10. Øverby, NC, Kristiansen, AL, Andersen, LF et al. (2009) Spedkost 12 Måneder – Norwegian National Dietary Survey Among Infants at 12 months. Report no. IS-1635. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
11. Scaglioni, S, Arrizza, C, Vecchi, F et al. (2011) Determinants of children’s eating behavior. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 6 Suppl., 2006S2011S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12. Wardle, J & Cooke, L (2008) Genetic and environmental determinants of children’s food preferences. Br J Nutr 99, Suppl. 1, S15S21.Google Scholar
13. Savage, JS, Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (2007) Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics 35, 2234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14. Cooke, L (2007) The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet 20, 294301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15. Bjelland, M, Brantsaeter, AL, Haugen, M et al. (2013) Changes and tracking of fruit, vegetables and sugar-sweetened beverages intake from 18 months to 7 years in the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study. BMC Public Health 13, 793.Google Scholar
16. Craigie, AM, Lake, AA, Kelly, SA et al. (2011) Tracking of obesity-related behaviours from childhood to adulthood: a systematic review. Maturitas 70, 266284.Google Scholar
17. Lien, N, Lytle, LA & Klepp, KI (2001) Stability in consumption of fruit, vegetables, and sugary foods in a cohort from age 14 to age 21. Prev Med 33, 217226.Google Scholar
18. Totland, TH, Gebremariam, MK, Lien, N et al. (2013) Does tracking of dietary behaviours differ by parental education in children during the transition into adolescence? Public Health Nutr 16, 673682.Google Scholar
19. Rosenkranz, RR & Dzewaltowski, DA (2008) Model of the home food environment pertaining to childhood obesity. Nutr Rev 66, 123140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Blanchette, L & Brug, J (2005) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. J Hum Nutr Diet 18, 431443.Google Scholar
21. Cook, LT, O’Reilly, GA, DeRosa, CJ et al. (2015) Association between home availability and vegetable consumption in youth: a review. Public Health Nutr 18, 640648.Google Scholar
22. Krolner, R, Rasmussen, M, Brug, J et al. (2011) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part II: qualitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8, 112.Google Scholar
23. Pearson, N, Biddle, SJ & Gorely, T (2009) Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 12, 267283.Google Scholar
24. Rasmussen, M, Krolner, R, Klepp, KI et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: Quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 22.Google Scholar
25. van der Horst, K, Oenema, A, Ferreira, I et al. (2007) A systematic review of environmental correlates of obesity-related dietary behaviors in youth. Health Educ Res 22, 203226.Google Scholar
26. Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Owens, E et al. (2003) Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100% fruit juice, and vegetables influence children’s dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav 30, 615626.Google Scholar
27. Glasson, C, Chapman, K & James, E (2011) Fruit and vegetables should be targeted separately in health promotion programmes: differences in consumption levels, barriers, knowledge and stages of readiness for change. Public Health Nutr 14, 694701.Google Scholar
28. de Lauzon-Guillain, B, Oliveira, A, Charles, MA et al. (2012) A review of methods to assess parental feeding practices and preschool children’s eating behavior: the need for further development of tools. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 15781602.Google Scholar
29. Vaughn, AE, Tabak, RG, Bryant, MJ et al. (2013) Measuring parent food practices: a systematic review of existing measures and examination of instruments. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 61.Google Scholar
30. Vaughn, AE, Ward, DS, Fisher, JO et al. (2016) Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutr Rev 74, 98117.Google Scholar
31. Spurrier, NJ, Magarey, AA, Golley, R et al. (2008) Relationships between the home environment and physical activity and dietary patterns of preschool children: a cross-sectional study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 5, 31.Google Scholar
32. Baranowski, T, Beltran, A, Chen, TA et al. (2013) Psychometric assessment of scales for a Model of Goal Directed Vegetable Parenting Practices (MGDVPP). Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 10, 110.Google Scholar
33. Bjelland, M, Lien, N, Grydeland, M et al. (2011) Intakes and perceived home availability of sugar-sweetened beverages, fruit and vegetables as reported by mothers, fathers and adolescents in the HEIA (HEalth In Adolescents) study. Public Health Nutr 14, 21562165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34. O’Connor, TM, Hughes, SO, Watson, KB et al. (2010) Parenting practices are associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school children. Public Health Nutr 13, 91101.Google Scholar
35. Zeinstra, GG, Koelen, MA, Kok, FJ et al. (2010) Parental child-feeding strategies in relation to Dutch children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Public Health Nutr 13, 787796.Google Scholar
36. Norwegian Directorate of Heath (2012) Meals, Physical Activity and Environment Health Care in Kindergartens. Report no. IS-0345. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health (in Norwegian).Google Scholar
37. Norwegian Directorate of Health (2015) Weights, Measures and Portion Sizes for Foods. Report no. IS-2286. Oslo: Norwegian Directorate of Health.Google Scholar
38. Andersen, LF, Lioret, S, Brants, H et al. (2011) Recommendations for a trans-European dietary assessment method in children between 4 and 14 years. Eur J Clin Nutr 65, Suppl. 1, S58S64.Google Scholar
39. Baranowski, T, Sprague, D, Baranowski, JH et al. (1991) Accuracy of maternal dietary recall for preschool children. J Am Diet Assoc 91, 669674.Google Scholar
40. Kline, P (1994) An Easy Guide to Factor Analysis. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
41. Nunnally, JC & Bernstein, IH (1994) Psychometric Theory, 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
42. Field, A (2009) Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 3rd ed. London: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
43. Heck, R, Thomas, S & Tabata, L (2010) Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling with IBM SPSS. New York: Routledge Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
44. Wyse, R, Campbell, E, Nathan, N et al. (2011) Associations between characteristics of the home food environment and fruit and vegetable intake in preschool children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 11, 938.Google Scholar
45. McGowan, L, Croker, H, Wardle, J et al. (2012) Environmental and individual determinants of core and non-core food and drink intake in preschool-aged children in the United Kingdom. Eur J Clin Nutr 66, 322328.Google Scholar
46. Krebs-Smith, SM, Heimendinger, J, Subar, AF et al. (1995) Using food frequency questionnaires to estimate fruit and vegetable intake: association between the number of questions and total intakes. J Nutr Educ 27, 8085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47. Roark, RA & Niederhauser, VP (2013) Fruit and vegetable intake: issues with definition and measurement. Public Health Nutr 16, 27.Google Scholar
48. Shaikh, AR, Yaroch, AL, Nebeling, L et al. (2008) Psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption in adults a review of the literature. Am J Prev Med 34, 535543.Google Scholar
49. Campbell, KJ, Crawford, DA & Ball, K (2006) Family food environment and dietary behaviors likely to promote fatness in 5–6 year-old children. Int J Obes (Lond) 30, 12721280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50. Foster, E & Adamson, A (2014) Challenges involved in measuring intake in early life: focus on methods. Proc Nutr Soc 73, 201209.Google Scholar
51. Statistics Norway (2016) Population’s level of education, 1 October 2015. https://ssb.no/en/utdanning/statistikker/utniv/aar/2016-06-20?fane=tabell&sort=nummer&tabell=270244 (accessed July 2016).Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flowchart showing recruitment of and data collection from invited and participating 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study, Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway, in total and according to methods used in relation to the home environment at baseline

Figure 1

Table 1 Characteristics of 3–5-year-old children and their parents in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

Figure 2

Table 2 Items and factor loadings* for the factors derived from the principal component analysis reported by parents of 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

Figure 3

Table 3 Mean value, standard deviation, corrected item–total correlation (CITC) and Cronbach’s alpha (α) for the factors derived from the principal component analysis reported by parents of 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study (n 439), Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway

Figure 4

Table 4 Adjusted bivariate associations* between physical home environmental factors and vegetable consumption among 3–5-year-old children in the BRA-study, Vestfold and Buskerud counties, Norway