Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T17:12:12.019Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developing sugar-sweetened beverage warning labels for young adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2021

Jennifer Falbe*
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Studies Program, Department of Human Ecology, UC Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
Astrid Montuclard
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Studies Program, Department of Human Ecology, UC Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
Alina Engelman
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, California State University East Bay, Hayward, CA, USA
Sabrina Adler
Affiliation:
ChangeLab Solutions, Oakland, CA, USA
Athena Roesler
Affiliation:
Human Development and Family Studies Program, Department of Human Ecology, UC Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

There is a lack of qualitative research developing sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) warning labels with their intended end users. We sought to identify promising SSB warning elements for improving label effectiveness and for future testing in policy and institutional settings.

Design:

Mixed methods design using ten focus groups, a design task and a survey. The design task was used to generate ideas for an icon that would dissuade SSB consumption. The survey and focus group guide assessed participant perceptions of SSB warning label mock-ups of text (loss frame, gain frame and loss frame with attribution), colour and icon options.

Setting:

Three large public universities in California from February to March 2018.

Participants:

Young adult SSB consumers (n 86) enrolled in one of three diverse California public universities.

Results:

Participants perceived the following elements as most effective for reducing SSB consumption: loss-frame text with attribution to a credible source, yellow and red colour for label background and an image or icon to accompany the text. Preferred images included sugar near or inside of an SSB, intuitive shapes like a triangle with exclamation mark or octagon and a visual indicator of SSB sugar content compared with recommended limits. Support was high for using SSB warning labels in university cafeterias and on bottles/cans.

Conclusions:

Loss-frame text with a credible source, yellow or red label colour and icons could potentially enhance effectiveness of SSB warning labels and warrant further testing.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Reducing sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) consumption is a global priority for obesity prevention(1,2) . Based on tobacco control successes, excise taxes(2) and warning labels(Reference Falbe and Madsen3,Reference Grummon, Hall and Block4) have been identified as transferrable policies for SSB reduction. Evaluations of SSB taxes demonstrate reductions in SSB consumption, sales and/or purchasing(Reference Falbe5Reference Roberto, Lawman and LeVasseur7). Because SSB warning labels have not been widely implemented, there is comparatively less evidence of effectiveness. Online experiments(Reference Roberto, Wong and Musicus8Reference Mantzari, Vasiljevic and Turney11) and a small number of laboratory(Reference Grummon, Taillie and Golden12) and field studies(Reference Donnelly, Zatz and Svirsky13,Reference Leung, Wolfson and Hsu14) indicate that warning labels reduce SSB purchasing or selection(Reference Grummon and Hall15). Furthermore, a Chilean law that mandates front-of-package warning labels on products high in added sugars, among other measures, reduced SSB purchasing(Reference Taillie, Reyes and Colchero16).

Although US SSB labelling laws have not yet been implemented, interest is growing. Nine US jurisdictions have introduced twenty-eight SSB labelling bills(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha17). Polling data show high bipartisan support for SSB warning labels(Reference Grummon, Hall and Block4), indicating political feasibility despite potential legal challenges(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha17). Furthermore, institutions like workplaces and universities could voluntarily place SSB warning labels at point-of-purchase. Higher education may be a particularly impactful venue for SSB warning labels since 41 % of young adults enrol in college(Reference Hussar, Zhang and Hein18), young adults consume high amounts of SSB(Reference Han and Powell19), and emerging adulthood is a developmental period important for establishing lasting dietary habits(Reference Nelson, Story and Larson20). There is currently an opportunity to identify warning designs most likely to be effective before such policies are implemented more widely, either by law or by institutions.

Lessons from warning label development for other products suggest enhanced efficacy with images and symbols(Reference Hammond21Reference Wogalter, Conzola and Smith-Jackson23), bright and contrasting colours(Reference Wogalter, Conzola and Smith-Jackson23,Reference Lempert and Glantz24) and attribution to trusted sources(Reference Guttman and Peleg25). Evidence is more mixed regarding loss- v. gain-frame messaging(Reference Hammond21). Despite the potential for these elements to improve SSB warning labels, there is a dearth of formative qualitative or mixed methods research developing SSB warning labels with their intended end users. The importance of such studies is underscored by a qualitative study that was used to develop Chile’s ‘high-in’ nutrient warning label(Reference Reyes, Garmendia and Olivares26). Research on the design of SSB warning labels has consisted of quantitative studies testing a limited number of design variations(Reference Roberto, Wong and Musicus8,Reference VanEpps and Roberto9,Reference Acton, Vanderlee and Roberto27,Reference Grummon, Hall and Taillie28) . Although prior quantitative research has been instrumental in designing SSB warning labels, the lack of formative research exploring a wide range of designs with end users means that important design variations could be overlooked.

We conducted a mixed methods study using focus groups and a survey of a diverse sample of young adults enrolled in public universities to identify promising SSB warning elements for improving label effectiveness and for future testing. The warning label elements assessed included variations on message framing, attribution to a source, icons and images, and marker words. We examined designs that could be used in policy and institutional settings and assessed support for using SSB warnings in college settings.

Methods

Participants and recruitment

We conducted ten focus groups from February to March 2018 with eighty-six participants enrolled in one of three California public universities. The universities were racially/ethnically and socio-economically diverse, reflected in 2018 total undergraduate enrolment at universities 1, 2 and 3: 34 %, 42 % and 26 % non-Hispanic Asian/Pacific Islander; 28 %, 18 % and 39 % Hispanic and 3 %, 2 % and 11 % non-Hispanic Black, respectively, with 19–54 % receiving Federal Pell grants(29). Pell grants are an indicator of low socio-economic status because they are awarded to students of ‘exceptional financial need(30)’.

Participants were recruited through paper and electronic fliers and class announcements across multiple disciplines, inviting participants to share ‘opinions on beverages and health messages.’ Eligibility was assessed by phone and included: undergraduate, age ≥ 18 years and regular SSB consumption (≥1 time/d). Participants received a $25 incentive.

Following pragmatic recommendations on sample size for focus groups(Reference Carlsen and Glenton31), we aimed to conduct at least three focus groups per university with 8–12 participants per focus group to adequately capture the range of perspectives across sites. Because the number of participants who could attend the first two focus groups at University 2 (n 5 and 7) was lower than planned, a fourth focus group was conducted at this site. Data saturation from focus groups was monitored and used as the ultimate criterion for ending new enrolment of participants. Figure 1 shows participant flow. Focus group sizes ranged from 5 to 13 participants (mean: 8·6 ± 2·4). All procedures were approved by the universities’ institutional review boards.

Fig. 1 Participant flow

Procedures

Before focus group discussions, participants completed independent tasks – a background questionnaire, design task and warning label questionnaire, all completed on paper.

Design task

The moderator asked, ‘What if you were tasked to design an icon or graphic that would be posted on or near sugary beverages in college cafeterias.…to get people to drink fewer sugary beverages?’ Warnings were not referenced. Participants were provided paper, markers and 10 min to independently generate ideas.

Warning label questionnaire

The questionnaire assessed ratings and rankings of warning label mock-ups. Participants viewed images of where labels could be placed – near SSB dispensers and on bottles/cans (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Figure 1). Participants viewed and rated print-outs of five randomly ordered mock-ups of rectangular warning labels (Fig. 2), including three text variations in black-and-white: loss frame, gain frame and loss frame with attribution to the ‘Health and Wellness Center’ – generic for the student health centre. Mock-ups also included two background colour variations: yellow and orange. The loss-frame text was based on SSB warnings shown to be efficacious(Reference Roberto, Wong and Musicus8,Reference VanEpps and Roberto9) : ‘Drinking beverages with added sugar(s) contributes to obesity, type 2 diabetes, and tooth decay.’ We developed a gain-frame message in consultation with four prominent SSB researchers: ‘Drinking fewer beverages with added sugar(s) helps prevent obesity, type 2 diabetes, and tooth decay.’

Fig. 2 Five initial warning label mock-ups and additional colour variations rated and discussed by focus group participants. Note: The first five labels were shown to participants on a single page against a white background in random order. Participants were also shown an image of where labels would be placed (near blue and multi-coloured fountain drink dispensers and on red, yellow and green cans or bottles). Additional colours were displayed against a blue dispenser only. **Colours ranked as most effective (P < 0·01)

Second, as a result of feedback from the first focus group, we presented additional colour variations for all subsequent focus groups to assess: red and additional shades of orange and yellow (Fig. 2).

Third, participants provided rankings of icons/images that could appear on warnings (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Table 1). We used an evolving set of images based on design tasks and discussions, where new images were introduced and unpopular ones removed. We developed an initial set of nine image concepts based on existing warnings (e.g. Chile’s ‘high in’ octagon, triangle with exclamation mark), public health campaigns (e.g. sugar cubes) and health consequences (e.g. tooth decay). Because we sought to design a label that, in addition to being policy relevant, could be acceptable for voluntary placement by institutions, we did not examine images intended to elicit disgust or that could stigmatise obesity(Reference Hayward and Vartanian32).

Focus group

Discussion questions (online appendix) assessed perceptions and recommendations for text, colours, attribution, icons/images and marker word. We audio-recorded and transcribed discussions.

The warning mock-ups, questionnaires and focus group guide were informed both by prior empirical research on tobacco and SSB warning labels as well by parts of the Message Impact Framework(Reference Noar, Hall and Francis33) used to explain how tobacco warning labels might impact consumption (e.g. attention, credibility, perceived effectiveness, knowledge and intention).

Measures

Background questionnaire

Questions assessed age, gender, race/ethnicity, height, weight, year in college, first generation status, food security(34), Pell grant status, parental income, frequency of eating on campus and beverage consumption(Reference Hedrick, Savla and Comber35).

Warning label questionnaire

Items on perceived message effectiveness (PME) were adapted from a protocol by Hammond and Reid to pre-test tobacco warning labels(Reference Hammond and Reid36). Prior research has found that PME is predictive of message effectiveness and subsequent behavioural response, making PME a useful outcome in formative research(Reference Noar, Barker and Bell37). PME ratings of the initial five mock-ups were assessed for text (loss frame, gain frame, loss + attribution) and colour (white, yellow, orange), separately, with: ‘Think about the [text/color] of this message. Please rate the effectiveness of the [text/color] by circling one number on the scale below’ (ten-point scale: LEAST effective – 1 to MOST effective – 10). Open-ended questions asked what participants liked/disliked about each label.

PME rankings of warning text were assessed with the item, ‘Please put the warning messages in order from most to least effective overall, in your opinion.’ Rankings of initial colours (white, yellow or orange) were assessed with: ‘Consider the color of all messages. Which one is most effective?’ Rankings of the additional colours (red and other shades of yellow/orange) and icons/image mock-ups were assessed with the items: ‘Choose the three [colors/images/icons] you think would be most effective. Rank them below.’ The questionnaire also assessed which icons were perceived as least effective. Finally, the questionnaire asked, ‘Would you prefer a text-only message or a message that includes a picture or icon?’ For rankings, we created a categorical variable for the highest ranked variation.

The questionnaires assessed support for SSB warnings in college dining commons and on bottles/cans with: ‘Do you support the use of sugary drink warning’… ‘messages in front of sugary drink dispensers in college dining commons?’ and ‘labels on bottles/cans of sugary drinks?’ (ten-point scale: not at all – 1 to extremely support – 10). Last, the questionnaire assessed SSB intention(Reference Zoellner, Estabrooks and Davy38) and perceived knowledge gain: ‘As a result of seeing these warnings, I plan to reduce my intake of sugary drinks’ (seven-point scale: strongly disagree to strongly agree), which was dichotomised as agree v. neutral/disagree; ‘Did you learn anything new after seeing these warnings’ (yes/no); and ‘What did you learn?’.

Analysis

We used quantitative analysis to determine the extent to which a particular variation of a label element was perceived as more effective than others. We used qualitative analysis to elaborate on the participants’ rationale for quantitative ratings and rankings, which could help reveal mechanisms of potential effectiveness. Qualitative analysis was also used to generate additional ideas for quantitative testing (e.g. additional colours and icons/images) and to corroborate quantitative findings.

Quantitative

We examined within-subjects differences in mock-up ratings using Friedman and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. χ 2 goodness-of-fit tests were used to examine statistical significance of top-ranked variation, and an exact binomial test was used to determine if the proportion preferring a pictorial warnings exceeded 50 %. All statistical tests were two-sided (α = 0·05) and were conducted using Stata15MP (College Station, TX). Because an evolving set of icons/images were used, we present only percentages for rankings. Also, the sample size was based on considerations for the qualitative component of this study and not on a formal power calculation for rating and ranking outcomes, so some analyses may be underpowered.

Qualitative

We developed a codebook with structural codes based on question theme and used NVivo11 (QSRInternational, Melbourne) to double-code transcripts. We developed a codebook for classifying each drawing/design idea by topic (e.g. health consequences, warning icon). Disagreements were resolved by the first and second authors. Data saturation was examined throughout the course of the study and was defined as there being no new topics depicted in the design tasks and no new perspectives during focus group discussions. Because data saturation was reached prior to the final focus group, no additional focus groups were conducted beyond the initial ten.

Results

Most participants (76 %) were Hispanic or non-white, and 59 % were female (Table 1). Almost half reported low food security, 31 % were first generation students and 31 % were Pell grant recipients. The racial/ethnic and Pell grant distribution of participants in our sample were within the range of the lowest and highest proportions across the three universities. Participants consumed a 2·5 ± 2·1 cans, bottles or glasses of SSB per day.

Table 1 Characteristics of eighty-six focus group participants

NH, non-Hispanic; SSB, sugar-sweetened beverage.

Observations with missing data were excluded from calculations of means, sd, and percentages.

* Neither parent completed 4-year college degree.

Design task

The most common topics in participant designs were health consequences of SSB (e.g. obesity, oral health, amputation, death, tombstone) – 49 %; SSB sugar content (e.g. soda can near sugar pile, sugar content warning) – 47 %; warning icons/symbols/labels – 24 % and water promotion (e.g. health and aesthetic benefits of drinking water) – 17 %. Other topics appearing repeatedly but less frequently were emojis, slogans (e.g. ‘save calories for food’), addictiveness, portrayals of SSB as chemicals or toxic, short-term effects of SSB (e.g. mood) and physical activity equivalents of SSB energies (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Figure 2). The mock-ups of icons/images we developed to elicit feedback contained examples of participants’ most frequently depicted topics (except obesity, graphic/morbid images and water promotion) and some less frequently depicted topics: emojis, portrayals of SSB as chemicals and slogans.

Warning questionnaire and focus group discussions

Quantitative results below for each theme are followed by quantitative findings that explain the rationale behind participants’ ratings and rankings of PME. Table 2 and Supplemental Table 1 contain additional quotes.

Table 2 Exemplary quotes from focus group discussions and free-response questionnaire items evaluating sugar-sweetened beverage warning label designs

Bold font indicates sentiments voiced with greater frequency.

Text

When participants rated PME of mock-ups one-by-one (on a ten-point scale from least – 1 to most effective – 10), PME ratings differed significantly by mock-up text (gain = 5·3, loss = 5·6 and loss + attribution = 6·2; P < 0·01). Loss + attribution was rated significantly more effective than loss frame (P = 0·01) and gain frame (P < 0·01), but there was no significant difference between loss- and gain-frame ratings (P = 0·14). When ranking all mock-ups side-by-side, participants ranked loss + attribution as most effective (52 %; P < 0·001), followed by a similar proportion for gain (27 %) and loss frame (20 %). However, gain frame was ranked least effective (49 %; P < 0·01) compared with loss frame (35 %) and loss + attribution (16 %).

In focus group discussions, the most common reason favouring attribution was credibility. However, many felt that the attribution reduced readability by adding text and was unnecessary. Regarding source, although some favoured the university health and wellness centre (e.g. ‘who doesn’t like them?’; ‘it makes me feel like [my university] is looking out for me’), many felt it lacked authority: ‘It’s not like the American Heart Association or a surgeon’s warning. It doesn’t have authority.’ The American Heart Association and FDA were mentioned most frequently as preferred sources, followed by research universities.

Participants who favoured the loss-frame text preferred its brevity, clarity and directness: ‘Concise, easy for most people to understand.’ Others perceived the loss-frame text as more effective because it sounded ‘stronger’ and more ‘concerning’ than gain frame: ‘If I see a warning, I really want it to warn me…instead of being soft…’

Participants favouring gain frame noted its positivity: ‘It uses a more proactive/positive angle.’ Others thought the gain-frame text may increase self-efficacy: ‘[Drinking] “fewer” makes it seem…doable.’ Critiques were that it was ineffective, confusing and did not quantify ‘fewer’: ‘How much is fewer? Is it two sips less?; it’s enabling.’ Others noted the contradiction of gain-frame text for a warning: ‘…that wasn’t really a warning.’

Colour

PME ratings of the three initial background colours differed (yellow = 7·1, orange = 6·3 and white = 4·1; P < 0·001). Participants perceived yellow (P < 0·001) and orange (P < 0·001) as more effective than white, and yellow as more effective than orange (P < 0·001). When participants were asked to rank a larger selection of twelve colours, including red, the highest proportion of participants ranked the following as most effective (Fig. 2): yellow with black font (16 %; P < 0·01) and red with white font (16 %; P < 0·01).

Participants perceived yellow as ‘attention-grabbing’ and contrasting with black font. Participants likened yellow to traffic lights and signs. Another said, ‘…when I see…a highlighter color, I will want to read it.’ Those favouring red with white font noted that red signified ‘danger’ and reminded them of traffic signals: ‘it made me stop and read it.’ However, several participants noted the poorer contrast of red and white font. Orange was also perceived as eye-catching and reminded participants of ‘construction zones’ and ‘chemistry’ warnings. Although black-and-white was perceived as ‘official’ and ‘readable,’ many thought it failed to grab attention. Participants emphasised the importance of the label colour contrasting with location placed.

Icon/image

Most (81 %; P < 0·001) preferred a warning with a picture/icon over a text-only warning. Icon/image ideas ranked as most effective by >10 % of participants were (Fig. 3): sugar next to/inside an SSB can, bottle or cup (38 %), a triangle-exclamation mark (15 %) and an octagon with a slogan, ‘rethink your drink’ or nutrient warning ‘high in sugar’ (14 %), and depiction of sugar content of SSB relative to daily limits (11 %).

Fig. 3 Top ranking icon or image concepts to accompany a sugar-sweetened beverage warning label

Note: Supplemental Figure 1 shows all icon and image concepts ranked by participants.

Reasons for preferring images of sugar were that they convey the high sugar content of SSB and reduced product appeal (e.g. ‘when I think of Coke or a lemonade, I think…that’s refreshing, not a liquid form of [sugar] cubes; that’s kind of disgusting’). However, some participants thought the sugar cubes were ice, underscoring the need for unambiguous icons.

Those in favour of the triangle-exclamation mark noted that it universally symbolised a warning, drew attention and motivated them to read the text. Those in favour of octagons noted they elicited attention, were catchy (‘rethink your drink’) and served as a concise reminder of the label’s message. However, a participant noted that if an icon also contains text, it creates ‘two information sources battling each other’ making it ‘hard to read.’

Icons depicting SSB sugar content v. daily limit were controversial. Although they can correct misperceptions about sugar content (e.g. ‘people probably think that amount of sugar is normal, but it shows you it isn’t’), others found them harder to quickly process and understand.

Icons perceived as least effective were emojis (34 %) and health symbols (e.g. heart; 14 %). Although emojis had proponents, others perceived them as removing ‘the weight of the problem.’ Participants noted that health symbols may send an unintended message, ‘like it was a doctor-approved soda.’ Last, some participants said certain image concepts would be effective on a billboard (e.g. sugar packets) but would be hard to comprehend on a small label.

Marker word

Participants voiced their preference for the marker word, ‘warning,’ to be in all-caps, bold or larger font. Regarding other marker words, participants generally preferred ‘warning’ but noted that ‘caution’ or ‘attention’ fit better with gain-frame text.

Support

Support was high for using warnings in front of sugary drink dispensers in college dining commons and on bottles/cans of sugary drinks: 8·1 ± 1·7 and 8·1 ± 2·0, respectively, on a ten-point scale (not at all – 1 to extremely support – 10). The proportion selecting a 6–10 on that scale was 92 % for the ‘dining commons’ and 87 % for ‘bottles/cans.’ Reasons for supporting warnings in dining commons were perceived effectiveness and to: increase knowledge about the health consequences of SSB, ‘remind’ those who already have this knowledge, increase risk perception and promote informed choice (e.g. ‘this is a low-cost way to promote health without infringing on students’ liberty…’). Those unsupportive expressed scepticism about effectiveness (e.g. ‘…college students would mostly ignore…the warnings….there is a low risk perception’).

Perceived knowledge and intention

Seventeen percentage reported learning something new from the warnings (e.g. specific disease consequences of SSB and that some ‘juice drinks’ contained added sugar), and 69 % reported intending to reduce SSB intake after seeing the warnings.

Discussion

This formative mixed methods study found that young adults enrolled in one of three public universities had a strong preference for warning labels in attention-grabbing colours, namely yellow and red, that evoke symbols of caution. Participants emphasised the importance of the warning colour contrasting with where it is placed and the need for enough contrast with font colour for readability. Of the text options, participants perceived the loss-frame text with attribution to a source as most effective, but some thought a source added unnecessary length. Although loss- and gain-frame text elicited similar rankings as ‘most effective,’ gain frame was also ranked ‘least effective,’ indicating ambivalence about gain-frame text. Reasons for preferring loss-frame text included brevity, clarity and directness. Consistent with a loss-frame text preference, participant-generated designs for images/icons were more likely to convey health consequences (disease, mortality) or nutrition consequences (excess sugar) of SSB than benefits of not consuming SSB or consuming water instead of SSB. Most participants preferred a warning that included an icon/image. Of the icon/image mock-ups, the following were perceived as the most effective: depiction of sugar near/inside SSB, a triangle-exclamation mark and octagon, and the sugar content of SSB relative to daily limits. Participants articulated a preference for the marker word to standout (e.g. caps). Most participants reported intending to reduce SSB consumption as a result of seeing the warnings in the study. Last, participants indicated strong support for SSB warning labels in college dining commons and on SSB cans/bottles.

All warning designs tested in this study could be used in voluntary warning labels or in labels placed by institutions themselves. However, not all may be legally viable as part of government policies that require businesses to label products. The more factual and less controversial the warning content, the more legally viable it will be(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha17,39) . Regarding images tested in our study, a triangle icon (proposed in California SB347) may be less risky than others as it is commonly understood to signify a warning and has been implemented in voluntary and mandatory(Reference Anekwe, Lent and Farley40,41) warning labels for other products. Also, an image that depicts an amount of sugar not exceeding that in the product is more factual and therefore may be less risky than an image exaggerating sugar content. Likewise, for textual statements, the more factual it is (e.g. ‘high in added sugar’), the more legally viable the label may be. Conversely, text or icons that convey an opinion (e.g. ‘Rethink your drink’) or that may be misleading or not factual (e.g. cup filled entirely with sugar) are unlikely to survive a legal challenge if required by government. The loss-frame text of mock-ups in this study has been written into bills and is similar to that in an enacted San Francisco ordinance that requires warning labels on ads for SSB (the difference being that the ordinance did not specify ‘type 2’ diabetes). A 2019 court decision blocked the implementation of San Francisco’s ordinance because of the size of the required warning and also suggested, without ruling definitively, that words like ‘may’ and ‘type 2’ diabetes could be needed in the text to make it factually accurate(Reference Pomeranz, Mozaffarian and Micha17). However, including ‘can’ or ‘may’ could attenuate label effectiveness(Reference Hall, Grummon and Maynard42). San Francisco has since enacted a revised warning label ordinance, which has also been the subject of litigation, that requires the following text: ‘…Drinking beverages with added sugar(s) can cause weight gain, which increases the risk of obesity and type 2 diabetes’(43).

Similar to our findings for colour, Reyes et al. (Reference Reyes, Garmendia and Olivares26) used focus groups to develop Chile’s nutrient warnings and found that red, yellow and orange were preferred over black-and-white. In a quantitative study, Grummon et al. (Reference Grummon, Hall and Taillie28) found that although most participants perceived red as most effective, few perceived yellow as most effective, perhaps because their study’s yellow label lacked a black border, causing low contrast against light backgrounds. Prior research has shown consumer preference for a border(Reference Acton, Vanderlee and Roberto27). Similar to our finding, Lempert et al.’s analysis(Reference Lempert and Glantz24) of tobacco industry research found that ‘yellow most quickly and effectively seizes and holds consumers’ attention and signals warning…’ Acton et al. (Reference Acton, Vanderlee and Roberto27) found that yellow was rated as more noticeable than white but less readable. When tested against control conditions (e.g. no label), online experiments have found that black-and-white SSB warnings were efficacious(Reference Roberto, Wong and Musicus8,Reference Grummon, Taillie and Golden12,Reference Acton, Jones and Kirkpatrick44) , whereas real-world experiments have only tested labels in colour, finding that a red octagon(Reference Grummon, Taillie and Golden12) and yellow warning with a triangle-exclamation icon(Reference Leung, Wolfson and Hsu14) reduced SSB purchase or consumption. For other products, not all studies consistently showed greater efficacy for red and yellow v. black/white. For instance, Musicus et al. (Reference Musicus, Moran and Lawman45) found no significant difference between red and black sodium warnings.

There is a lack of studies testing gain- v. loss-frame text for SSB warning labels, and to our knowledge, experiments of SSB warnings have used loss frame(Reference Grummon and Hall15). However, a qualitative study eliciting opinions about a variety of SSB interventions (e.g. warning labels) among parents and providers of infants and pregnant women found that although gain-frame messages were well-received, loss-frame messages (e.g. risk of diabetes, heart disease) drew more attention, elicited stronger emotional responses and were perceived as particularly effective among parents for discouraging SSB consumption(Reference Morel, Nichols and Nong46). Also, an online experiment among Brazilian adults tested a loss-frame labelling scheme for unhealthy products against a gain-frame labelling scheme for healthy products; the authors found that the loss-frame scheme was more efficacious(Reference Alcantara, Ares and de Castro47). Similarly, tobacco literature suggests that although participants may voice preference for gain frame in some studies, loss-frame warnings are typically more effective(Reference Hammond21,Reference Bansal-Travers, Hammond and Smith22) . However, the literature on health behaviours more broadly has demonstrated between-person variability in effectiveness of loss- and gain-frame messages(Reference Wansink and Pope48). Thus, using multiple variations of a warning by frame and other characteristics (e.g. colour) simultaneously or on a rotating basis may be effective in a larger proportion of the population than using a single warning variation.

Similar to our findings that participants preferred intuitive icons, online studies indicated that triangle-exclamation mark, octagon and circle-exclamation mark were perceived as more effective than text-only labels(Reference Reyes, Garmendia and Olivares26Reference Grummon, Hall and Taillie28), and two real-world experiments using such icons on labels reduced SSB purchase(Reference Grummon, Taillie and Golden12) or consumption(Reference Leung, Wolfson and Hsu14).

Support for SSB warning labels on cans/bottles of SSB was high in this study, similar to prior findings(Reference Grummon, Hall and Block4). However, our study also finds high support among college students for SSB warning labels in dining commons.

Limitations of this study include not eliciting feedback on gruesome/morbid imagery or depictions of obesity and the potential limited generalisability of results to other regions or age groups. However, young adults are among the highest SSB consumers(Reference Han and Powell19), making it critical to design effective warnings for this group. Further, because our sample consisted of SSB consumers who were disproportionately food insecure, a population at increased chronic disease risk(Reference Malik, Popkin and Bray49,Reference Leung and Zhou50) , our sample was well-suited for testing SSB warnings. Also, although we tested a comprehensive set of images/icons, the execution of mock-ups may have influenced perceptions. Since this study was designed, a validated brief scale of PME was developed and published(Reference Baig, Noar and Gottfredson51). Scales such as these should be used in future formative studies. We assessed only self-reported perceptions of labels and not actual effectiveness, so further quantitative testing with objective outcomes is needed to definitively identify the most effective design variations. Future testing of warning label variations would benefit from measures and procedures that not only test efficacy but also provide further evidence on the mechanisms(Reference Grummon and Brewer52,Reference Taillie, Hall and Popkin53) through which labels impact consumption. Last, our sample size may have been underpowered to detect some quantitative differences in perception of label design variations.

Interest in SSB warning label legislation is growing, but there has yet to be such a law implemented in the USA. To maximise public health impacts of such laws, it is important to thoroughly consider design elements that could enhance label effectiveness. This study identified promising SSB warning label variations for further testing and revealed reasons young adult SSB consumers preferred these variations. It will be important to quantitatively test the top designs against one another as existing research has already begun to do. These results could also be used to design SSB warning labels in institutions like hospitals, workplaces and universities. The feasibility of voluntary SSB warning labels is underscored by participant’s strong support for SSB warning labels in dining commons.

Acknowledgements

Financial support: This research was supported by The National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases grant K01DK113068. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH. The sponsors had no role in the design or conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis or interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review or approval of the manuscript. Conflict of interest: None for all authors. Authorship: J.F. conceptualised and designed the study, analysed the data and drafted the manuscript. A.M. contributed to the design, collected data, analysed data, and contributed to writing the manuscript. A.E., S.A. and A.R. contributed to the design and critically revised the manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving research participants were approved by the UC Davis Institutional Review Board. Verbal informed consent was obtained from all subjects. Verbal consent was witnessed and formally recorded.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021002287

References

Centers for disease Control and Prevention (CDC) (2010) The CDC Guide to Strategies for Reducing the Consumption of Sugar-Sweetened Beverages. https://stacks.cdc.gov/view/cdc/51532 (accessed August 2020).Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2017) Taxes on sugary drinks: why do it? World Health Organization. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/260253 (accessed August 2020).Google Scholar
Falbe, J & Madsen, K (2017) Growing momentum for sugar-sweetened beverage campaigns and policies: costs and considerations. Am J Public Health 107, 835838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grummon, AH, Hall, MG, Block, JP et al. (2020) Ethical considerations for food and beverage warnings. Physiol Behav 222, 112930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falbe, J (2020) The ethics of excise taxes on sugar-sweetened beverages. Physiol Behav 225, 113105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, MM, Falbe, J, Schillinger, D et al. (2019) Sugar-sweetened beverage consumption 3 years after the Berkeley, California, sugar-sweetened beverage tax. Am J Public Health 109, 637639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberto, CA, Lawman, HG, LeVasseur, MT et al. (2019) Association of a beverage tax on sugar-sweetened and artificially sweetened beverages with changes in beverage prices and sales at chain retailers in a large urban setting. JAMA 321, 17991810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberto, CA, Wong, D, Musicus, A et al. (2016) The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage health warning labels on parents’ choices. Pediatrics 137, e20153185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanEpps, EM & Roberto, CA (2016) The influence of sugar-sweetened beverage warnings: a randomized trial of adolescents’ choices and beliefs. Am J Prev Med 51, 664672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billich, N, Blake, MR, Backholer, K et al. (2018) The effect of sugar-sweetened beverage front-of-pack labels on drink selection, health knowledge and awareness: an online randomised controlled trial. Appetite 128, 233241 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mantzari, E, Vasiljevic, M, Turney, I et al. (2018) Impact of warning labels on sugar-sweetened beverages on parental selection: an online experimental study. Prev Med Rep 12, 259267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grummon, AH, Taillie, LS, Golden, SD et al. (2019) Sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings and purchases: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Prev Med 57, 601610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donnelly, GE, Zatz, LY, Svirsky, D et al. (2018) The effect of graphic warnings on sugary-drink purchasing. Psychol Sci 29, 13211333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, CW, Wolfson, JA, Hsu, R et al. (2021) Warning labels reduce sugar-sweetened beverage intake among college students. J Nutr 151, 179185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grummon, AH & Hall, MG (2020) Sugary drink warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. PLoS Med 17, e1003120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taillie, LS, Reyes, M, Colchero, MA et al. (2020) An evaluation of Chile’s Law of Food Labeling and Advertising on sugar-sweetened beverage purchases from 2015 to 2017: a before-and-after study. PLoS Med 17, e1003015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pomeranz, JL, Mozaffarian, D & Micha, R (2020) Sugar-sweetened beverage warning policies in the broader legal context: health and safety warning laws and the first amendment. Am J Prev Med 58, 783788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hussar, B, Zhang, J, Hein, S et al. (2020) The Condition of Education 2020. NCES 2020–144. National Center for Education Statistics. https://nces.ed.gov/pubs2020/2020144.pdf (accessed January 2021).Google Scholar
Han, E & Powell, LM (2013) Consumption patterns of sugar-sweetened beverages in the United States. J Acad Nutr Diet 113, 4353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, MC, Story, M, Larson, NI et al. (2008) Emerging adulthood and college-aged youth: an overlooked age for weight-related behavior change. Obesity 16, 22052211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, D (2011) Health warning messages on tobacco products: a review. Tob Control 20, 327337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bansal-Travers, M, Hammond, D, Smith, P et al. (2011) The impact of cigarette pack design, descriptors, and warning labels on risk perception in the U.S. Am J Prev Med 40, 674682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wogalter, MS, Conzola, VC & Smith-Jackson, TL (2002) Research-based guidelines for warning design and evaluation. Appl Ergon 33, 219230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lempert, LK & Glantz, SA (2016) Implications of tobacco industry research on packaging colors for designing health warning labels. Nicotine Tob Res 18, 19101914.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guttman, N & Peleg, H (2003) Public preferences for an attribution to government or to medical research versus unattributed messages in cigarette warning labels in Israel. Health Commun 15, 125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyes, M, Garmendia, ML, Olivares, S et al. (2019) Development of the Chilean front-of-package food warning label. BMC Public Health 19, 906.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acton, RB, Vanderlee, L, Roberto, CA et al. (2018) Consumer perceptions of specific design characteristics for front-of-package nutrition labels. Health Educ Res 33, 167174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grummon, AH, Hall, MG, Taillie, LS et al. (2019) How should sugar-sweetened beverage health warnings be designed? A randomized experiment. Prev Med 121, 5866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) (2018) Integrated Postsecondary Education Data System (IPEDS). https://nces.ed.gov/ipeds/use-the-data (accessed June 2019).Google Scholar
Federal Student Aid of the U.S. Department of Education (2020) Federal Pell Grants are usually awarded only to undergraduate students. https://studentaid.gov/understand-aid/types/grants/pell (accessed March 2021).Google Scholar
Carlsen, B & Glenton, C (2011) What about N? A methodological study of sample-size reporting in focus group studies. BMC Med Res Methodol 11, 26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayward, LE & Vartanian, LR (2019) Potential unintended consequences of graphic warning labels on sugary drinks: do they promote obesity stigma? Obes Sci Pract 5, 333341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noar, SM, Hall, MG, Francis, DB et al. (2016) Pictorial cigarette pack warnings: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Tob Control 25, 341354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Economic Research Service, USDA (2012) U.S. Household Food Security Survey Module: Six-Item Short Form. https://www.ers.usda.gov/media/8282/short2012.pdf (accessed February 2017).Google Scholar
Hedrick, VE, Savla, J, Comber, DL et al. (2012) Development of a brief questionnaire to assess habitual beverage intake (BEVQ-15): sugar-sweetened beverages and total beverage energy intake. J Acad Nutr Diet 112, 840849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, D & Reid, J (2011) Pre-testing and evaluating warning messages for tobacco products. http://www.tobaccolabels.ca/wp/wp-content/uploads/2013/11/Pre-testing-and-Evaluating-Warning-Messages-for-Tobacco-Products-Guide-Hammond-and-Reid-2011.pdf (accessed September 2017).Google Scholar
Noar, SM, Barker, J, Bell, T et al. (2020) Does perceived message effectiveness predict the actual effectiveness of tobacco education messages? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Commun 35, 148157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zoellner, J, Estabrooks, PA, Davy, BM et al. (2012) Exploring the theory of planned behavior to explain sugar-sweetened beverage consumption. J Nutr Educ Behav 44, 172177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
ChangeLab Solutions (2019) Sugary Drink Warning Labels: A Legal Update. https://www.changelabsolutions.org/product/sugary-drink-warning-labels (accessed April 2020).Google Scholar
Anekwe, AV, Lent, M, Farley, SM et al. (2019) New York City’s sodium warning regulation: from conception to enforcement. Am J Public Health 109, 11911192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
California Office of Environmental Health Hazard Assessent (2021) Proposition 65 Warnings Website. https://www.p65warnings.ca.gov/ (accessed January 2021).Google Scholar
Hall, MG, Grummon, AH, Maynard, OM et al. (2019) Causal language in health warning labels and US Adults’ perception: a randomized experiment. Am J Public Health 109, 14291433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Center for Science in the Public Interest (2021) San Francisco Sugary Drink Warning. https://www.cspinet.org/protecting-our-health/courts (accessed March 2021).Google Scholar
Acton, RB, Jones, AC, Kirkpatrick, SI et al. (2019) Taxes and front-of-package labels improve the healthiness of beverage and snack purchases: a randomized experimental marketplace. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 16, 46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musicus, AA, Moran, AJ, Lawman, HG et al. (2019) Online randomized controlled trials of restaurant sodium warning labels. Am J Prev Med 57, e181e93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morel, K, Nichols, K, Nong, Y et al. (2019) Parental and provider perceptions of sugar-sweetened beverage interventions in the first 1000 days: a qualitative study. Acad Pediatr 19, 748755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcantara, M, Ares, G, de Castro, IPL et al. (2020) Gain vs. loss-framing for reducing sugar consumption: insights from a choice experiment with six product categories. Food Res Int 136, 109458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wansink, B & Pope, L (2015) When do gain-framed health messages work better than fear appeals? Nutr Rev 73, 411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malik, VS, Popkin, BM, Bray, GA et al. (2010) Sugar-sweetened beverages, obesity, type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease risk. Circulation 121, 13561364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, CW & Zhou, MS (2020) Household food insecurity and the association with cumulative biological risk among lower-income adults: results from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys 2007–2010. Nutrients 12, 1517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baig, SA, Noar, SM, Gottfredson, NC et al. (2018) UNC Perceived Message Effectiveness: validation of a Brief Scale. Ann Behav Med 53, 732742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grummon, AH & Brewer, NT (2020) Health Warnings and Beverage Purchase Behavior: mediators of Impact. Ann Behav Med 54, 691702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taillie, LS, Hall, MG, Popkin, BM et al. (2020) Experimental studies of front-of-package nutrient warning labels on sugar-sweetened beverages and ultra-processed foods: a scoping review. Nutrients 12, 569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Participant flow

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Five initial warning label mock-ups and additional colour variations rated and discussed by focus group participants. Note: The first five labels were shown to participants on a single page against a white background in random order. Participants were also shown an image of where labels would be placed (near blue and multi-coloured fountain drink dispensers and on red, yellow and green cans or bottles). Additional colours were displayed against a blue dispenser only. **Colours ranked as most effective (P < 0·01)

Figure 2

Table 1 Characteristics of eighty-six focus group participants

Figure 3

Table 2 Exemplary quotes from focus group discussions and free-response questionnaire items evaluating sugar-sweetened beverage warning label designs

Figure 4

Fig. 3 Top ranking icon or image concepts to accompany a sugar-sweetened beverage warning labelNote: Supplemental Figure 1 shows all icon and image concepts ranked by participants.

Supplementary material: File

Falbe et al. supplementary material

Falbe et al. supplementary material 1

Download Falbe et al. supplementary material(File)
File 7.5 MB
Supplementary material: File

Falbe et al. supplementary material

Falbe et al. supplementary material 2

Download Falbe et al. supplementary material(File)
File 21 KB