INTRODUCTION
In learning language, young children face a challenging task. Not only must they work out what the words they hear mean, but they must also remember these words and their referents. Given the complexity of the speech that children hear and the limitless potential meanings of these words, this task seems almost impossible (Quine, Reference Quine1960), and there has been much interest in the general mechanisms by which children are able to learn words. For example, children constrain the potential meaning of words they hear according to the shape (Landau, Smith & Jones, Reference Landau, Smith and Jones1988) and taxonomic category (Markman & Hutchinson, Reference Markman and Hutchinson1984) of potential referents. Constraining the potential meaning of heard words in these ways is the first step towards successful word learning (McMurray, Horst & Samuelson, Reference McMurray, Horst and Samuelson2012).
While a focus on the general mechanisms underlying word learning has helped to better understand the processes by which children acquire language, the vast variability in early language development must also be more thoroughly examined. At the age of two, the number of different words that a child can typically produce as reported by parents can range from 7 to 668 (Dale & Fenson, Reference Dale and Fenson1996), and a similarly wide range is demonstrated in two-year-old children's spontaneous speech production (Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer & Lyons, Reference Huttenlocher, Haight, Bryk, Seltzer and Lyons1991). Nevertheless, the large majority of children with low productive vocabularies will go on to develop language normally (Desmarais, Sylvestre, Meyer, Bairati & Rouleau, Reference Desmarais, Sylvestre, Meyer, Bairati and Rouleau2008; Kelly, Reference Kelly1998; Whitehurst & Fischel, Reference Whitehurst and Fischel1994).
Variability in vocabulary scores has been traditionally explored by examining the extrinsic factors that can serve to affect a child's language exposure, with a particular focus on the effect of socioeconomic status. Children from poorer backgrounds are consistently shown to have lower vocabularies than their better-off peers (Evans, Reference Evans2004). While at first this effect was understood to be due to differences in the amount of language to which a child is exposed (Hart & Risely, Reference Hart and Risley1995), more recent research has emphasized that the quality of early language exposure is also critical (Hirsh-Pasek et al., Reference Hirsh-Pasek, Adamson, Bakeman, Owen, Golinkoff, Pace and Suma2015). While this research has been influential in highlighting that the quality of language exposure impacts on language acquisition, there have also been calls for a more thorough examination of the intrinsic factors that affect cognitive development (Mareschal, Johnson, Sirois, Spratling, Thomas & Westermann, Reference Mareschal, Johnson, Sirois, Spratling, Thomas and Westermann2007). Children's behavioral styles vary widely, yet the effect of these variations is often neglected in explaining language development. One intrinsic factor that has, however, been shown to affect early language acquisition is temperament (Slomkowski, Nelson, Dunn & Plomin, Reference Slomkowski, Nelson, Dunn and Plomin1992), specifically shyness (Coplan & Evans, Reference Coplan and Evans2009).
There have been many attempts to operationalize the construct of shyness in young children, from general constructs such as behavioral inhibition as studied in young infants (Kagan, Reznick & Snidman, Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1987), to more specific conceptualizations focusing on the specific shy-type behaviors exhibited in certain contexts (e.g. peer-to-peer interactions; Gazelle, Reference Gazelle2008). The current work draws on a broader definition of shyness, based on Putnam, Gartstein, and Rothbart's (Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006) operationalization of shyness as an enduring, biologically based individual difference characterized by discomfort in (predominantly novel) social situations, as measured via the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ). Reports on use of the ECBQ have shown that shyness can be reliably measured in children from the age of 18 months (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006).
The effect of shyness on verbal communication has been well documented; for example, it is known that shyness reduces children's verbal interaction in novel and familiar contexts (e.g. Asendorpf & Meier, Reference Asendorpf and Meier1993). While this and similar research has established a clear link between shyness and language development, the effect of shyness on language has typically been measured relatively late in development, when children are at least four years old (Asendorpf & Meier, Reference Asendorpf and Meier1993; Broberg, Lamb, Hwang & Bookstein, Reference Broberg, Lamb, Hwang and Bookstein1990; Coplan & Weeks, Reference Coplan and Weeks2009; Crozier & Badawood, Reference Crozier and Badawood2009; Crozier & Perkins, Reference Crozier and Perkins2002; Evans, Reference Evans1996; Reynolds & Evans, Reference Reynolds and Evans2009). Recently, however, interest has turned to the effect of shyness on the critical early stage of language acquisition during the second year of life (Smith Watts et al., Reference Smith Watts, Patel, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt, Robinson and Rhee2014). Smith Watts and colleagues examined how shyness impacts on receptive and productive language development, and reported that shyness is more strongly related to expressive than receptive vocabulary size. These authors argued that shy children's lower language ability during the second year of life can be explained in terms of a reticence to respond, rather than any difficulty in learning the meaning of words. Shy children's reticence to respond has been demonstrated in home settings (Reynolds & Evans, Reference Reynolds and Evans2009) and in experimental settings (Crozier & Perkins, Reference Crozier and Perkins2002), and it is also supported by research in non-Western cultures (Crozier & Badawood, Reference Crozier and Badawood2009).
One critical limitation of the extant literature in support of a reticence to respond explanation is that it does not predict an effect of shyness on receptive vocabulary. However, there are many reports of a relation between shyness and receptive vocabulary (Crozier & Badawood, Reference Crozier and Badawood2009; Crozier & Hostettler, Reference Crozier and Hostettler2003; Crozier & Perkins, Reference Crozier and Perkins2002; Rudasill, Prokasky, Tu, Frohn, Sirota & Molfese, Reference Rudasill, Prokasky, Tu, Frohn, Sirota and Molfese2014; Slomkowski et al., Reference Slomkowski, Nelson, Dunn and Plomin1992; Spere, Schmidt, Theall-Honey & Martin-Chang, Reference Spere, Schmidt, Theall-Honey and Martin-Chang2004). Interestingly, even Smith Watts and colleagues (Reference Smith Watts, Patel, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt, Robinson and Rhee2014) found relations between receptive vocabulary and shyness, but they argued that their data supported a reticence to respond explanation because the relations between shyness and productive vocabulary were more pronounced. Here we suggest that further insights into the ways in which shyness affects language development can be achieved by investigating the role of shyness in the processes that support word learning.
Laboratory examinations of children's “remarkable” ability to acquire words during the second year of life often employ fast mapping tasks that demonstrate the ability to rapidly associate novel labels with novel objects (Axelsson, Churchley & Horst, Reference Axelsson, Churchley and Horst2012; Halberda, Reference Halberda2006; Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008; Horst, Samuelson, Kucker & McMurray, Reference Horst, Samuelson, Kucker and McMurray2011; Spiegel & Halberda, Reference Spiegel and Halberda2011; Vlach & Sandhofer, Reference Vlach and Sandhofer2012). In these tasks, children are usually presented with a novel object alongside objects for which a label is already known and are asked to identify the object referred to by a novel pseudo-word (e.g. “Where's the blicket?”; Markman & Wachtel, Reference Markman and Wachtel1988). Children begin to demonstrate the ability to pick the novel object from the age of 15 months (Markman, Wasow & Hansen, Reference Markman, Wasow and Hansen2003), and more recent work indicates that some children may do this as early as 12 months (Xu, Cote & Baker, Reference Xu, Cote and Baker2005).
Nevertheless, a closer look at the literature reveals considerable individual differences in fast mapping ability. For example, in a study investigating the age-related development of fast mapping, Wilkinson, Ross, and Diamond (Reference Wilkinson, Ross and Diamond2003) classed 25% of the under 42-month-old children in their experiment as unreliable fast mappers. In other studies 24-month-old children selected the novel object only on approximately three-quarters of trials (Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008; Twomey, Ranson & Horst, Reference Twomey, Ranson and Horst2014). Similarly, in a study by Mervis and Bertrand (Reference Mervis and Bertrand1994) with 16–20-month-old children, half of the children did not reliably select the novel object upon hearing a novel label.
Critically, children's selection of the novel object as the referent of a novel label has been attributed to a novelty bias, which is so strong at 18 months that it leads children to select the novel object as the referent of even a familiar word (Samuelson, Kucker & Spencer, Reference Samuelson, Kucker and Spencer2016). Even at 24 months of age, children reliably select a familiar target object (Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008), but they also select a novel object as the referent of a novel label when competitors are nameless but pre-familiarized objects (Horst et al., Reference Horst, Samuelson, Kucker and McMurray2011), demonstrating the importance of novelty preference in driving children's object selection on referent selection trials. Individual differences in performance on referent selection trials may therefore be explained in terms of differences in novelty preference. That novelty preference is critical in supporting referent selection would indicate that shyness could come to bear on this ability, because we know that shy children are averse to novelty (Kagan et al., Reference Kagan, Reznick and Snidman1987). This aversion to novelty could mean that shy children's novelty preference is reduced in comparison to not-shy children.
Even once the novel object is selected as the referent of a novel word, this novel word–object mapping is not necessarily learned. A number of studies have argued that children's ability to select the correct referent and the ability to retain the label–object mapping over a period of time are governed by related, but separate, processes (Axelsson et al., Reference Axelsson, Churchley and Horst2012; Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008; Kucker, McMurray & Samuelson, Reference Kucker, McMurray and Samuelson2015; Munro, Baker, McGregor, Docking & Arciuli, Reference Munro, Baker, McGregor, Docking and Arciuli2012). In tests of retention, children are presented with (usually three) objects for which they have fast mapped novel labels and are asked for one of these objects using its novel label. Despite success in forming the initial word–object mappings, children often have difficulty retaining these mappings. For example, in one typical study Horst and Samuelson (Reference Horst and Samuelson2008) found that after a 5-minute delay, 24-month-old children were unable to select the correct object above levels that would be expected by chance.
We know that extrinsic factors can affect whether a mapping is retained. Horst and Samuelson (Reference Horst and Samuelson2008) found that by reinforcing the mapping by ostensively naming the object (e.g. by pointing and labeling) after the child had made the initial word–object mapping, the likelihood that it would be retained was increased. This finding suggested that attention to the referent during referent selection determines whether the novel word–object mapping will be retained, supported by subsequent work which found that highlighting the target object by illuminating it and dampening attention to competitors by partially covering them also increased retention (Axelsson et al., Reference Axelsson, Churchley and Horst2012). This work suggested that the attention directed toward the target during referent selection alone is not sufficient to support retention. Again, then, shy children's aversion to novelty could modulate their attentional pattern across the objects during referent selection, which would impact on their retention of any word–object mappings.
Children are clearly variable in their demonstration of the mechanisms that support word learning, and we suggest that shyness could explain some of this variability. If shyness impacts on the most basic and early processes of word learning, this could help to explain the relation between shyness and language development as measured more generally (e.g. Spere et al., Reference Spere, Schmidt, Theall-Honey and Martin-Chang2004). Specifically, one can assume that children who are better able to focus their attention on the novel object during labeling should be better able to retain the word–object mapping. We hypothesize that shyness can affect retention of novel word–object mappings formed via referent selection by modulating children's attention during referent selection. Such a hypothesis is in line with recent accounts arguing that differences in engagement with objects can explain differences in word learning and categorization of these objects (e.g. Perry, Reference Perry2015). If shy children demonstrate lower levels of retention than their not-shy peers, it would mean that shyness is affecting the acquisition of language from one of its earliest stages: the initial mapping of a label to its referent. Such a fine-grained analysis of the effects of shyness on the formation of label–object mappings, rather than focusing solely on vocabulary size or language production, means that we can begin to understand the precise ways in which language is shaped by shyness.
In the present study, two-year-old children took part in both referent selection and retention tests of mapping novel labels to novel objects. A sample of children who varied in their shyness completed the same referent selection trials in which they were presented with one novel object alongside two familiar objects and were asked for a novel label. Trials were included in which children were asked for a familiar object. After a 5-minute break, all children then took part in retention trials. We also investigated whether external directing of attention to the target object via ostensive naming of targets during referent selection facilitated shy children's retention.
METHOD
Participants
Thirty-two typically developing 24-month-old children participated (M = 24 m, 8 days, SD = 11 days; range = 23 m, 19 days to 24 m, 29 days; 16 girls and 16 boys). All children were monolingual, predominantly from middle-class families of Caucasian ethnicity living in the north-west of England, UK. Families were recruited by contacting parents who had previously indicated interest in participating in child development research. Parents’ travel expenses were reimbursed and children were offered a storybook for participating. Data from nine additional children were excluded from analyses due to experimenter error (n = 1), parental involvement (n = 2), refusal to complete the task (n = 3), and the parent not returning the questionnaire (n = 3).
The Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ) was used to assess children's shyness as an enduring, biologically based individual difference. The ECBQ asks parents to rate on a scale from 1 to 7 how often their child has demonstrated particular behaviors over the previous two weeks (1 = ‘never’, 7 = ‘all the time’), as a means of assessing the child's score on eighteen fine-grained subdimensions of temperament. While these subdimensions include areas such as attention focusing, activity level, and high- or low-intensity pleasure, the shyness subdimension was of particular interest for the current study. The shyness scale consists of twelve items that assess how children react in situations that elicit shy behavior, for example “When approaching unfamiliar children playing, how often did your child watch rather than join?” and “In situations where s/he is meeting new people, how often did your child become quiet?”. Each child was scored from 1 to 7 by averaging their parents’ responses to the twelve questions relating to shyness (after correcting for reverse coded questions). A score of 1 is considered the least shy, and a score of 7 is considered the most shy. Standardized Cronbach's alpha for the shyness measure was 0.76. Once all data were collected, the median score for all children was calculated (3·67). Children scoring above the median formed the shy group and those scoring less than the median formed the not-shy group. Three children's scores equaled the median, and they were randomly assigned to a group, ensuring equal sample size (n = 16). There were equal numbers of boys and girls (n = 8) in each group. There was no difference between groups in age (t(30) = 1·45, p = ·16). Unsurprisingly, children in the not-shy group were scored as less shy (M = 2·73, SD = 0·62) than children in the shy group (M = 4·14, SD = 0·48) (t(30) = –7·19, p < ·001). Shyness scores were calculated after the testing session had taken place, which meant that the shyness grouping of the participants was not evident during testing.
In order to examine whether ostensive naming interacts with shyness to affect referent selection and retention, eight boys and eight girls were assigned to an ostensive naming and a no ostensive naming condition. Assignment of children to ostensive and no ostensive naming conditions occurred before it was possible to calculate a median shyness score, so children were randomly assigned to each condition prior to each testing session. There was no difference between conditions in age (t(30) = –1·34, p = ·19). As expected by a random assignment, there were equal numbers of not-shy children in the ostensive naming (n = 8; 5 girls), and no ostensive naming (n = 8; 3 girls) conditions. Similarly, there were equal numbers of shy children in the ostensive naming (n = 8; 3 girls) and no ostensive naming (n = 8; 5 girls) conditions.
Stimuli
The same stimuli were used in both conditions. Eleven objects, selected because they are easily named by 24-month-olds, acted as familiar objects, and consisted of animals (duck, elephant, fish, pig), vehicles (car, motorbike, helicopter), and household objects (spoon, fork, cup, and ball). Four novel objects, selected because they are not normally familiar to 24-month-old children, were a plastic roller, a wooden massager, a tea strainer, and a miniature dumbbell (see Figure 1). Four distinct nonwords (cheem, koba, sprock, tannin) were used to name the novel objects, taken from previous research using similar methodology (Behrend, Scofield & Kleinknecht, Reference Behrend, Scofield and Kleinknecht2001; Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008; Markson & Bloom, Reference Markson and Bloom1997; Samuelson & Horst, Reference Samuelson and Horst2007). The assignment of each name to each novel object was randomized across participants. To ensure the objects would be familiar and novel to each participant as expected, parents were shown photographs of the objects before the experiment began and confirmed their familiarity or novelty. All objects were similar in size (approximate mean size 5 cm × 6 cm × 9 cm). The objects were presented on a wooden tray, divided into three equal compartments for the objects to be placed.
PROCEDURE AND DESIGN
Prior to their visit to the lab, parents completed the ECBQ, and during their visit children took part in the word learning task.
Word learning task
Warm-up trials
During the task children sat on their parent's lap across a white table from the experimenter. Once settled, the warm-up trials began. All trials consisted of three objects being presented to the child on the tray. For warm-up trials, the three objects were familiar and their purpose was to introduce the child to the forced-choice task, and to ensure that they understood the instructions of the experimenter. For each trial the experimenter presented the objects and asked for the target six times (i.e. “Wow a duck! Where's the duck? See the duck! Find the duck! Get the duck! Can you get the duck?”). The experimenter then slid the tray forward into reaching distance for the child, and gave the child the opportunity to retrieve the requested object. If correct, the child was praised, and if incorrect, the child was corrected. The order in which the objects were requested was randomly determined prior to testing. The three familiar objects presented on warm-up trials were not presented in subsequent referent selection trials. Warm-up trials continued until children had correctly selected each referent once. The majority of children needed no more than three warm-up trials (8 children needed four warm-up trials and one child needed six warm-up trials.)
Referent selection trials
The referent selection trials began immediately after the warm-up trials. Referent selection trials proceeded in the same manner as the warm-up trials, but differed in three ways. First, two of the objects were familiar and one was novel. Second, neither praise nor corrections were given following the child's referent selection. Third, children were offered the chance to explore the three objects for 30 seconds before they were asked to select an object. During this exploration time, the three objects were presented on the tray to the child, and the child and caregiver played with the objects for 30 seconds. During this exploration, parents were instructed to avoid labeling or asking for labels for the objects, and if needed the experimenter directed the child's attention to each of the objects at least once, either by pointing at it, or if that was unsuccessful, by maneuvering it into the child's line of vision. Once 30 seconds had elapsed, the objects were returned to their positions on the tray, and the child was asked for the target object.
Once children had selected an object, it was returned to its position on the tray. Children in the ostensive naming condition then saw the target object ostensively named (irrespective of whether they had correctly chosen the target object or not), meaning that the experimenter pointed at the target object and labeled it (e.g. “It's a car.”). Children in the no ostensive naming condition saw the experimenter point to the target object and say “There it is”. To ensure that children in each condition heard the label an equal number of times (which was relevant for the subsequent retention test), children in the ostensive naming condition heard the label one time fewer during the initial request, by removing a labeling phrase (e.g. “Where's the koba?”). This arrangement meant that children in both conditions heard the object labeled six times. The object label was spoken multiple times, in line with previous research (e.g. Axelsson et al., Reference Axelsson, Churchley and Horst2012).
Each child was presented with four sets of three objects, and each set was shown twice. In one of these presentations the child was asked for a novel object, and in the other was asked for a familiar object. The objects comprising each set and the labels assigned to each novel object were randomized across children. The order of sets and trials was pseudo-randomized, ensuring that any trial type (i.e. familiar or novel) was not presented on more than two consecutive trials.
Retention trials
Following the referent selection trials there was a 5-minute break, during which the child was offered crayons and paper for coloring. This break was to ensure that any retention in the following trials was the product of long-term memory (following Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008). After the 5-minute break, the child participated in four retention trials, which did not differ between conditions. Again, children were presented with sets of three objects. These were all novel objects that had been presented and labeled during referent selection. Across the four retention trials, each novel object was the target once. The two novel objects presented alongside the target were randomly determined. On presentation of each set, the child was asked to select the target once (e.g. “Where's the koba?”). The tray was slid forward and the child indicated the chosen object. The order of trials was randomized across participants.
RESULTS
Word learning tasks
Each child took part in four familiar and four novel referent selection trials. For each, a ‘proportion of correct choices’ measure was calculated by dividing the number of correct responses by the total possible (i.e. 4 for familiar and 4 for novel referent selection trials). One participant (shyness score = 2·5) did not respond on two referent selection trials, and one participant (shyness score = 2·7) did not respond on one referent selection trial. A response was elicited from children on all other referent selection trials. A response was elicited for all retention trials.
Performance on referent selection trials replicated previous work (e.g. Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008). Overall, selection of the correct familiar object was higher than would be expected by chance (M = 0·87, SD = 0·18) (t(31) = 16·94, p < ·001; d = 2·99). Furthermore, in trials where a novel label was heard, children selected the novel object significantly above chance, demonstrating the ability to fast map (M = 0·59, SD = 0·32) (t(31) = 4·60, p < ·001; d = 0·81). Overall, children were better at selecting the familiar than the novel object (t(31) = 6·12, p < ·001; d = 1·08). As expected, because the critical manipulation happened after children had selected the referent, there was no difference between ostensive naming conditions on either familiar referent selection trials (t(30) = 0·73, p = ·47), or novel referent selection trials (t(30) = 1·28, p = ·21).
The following analyses only included retention trials in which the child had correctly selected the target during the corresponding novel referent selection trials. For example, if the child had chosen the car when asked for the cheem during referent selection, the retention trial for the mapping of cheem was not included (following Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008). Overall, children were unable to retain the object–label mappings they had made during referent selection, selecting the target object no more often than would be expected by chance (M = 0·38, SD = 0·35) (t(31) = 0·82, p = ·42). We investigated whether particular objects were more likely to be selected than others during referent selection and retention trials. We found that children did not select certain familiar objects more often than others (χ 2 (7, N = 128) = 1·08, p > ·99). Likewise, novel object selection was not greater for any particular object (χ 2 (3, N = 125) = 2·56, p = ·46). Again during the retention task children did not choose any novel objects more often than others (χ 2 (3, N = 128) = 5·19, p = ·16).
Shyness and referent selection
Shyness was not significantly related to children's referent selection on familiar label trials (r s (30) = –0·27, p = ·14), but shyness was significantly related to children's referent selection on novel label trials (r s (30) = –0·38, p = ·03), demonstrating that an increase in shyness score meant a decrease in proportion of trials on which the novel object was selected as the referent of a novel label.
The observed effects were specific to scores on the shyness subscale of the ECBQ. There was no relation between novel referent selection and negative affectivity, the core factor of temperament of which shyness is a subscale (r s (30) = –0·14, p = ·44), or with the other core factors: surgency (r s (30) = –0·12, p = ·50) and effortful control (r s (30) = 0·07, p = ·70). There was also no relation between novel referent selection other subscales of Negative Affectivity, such as fear (r s (30) = –0·14, p = ·45), cuddliness (r s (30) = –0·03, p = ·89), or sociability (r s (30) = –0·06, p = ·76).
We also wanted to examine the extent to which shyness impacted on referent selection by examining performance in relation to chance. Shyness groups were thus compared.
As shown in Figure 2, children in the not-shy group were better than would be expected by chance at selecting the familiar object (t(15) = 16·84, p < ·001; d = 4·21) and novel object (t(15) = 5·76, p < ·001; d = 1·44). In contrast, children in the shy group were better than would be expected by chance only at selecting the familiar object (t(15) = 9·96, p < ·001; d = 2·49), but not at selecting the novel object (t(15) = 1·53, p = ·15).
In order to further examine how shyness affects children's referent selection, a 2 (trial type: familiar or novel) × 2 (shy group) mixed Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was performed. This analysis revealed a main effect of trial type (F(1,30) = 40·00, p < ·001, η p 2 = ·55), meaning that children chose the target often more often on familiar trials (M = 0·87, SD = 0·18) than novel trials (M = 0·59, SD = 0·32). There was no interaction between trial type and shyness (F(1,30) = 3·09, p = ·09). As expected, there was a significant main effect of shyness group (F(1,30) = 10·16, p = ·003, η p 2 = ·25), meaning that children in the not-shy group chose the target object more often (M = 0·84, SD = 0·24) than children in the shy group (M = 0·62, SD = 0·30).
Shyness and retention trials
There was no relation between shyness and retention (r s (30) = –0·29, p = ·11) when trials testing retention of novel mappings not formed during referent selection were excluded. However, as shown in Figure 3, children in the not-shy group were significantly better at retaining the word–object mappings than would be expected by chance (t(15) = 2·46, p = ·03; d = 0·61). In contrast, children in the shy group performed at chance during retention (t(15) = –0·67, p = ·52). A 2 (shy group) × 2 (ostensive naming) Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) revealed no main effect of ostensive naming (F(1,28) = 0·07, p = ·79), no main effect of shyness (F(1,28) = 3·65, p = ·07), and no significant interaction (F(1,28) = 0·76, p = ·39), demonstrating that ostensive naming had no effect on retention.
It was possible that shy children learned the novel word–object mappings even though they did not select the novel object during referent selection. Therefore, we also examined whether shyness affected children's retention when averaged across all retention trials, regardless of whether the novel object was selected as the referent of the novel word during referent selection. No relation was found (r s (30) = –0·18, p = ·32). Shy children did not retain the word–object mappings above levels expected by chance (M = 0·38, SD = 0·29) (t(15) = 0·62, p = ·54), whereas not-shy children did retain the mappings (M = 0·45, SD = 0·16) (t(15) = 3·01, p < ·01; d = 0·75).
DISCUSSION
The present study found that shy children were less likely than not-shy children to correctly identify a requested familiar or a novel object from a set of three. Specifically, shy children performed at chance in trials in which a novel object was requested using a novel label, whereas not-shy children performed above chance. Furthermore, after a 5-minute break, shy children did not show evidence of retaining the mappings that they had previously made, whereas not-shy children demonstrated retention above levels expected by chance. This study found no evidence that ostensive naming affected children's retention of novel word–object mappings. Importantly, our results emphasize that a child's emerging temperament can affect basic processes involved in word learning. When considered alongside the finding that shy children do not reliably retain any mappings that they have made, this study suggests that intrinsic factors, and not just differences in input, are important contributors to variability in early language acquisition.
The results of our study suggest that the mapping of a novel label to a novel object is not as robust and automatic for all children as has been previously reported (Halberda, Reference Halberda2003; Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008), and it shows that shy children as a group do not demonstrate use of fast mapping to disambiguate the referents of words that they hear in a lab setting. Furthermore, the mappings that were formed by shy children were not then subsequently retained. The present study also indicated that selection of a correct familiar referent is similarly affected by shyness, as expected in light of recent suggestions that familiar word recognition is supported by the same processes as the mapping of a novel word to a novel object (McMurray et al., Reference McMurray, Horst and Samuelson2012).
Several hypotheses have been proposed to explain shy children's lower vocabulary scores, and each hypothesis offers a different explanation for the findings of the present study. The “I know it but won't say it” hypothesis (Coplan & Evans, Reference Coplan and Evans2009) suggests that shy children learn words normally, but are reticent to demonstrate this ability. This hypothesis thus assumes that shyness does not affect receptive vocabulary, supported by a recent paper which reported that expressive vocabulary size is more affected by shyness than receptive vocabulary size (Smith Watts et al., Reference Smith Watts, Patel, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt, Robinson and Rhee2014). However, the present study employed a measure of receptive language ability, which suggests that a reticence to respond is not an adequate explanation of the relation between shyness and language development, instead supporting earlier work that has reported a relation between shyness and receptive language development (e.g. Spere et al., Reference Spere, Schmidt, Theall-Honey and Martin-Chang2004).
Furthermore, while the present study found that shy children were less likely to respond correctly when asked to select a familiar object, and to map novel labels onto novel objects, shy children did not simply refuse to respond. In fact, across all participants only three trials (out of a possible 384) were coded as non-responses. The “I know it but won't say it” hypothesis does not explain why shy children will offer a supposedly incorrect response; their behavior may be better described as “I may know it, but won't say it, but I will say something anyway.” Importantly, then, this study demonstrates that simply eliciting a response from a shy child may not provide a valid measure of their knowledge.
The present study found that shy children do not show evidence of retaining the (few) word–object mappings that they successfully made during referent selection. Pre-familiarization of stimuli prior to labeling usually boosts levels of retention (Kucker & Samuelson, Reference Kucker and Samuelson2012), so it is remarkable that even under these conditions shy children showed no retention of any novel word–object mappings. This finding clearly demonstrates that shyness does not only impact on in-the-moment online processes, but also the longer-term processes that support retention necessary in the acquisition of vocabulary. Beyond demonstrating that receptive language is not immune to the effects of shyness, these findings suggest that previous research describing poor retention on the group level (Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008) might be explained by not having included shyness as a covariate. Again, the failure of shy children to retain mappings cannot be explained by reticence to respond; they offered a response on all retention trials, too.
Apart from the reticence to respond explanation, several alternative explanations have been put forward to account for the differences between shy and not-shy children on measures of language development. One, known as the “lack of practice” hypothesis (Coplan & Evans, Reference Coplan and Evans2009), argues that shy children tend to avoid social situations, and in doing so limit both their exposure to others’ language and an opportunity to practice language while receiving contingent feedback. Empirical support for this hypothesis has been found, for example, in the discovery that shy children speak less than their not-shy peers in experimental settings (Crozier & Perkins, Reference Crozier and Perkins2002) and at home (Reynolds & Evans, Reference Reynolds and Evans2009). Again, this explanation draws heavily on language development as measured in terms of vocabulary size, and in light of the present findings, assumes that children's ability to form and retain object–label mappings can be moderated by experience and practice. To explain our results according to this hypothesis, one would have to assume that children's previous experience and interactions are shaped by shyness to such an extent that either shy children miss out on the opportunity to develop the understanding that requests should be responded to correctly, or that they restrict their language input to the extent that they cannot acquire the mechanisms needed to map novel labels onto novel objects. Therefore, while the lack of practice might well explain a lower vocabulary size in young children due to lack of exposure, it is unlikely that it affects the mechanisms of word learning per se, particularly given the shift towards an understanding of the domain-general nature of the processes that support this ability (e.g. Samuelson & Smith, Reference Samuelson and Smith1998).
A third hypothesis is based on findings that older shy individuals are less likely to take risks (Addison & Schmidt, Reference Addison and Schmidt1999; Levin & Hart, Reference Levin and Hart2003). While it is still unclear whether such an aversion to risk has emerged in shy children at this young age, we argue that it is possible that shy children are less likely to rely on a guess when responding, which also explains the effect of shyness on expressive vocabulary (Coplan & Evans, Reference Coplan and Evans2009). The effect may be particularly pronounced in tests of word learning that involve novel words with ambiguous referents, as in the present study where children were required to map the novel word to the novel object using only novelty as a cue. Novelty is inherently ambiguous and does not rule out competitors as potential referents with certainty. Therefore, an aversion to risk could disrupt children's use of novelty as a cue to referent selection.
Alongside an aversion to risks, shy children also demonstrate inhibited approach and discomfort in social situations involving novelty (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006). Most experimental settings are fundamentally unfamiliar to a young participant. Tests are typically carried out by a researcher who is unknown to the child, often in an unfamiliar room (Heibeck & Markman, Reference Heibeck and Markman1987; Horst & Samuelson, Reference Horst and Samuelson2008; Horst et al., Reference Horst, Samuelson, Kucker and McMurray2011; Horst, Scott & Pollard, Reference Horst, Scott and Pollard2010; Spiegel & Halberda, Reference Spiegel and Halberda2011; Zosh, Brinster & Halberda, Reference Zosh, Brinster and Halberda2013). This inhibited approach may also have come to bear on performance in the present study. When shy children were certain that their response was correct, they were able to overcome the problems posed by the unfamiliar setting to some extent, because as a group they reliably selected a familiar object when requested. Yet shy children selected the correct familiar object less often than not-shy children, suggesting that an unfamiliar setting can impact on shy children's demonstration of label referents. Furthermore, when asked for a novel label, shy children were just as likely to select a familiar object as the novel object. It is therefore likely that shy children's reluctance to rely on ambiguity in responding to requests, alongside their aversion to novelty, produce a familiarity preference on novel label trials.
These effects must, however, be further teased apart in order to better understand how best to tailor the testing environment so as to not disadvantage shy children. Reducing the unfamiliarity of the testing procedure could reduce shy children's preference for familiar objects, and may allow children to feel more comfortable in taking a risk and making use of novelty as a cue to the correct referent on novel label trials. However, shy children's aversion to novelty may affect their responses by restricting their attention to and encoding of the novel label–object mapping, supported by the finding that even on trials where they attended to the novel object enough to select it during referent selection, these mappings were not retained, unlike the mappings formed by the not-shy children. It may be the case that the encoding of the word–object mapping by shy children is different to that of not-shy children, calling for more implicit measures of children's mapping of novel labels to novel objects in order to explore the effect of shyness on attention during the formation of the mappings.
Interestingly, ostensive naming did not facilitate shy children's retention of the novel word–object mappings, but no effect of ostensive naming was found overall either. This finding may then suggest that pointing at and labeling the object was not a robust enough cue to support retention. Instead, it may be necessary to move the object away from competitors during ostensive naming (e.g. by lifting the target off of the tray before labeling; see also Axelsson, Perry, Scott & Horst, Reference Axelsson, Perry, Scott and Horst2016), and future work will take this suggestion into account.
This study has shed further light on previous explanations for the effect of shyness on measures of language development, and examined whether they can account for the effect of shyness on the processes used to form word–object mappings. While researchers have been confident in claiming that this effect can be best explained in terms of shy children's reticence to respond (Smith Watts et al., Reference Smith Watts, Patel, Corley, Friedman, Hewitt, Robinson and Rhee2014), the current study found that shy children will respond to questions even when they are posed by an unfamiliar adult. The ‘lack of practice’ argument also offers an inadequate explanation for these findings, given that it is based on the unlikely assumption that lack of practice has impaired the very mechanisms by which they map words to objects. The explanation that best fits the present data is that shy children's aversion to risk-taking and novelty serves to enhance their preference for familiar objects on novel label trials, and their attention to and encoding of any possible word–object mappings is disrupted.
Most importantly, this study is a clear demonstration that intrinsic factors related to emerging temperamental domains can affect the acquisition of language by moderating the processes by which shy children disambiguate and retain word–object mappings. Therefore, variability in early language acquisition must be explored in terms of differences in the mechanisms by which language is acquired, not only differences in input. The present study has shown that, given the same input and cues to word learning, shy children respond differently from their not-shy peers. In further exploring the effect of emerging temperament on the processes involved in word learning, the field can be better informed in identifying children for whom language acquisition is more difficult, or just different.