No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Cultural evolution in more than two dimensions: Distinguishing social learning biases and identifying payoff structures
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 26 February 2014
Abstract
Bentley et al.’s two-dimensional conceptual map is complementary to cultural evolution research that has sought to explain population-level cultural dynamics in terms of individual-level behavioral processes. Here, I qualify their scheme by arguing that different social learning biases should be treated distinctly, and that the transparency of decisions is sometimes conflated with the actual underlying payoff structure of those decisions.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014
References
Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W. & Shennan, S. J. (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B
271:1443–50.Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Gintis, H. (2007) A framework for the unification of the behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
30(1):1–61.Google Scholar
Griffiths, T. L., Kalish, M. L. & Lewandowsky, S. (2008) Theoretical and empirical evidence for the impact of inductive biases on cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B
363:3503–14.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2008) An experimental simulation of the “copy-successful-individuals” cultural learning strategy: Adaptive landscapes, producer–scrounger dynamics, and informational access costs. Evolution and Human Behavior
29:350–63.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011) Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & Lycett, S. J. (2009) Random copying, frequency-dependent copying and culture change. Evolution and Human Behavior
30:41–48.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & O'Brien, M. J. (2008a) The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity
73:3–28.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & O'Brien, M. J. (2008b) The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile point technology II: An agent-based computer simulation. American Antiquity
73:627–44.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A. & Laland, K. N. (2006) Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
29:329–83.Google Scholar
O'Brien, M. J., Darwent, J. & Lyman, R. L. (2001) Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the southeastern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science
28:1115–36.Google Scholar
Wright, S. (1932) The roles of mutation, inbreeding, crossbreeding and selection in evolution. Proceedings of the Sixth International Congress of Genetics 1:356–66.Google Scholar
Target article
Cultural evolution in more than two dimensions: Distinguishing social learning biases and identifying payoff structures
Related commentaries (1)
Mapping collective behavior in the big-data era