Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:59:00.610Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between children’s home food environment and dietary patterns in childhood and adolescence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2010

Carine Vereecken*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Research Foundation-Flanders, Ghent University, De Pintelaan 185, 9000 Gent, Belgium Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
Leen Haerens
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Research Foundation-Flanders, Ghent University, De Pintelaan 185, 9000 Gent, Belgium Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
Ilse De Bourdeaudhuij
Affiliation:
Department of Movement and Sports Sciences, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
Lea Maes
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To identify the correlates of the home food environment (parents’ intake, availability and food-related parenting practices) at the age of 10 years with dietary patterns during childhood and in adolescence.

Setting

Primary-school children of fifty-nine Flemish elementary schools completed a questionnaire at school in 2002. Four years later they completed a questionnaire by e-mail or mail at home. Their parents completed a questionnaire on food-related parenting practices at baseline.

Design

Longitudinal study.

Subjects

The analyses included 609 matched questionnaires.

Statistics

Multi-level regression analyses were used to identify baseline parenting practices (pressure, reward, negotiation, catering on demand, permissiveness, verbal praise, avoiding negative modelling, availability of healthy/unhealthy food items and mothers’ fruit and vegetable (F&V) and excess scores) associated with children’s dietary patterns (F&V and excess scores).

Results

Mother’s F&V score was a significant positive independent predictor for children’s F&V score at baseline and follow-up, whereas availability of unhealthy foods was significantly negatively associated with both scores. Negotiation was positively associated with children’s follow-up score of F&V, while permissiveness was positively associated with children’s follow-up excess score. Availability of unhealthy foods and mother’s excess score were positively related to children’s excess score at baseline and follow-up.

Conclusions

Parental intake and restricting the availability of unhealthy foods not only appeared to have a consistent impact on children’s and adolescents’ diets, but also negotiating and less permissive food-related parenting practices may improve adolescents’ diets.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2010

Parents are of high importance in the development of their children’s dietary preferences that eventually lead to their dietary patterns. Parents directly determine the child’s physical and social environment by deciding which foods are available and in what quantity(Reference Nicklas, Baranowski and Baranowski1Reference Hearn, Baranowski and Baranowski3). Parents’ own food-related behaviours serve as a role model and thereby affect the dietary habits of their children(Reference Birch and Fisher4, Reference Cullen, Baranowski and Rittenberry5). Indirectly, parents influence their children’s behaviour and habits through socialization. They can apply different food management practices that control, encourage or restrict the intake of certain foods. Research of the recent past has drawn attention to these food-related parenting practices as having an important influence on children’s diet(Reference Faith, Scanlon and Birch6).

With the transition from childhood to adolescence, at the age of 12–13 years, adolescents become more independent, and therefore it might well be that the impact of parenting practices diminishes(Reference De Bourdeaudhuij7). Few studies have, however, investigated the influences of the home food environment (parental intake, availability and parenting practices) on adolescents’ diets.

In a study by Young et al.(Reference Young, Fors and Hayes8), perceived parental modelling and home availability were significantly associated with their children’s fruit and vegetable (F&V) consumption. In a cross-sectional study of project EAT (Eating among Teens)(Reference Hanson, Neumark-Sztainer and Eisenberg9), parental intake was positively associated with dairy intake for boys and with dairy, F&V intake for girls; significant positive associations were found for F&V intake by home availability among girls and for dairy intake by serving milk at meals for male adolescents. In a longitudinal study of project(Reference Arcan, Neumark-Sztainer and Hannan10) EAT, investigating a long-term role for parent modelling and availability above and beyond any short-term impact, parental intake predicted the dietary intake of young adults but not of high-school students; serving of vegetables at dinner (a measure of availability and accessibility) was a significant predictor of adolescents’ and young adults’ intake.

Focus groups suggest that food rules(Reference Neumark-Sztainer, Story and Perry11) and prior food rules(Reference De Bourdeaudhuij7) continue to exert their influence on adolescents’ food choices. In studies of van der Horst et al.(Reference van der Horst, Kremers and Ferreira12) and de Bruijn et al.(Reference de Bruijn, Kremers and de Vries13), more restrictive parenting practices were found to be associated with less soft drink consumption in adolescents. Congruent herewith are the retrospective studies of De Bourdeaudhuij(Reference De Bourdeaudhuij7), in which more fat and sweet foods were consumed by adolescents who reported more permissiveness (fewer restrictions and obligations) in their family at the age of 10 years. However, they found no evidence of a relationship of prior food rules with the consumption of healthy foods in adolescence. In a study by Haerens et al.(Reference Haerens, Craeynest and Deforche14), less restrictive food rules were associated with a higher fat intake in boys and lower fruit consumption in girls; however, no association was found with soft drink consumption.

The present study will build on the previous studies of De Bourdeaudhuij(Reference De Bourdeaudhuij7) in which a cross-sectional design was used to study influences on healthy and less healthy foods and in which the results are based on perceptions of family food rules in the past. In the present study, a longitudinal design was used: the mothers’ reports on parenting practices at the age of 10 years are used to predict intake of healthy and less healthy foods during childhood and adolescence as well as changes in intake during this transition. In addition, inclusion of measures of parental intake and availability will allow one to investigate the hypothesis that each of these factors is independently related to children’s and adolescents’ dietary intake.

Methodology

Procedure

The results presented here are based on the first and fourth measurements of the Longitudinal Eating and Activity study in which children have been followed from 2002 to 2005 (Fig. 1). In 2002, 100 elementary schools from two Flemish regions (East and West Flanders) were randomly selected from the official list of the Flemish government. The principals were sent a recruitment letter and afterwards contacted by phone. There were fifty-nine principals who agreed to cooperate in the study. The main reason for non-participation was lack of time. All children in the fifth grade (10-year-olds) were invited to participate in the study (n 1957). Informed consent to participate in the longitudinal study was received by 1725 parents (88 % of eligible children). In 2002 (T1), the children completed a self-administered questionnaire on eating habits and physical activity, demographic variables and possible psychosocial determinants in the classroom under the supervision of one researcher and their classroom teacher. The same procedure was followed for T2 (2003). In 2004, children changed, however, from primary to secondary schools, making classroom administration not feasible. Therefore, for T3 and T4, the children were contacted at home by postal mail. The envelope contained a letter addressed to the parents asking them to encourage their child to participate in the study, and a letter addressed to the child asking them to login with a personal code to a website and complete an online questionnaire. As the response rate was very low (about 30 %), non-respondents were sent a reminder including a paper–pencil questionnaire and a pre-stamped envelope, 8 weeks later.

Fig. 1 Response at school and individual levels – Longitudinal Eating and Activity study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005

In total, 874 adolescents completed the questionnaire at T4. Of these, 764 could be matched to T1. The main reason for not participating at T3 and T4 was inability to be reached (moved) or unwillingness to fill in the questionnaire. Only the respondents for whom the parental questionnaire was completed by the mother/stepmother were included in the current analyses (n 639).

Ethical approval was obtained from the Ethics Commission of the Ghent University Hospital.

Measures

Dietary patterns

The children/adolescents were asked how many times a week they usually consumed fruit, vegetables, sugared soft drinks, sweets and crisps (FFQ). The response options were: ‘never ( = 0)’, ‘<1 d/week ( = 0·25)’, 1 d/week ( = 1)’, ‘2–4 d/week ( = 3)’, ‘5–6 d/week ( = 5·5)’, ‘once a day’, ‘every day ( = 7)’, and ‘every day more than once ( = 14)’. The consumption of F&V is important in reducing the risk of cancer and CVD(Reference Johnson15Reference Ness and Powles17). The consumption of soft drinks and sweet and savoury snacks can impede the intake of more nutritious foods by reducing appetite control(Reference Ludwig, Peterson and Gortmaker18).

The two F&V items were added together to form an F&V score; the consumption of regular soft drinks, sweets and crisps was combined to form an excess score(Reference Vereecken, Rossi and Giacchi19).

Parents were asked to report their own intake with parallel questions at T1.

Food-related parenting practices

Items measuring food-related parenting practices were based on a pilot study(Reference Vereecken, Keukelier and Maes20). The items addressed the use of pressure (six items), encouragement through material reward (three items), encouragement through negotiation (five items), catering on children’s demand (four items), permissiveness (seven items), avoiding negative modelling (two items) and verbal praise (two items). Parents were asked to respond on a 5-point scale: 1 = never, 2 = mostly not, 3 = sometimes, 4 = most of the time and 5 = always. Internal consistencies of the subscales are reported in Table 1. For each scale, the average of the group of the respective practices was computed, if more than 50 % of the scale items were answered.

Table 1 Descriptives of dependent and independent variables: LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 609)

LEA, Longitudinal Eating and Activity study; F&V, fruit and vegetables.

The availability of fruit, soft drinks, biscuits, sweets and crisps was questioned on a 3-point scale (2 = always/mostly available, 1 = sometimes and 0 = rarely/never). The four unhealthy items were combined to form an unhealthy availability scale.

Statistics

Three sets of models were conducted. To examine associations between potential baseline correlates and the food scores, separate multi-level regression analyses were conducted for each parenting practice with baseline and follow-up food scores, controlled for sociodemographic characteristics (gender and mother’s educational level; model 1). To examine the multivariate association of all parenting practices simultaneously, all parenting practices were entered simultaneously in a second set of models (model 2). To examine the associations between parenting practices and change in food scores between baseline and follow-up, the previous model was additionally adjusted for baseline food scores (model 3).

All variables (except sociodemographics) were standardized to allow for relative comparisons of strength between the observed associations. The β-coefficients can be interpreted as the amount of sd change in food score associated with a 1 sd change in the respective parenting practice. Analyses were conducted using MLwiN version 2.02(Reference Rasbash, Charlton and Browne21), with respondents nested within schools at T1 (two-level random intercept model). To estimate the proportion of the explained variance of the home food environment, the proportion of unexplained variance of the full model is compared with a model including only a constant and sociodemographics for baseline measurement and including a constant, sociodemographics and baseline measurement for follow-up data. P values at <0·05 are considered significant.

Results

Of the 609 respondents included in the analyses (600 for the F&V score; 530 for the excess score), 50·6 % were boys. Mother’s educational level was distributed as follows: 24·5 % low (lower technical, higher vocational or less), 23·2 % medium (technical or general higher secondary education) and 52·4 % high (bachelor’s or master’s degree). There were 95% of mothers who reported that fruit was always or mostly available; therefore, this variable is not likely to be able to distinguish and was excluded for further analyses. Descriptives of the scale variables are presented in Table 1. Correlations between the dependent and independent variables are presented in Table 2.

Table 2 Correlations between standardized dependent and independent variables – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 609)

LEA, Longitudinal Eating and Activity study; F&V, fruit and vegetables.

*P < 0·05; **P < 0·01; ***P < 0·001.

Table 3 shows that lesser permissiveness, a lower unhealthy availability score and a higher F&V score for mothers were associated with a higher F&V score, both at baseline and follow-up (model 1). More negotiation was only significantly associated with a higher F&V score at baseline, although a significant association with follow-up data became visible when all variables were included in the analyses (model 2). However, the positive association between avoiding negative modelling and follow-up was washed out in the latter model. The only significant predictors for changes in F&V score were mother’s F&V score and availability of unhealthy foods.

Table 3 Associations of baseline factors with the F&V score at baseline and follow-up, 4 years later – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 600)

LEA, Longitudinal Eating and Activity study; F&V, fruit and vegetables.

Model 1: Separate regression analyses for each individual factor, controlling for gender and mother’s educational level; for the random part of model 1 only sociodemographics are included for baseline data and sociodemographics and child’s baseline F&V score for follow-up data.

Model 2: Multivariate regression model adjusted for all other variables.

Model 3: Model 2 additionally adjusted for baseline data.

Significant parameters are given in bold.

Reward, catering on demand, permissiveness, availability of unhealthy foods and mother’s excess score were all positively associated with children’s excess score at baseline, whereas avoiding negative modelling behaviour was negatively associated (Table 4, model 1). However, when all variables were entered in the same model (model 2), only availability and mother’s excess score remained significant. Availability of unhealthy foods, mother’s excess score and permissiveness were also positively related to the follow-up score (models 1 and 2) and changes in excess score from baseline to follow-up (model 3). However, the negative association of avoiding negative modelling disappeared in model 2.

Table 4 Associations of baseline factors with the excess score at baseline and follow-up, 4 years later – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 530)

LEA, Longitudinal Eating and Activity study; F&V, fruit and vegetables.

Sociodemographics are categorical and all other variables are scale variables.

Model 1: Separate regression analyses for each individual factor, controlling for gender and mother’s educational level; for the random part only sociodemographics are included for baseline data and sociodemographics and child’s baseline excess score for follow-up data.

Model 2: Multivariate regression model adjusted for all other variables.

Model 3: Model 2 additionally adjusted for baseline data.

Significant parameters are given in bold.

Finally, higher dietary scores at baseline predicted higher consumption at follow-up for both dietary scores.

The proportions of variance explained by parenting practices for the baseline measurements are 8 % for the F&V score and 12 % for the excess score. The proportions of variance explained for the follow-up measurements are 5 % and 10 %, respectively.

Attrition analyses

Significant differences were found between the participants included in the present analyses and the baseline only or excluded participants for six of the twelve variables. Those who did not participate at follow-up or were excluded were less likely to have a mother with higher education and had a higher excess score. Their mothers not only had a higher excess and a lower F&V score, but also reported less negotiation and more permissiveness. No gender difference was found.

Discussion

In the present study, a longitudinal design is used to identify parenting practices during childhood that predict intake of healthy and less healthy patterns during childhood and, in future, during adolescence.

The results indicate a consistent association between mothers’ and their children’s intake for both the F&V score and the excess score indicating that children and adolescents’ diets are associated with the foods eaten by their parents.

The present study also explored the role of availability. Availability of fruit seemed not to be a problem: 95 % had mostly or always fruit available at home. Congruent with a study by Larson et al., the availability of less healthy foods and beverages was not only positively associated with the excess score but also negatively with the F&V score(Reference Larson, Neumark-Sztainer and Harnack22), suggesting that parents should be encouraged to reduce the availability of less healthy foods. In a study of 12-year-olds by Haerens et al.(Reference Haerens, Craeynest and Deforche14), the availability of unhealthy foods was related to fat intake and soft drink consumption in boys, but not in girls; however, no association was found with the consumption of fruit.

Further, we investigated the role of food parenting practices on children’s and adolescents’ dietary intake. After taking into account all other factors, especially permissiveness, and to a lesser extent, negotiation seemed to be promising factors. Letting children decide on what they eat and when, and allowing them to consume sweets and soft drinks when they like was significantly detrimental to their future intake of less healthy foods. These results are in line with the findings of De Bourdeaudhuij(Reference De Bourdeaudhuij7): they also found a positive relationship between permissiveness in their family at the age of 10 years and the consumption of more fat and sweet foods during adolescence and no association with healthy foods. In contrast, other studies, however, found that strict parental control may have adverse effects such as increasing children’s preference for and intake of restricted foods(Reference Fisher and Birch23, Reference Fisher and Birch24).

Strengths and limitations

The first limitation of the present study is the use of brief scales to report very complex behaviours. In addition, the parenting practice scales differ from those used in the current literature and have not been validated. However, at the start of the present study, little research had been conducted in this area. The second limitation is the rather crude dietary intake assessment. We only asked about consumption frequency in d/week, and therefore no information on portion size is collected. Third, both dietary intake and parenting scales are based on self report and therefore might be responded to in a socially desirable way. The fourth limitation is the selective dropout, which compromises the generalizability of the results. A strength of the present study is the prospective character and the rather large sample despite the considerable attrition. Nonetheless, causality can still not be stated as prospective relationships can, as for cross-sectional studies, be due to a third antecedent.

Conclusion

These results extend previous ones obtained from cross-sectional studies and give further support to the importance of including the family in prevention campaigns aimed at children and adolescents. Parents’ intake and restricting the availability of unhealthy foods not only appeared to have a consistent impact on children’s and adolescents’ diet, but also less permissive food-related parenting practices may improve adolescents’ diet.

Acknowledgements

Sources of funding: The first and second surveys were funded by the Ghent University (Fund no. 011/105/04). Carine Vereecken and Leen Haerens are postdoctoral researchers supported by the FWO Flanders. The study has been facilitated by the EU-funded HOPE project: ‘Health-promotion through Obesity Prevention across Europe (the Commission of the European Communities, SP5A-CT-2006-044128). The study does not necessarily reflect the Commission’s views and in no way anticipates the Commission’s future policy in this area. Conflicts of interest declaration: The authors have no conflict of interest. C.V. performed most of the statistical analyses and the writing of the manuscript. Authorship responsibilities: L.M. and I.D.B. supervised the study. All authors advised on the study design and interpretation of the results and reviewed and approved the manuscript.

References

1.Nicklas, TA, Baranowski, T, Baranowski, JC et al. (2001) Family and child-care provider influences on preschool children’s fruit, juice, and vegetable consumption. Nutr Rev 59, 224235.Google Scholar
2.Crockett, SJ, Mullis, RM & Perry, CL (1988) Parent nutrition education: a conceptual model. J Sch Health 58, 5357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Hearn, M, Baranowski, T & Baranowski, J (1998) Environmental influences on dietary behavior among children: availability and accessibility of fruits and vegetables enable consumption. J Health Educ 29, 2632.Google Scholar
4.Birch, LL & Fisher, JO (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, 539549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Rittenberry, L et al. (2001) Child-reported family and peer influences on fruit, juice and vegetable consumption: reliability and validity of measures. Health Educ Res 16, 187200.Google Scholar
6.Faith, MS, Scanlon, KS, Birch, LL et al. (2004) Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res 12, 17111722.Google Scholar
7.De Bourdeaudhuij, I (1997) Family food rules and healthy eating in adolescents. J Health Psychol 2, 4556.Google Scholar
8.Young, EM, Fors, SW & Hayes, DM (2004) Associations between perceived parent behaviors and middle school student fruit and vegetable consumption. J Nutr Educ Behav 36, 28.Google Scholar
9.Hanson, NI, Neumark-Sztainer, D, Eisenberg, ME et al. (2005) Associations between parental report of the home food environment and adolescent intakes of fruits, vegetables and dairy foods. Public Health Nutr 8, 7785.Google Scholar
10.Arcan, C, Neumark-Sztainer, D, Hannan, P et al. (2007) Parental eating behaviours, home food environment and adolescent intakes of fruits, vegetables and dairy foods: longitudinal findings from Project EAT. Public Health Nutr 10, 12571265.Google Scholar
11.Neumark-Sztainer, D, Story, M, Perry, C et al. (1999) Factors influencing food choices of adolescents: findings from focus-group discussions with adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 99, 929937.Google Scholar
12.van der Horst, K, Kremers, S, Ferreira, I et al. (2007) Perceived parenting style and practices and the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages by adolescents. Health Educ Res 22, 295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.de Bruijn, GJ, Kremers, SP, de Vries, H et al. (2007) Associations of social-environmental and individual-level factors with adolescent soft drink consumption: results from the SMILE study. Health Educ Res 22, 227237.Google Scholar
14.Haerens, L, Craeynest, M, Deforche, B et al. (2008) The contribution of psychosocial and home environmental factors in explaining eating behaviours in adolescents. Eur J Clin Nutr 62, 5159.Google Scholar
15.Johnson, S (2004) Intake of fruit and vegetables and risk of stroke: an overview. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care 7, 665670.Google Scholar
16.Liu, S, Manson, JE, Lee, IM et al. (2000) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of cardiovascular disease: the Women’s Health Study. Am J Clin Nutr 72, 922928.Google Scholar
17.Ness, AR & Powles, JW (1997) Fruit and vegetables, and cardiovascular disease: a review. Int J Epidemiol 26, 113.Google Scholar
18.Ludwig, DS, Peterson, KE & Gortmaker, SL (2001) Relation between consumption of sugar-sweetened drinks and childhood obesity: a prospective, observational analysis. Lancet 357, 505508.Google Scholar
19.Vereecken, C, Rossi, S, Giacchi, M et al. (2008) Comparison of a short food frequency questionnaire and derived indices with a seven-day diet record in Belgian and Italian children. Int J Public Health 53, 297305.Google Scholar
20.Vereecken, C, Keukelier, E & Maes, L (2004) Influence of mother’s educational level on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite 43, 93103.Google Scholar
21.Rasbash, J, Charlton, C, Browne, WJ et al. (2005) MLwiN Version 2.02. Bristol: Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol.Google Scholar
22.Larson, NI, Neumark-Sztainer, DR, Harnack, LJ et al. (2008) Fruit and vegetable intake correlates during the transition to young adulthood. Am J Prev Med 35, 3337.Google Scholar
23.Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (1999) Restricting access to palatable foods affects children’s behavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr 69, 12641272.Google Scholar
24.Fisher, JO & Birch, LL (1999) Restricting access to foods and children’s eating. Appetite 32, 405419.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Response at school and individual levels – Longitudinal Eating and Activity study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005

Figure 1

Table 1 Descriptives of dependent and independent variables: LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 609)

Figure 2

Table 2 Correlations between standardized dependent and independent variables – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 609)

Figure 3

Table 3 Associations of baseline factors with the F&V score at baseline and follow-up, 4 years later – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 600)

Figure 4

Table 4 Associations of baseline factors with the excess score at baseline and follow-up, 4 years later – LEA study, East and West Flanders, Belgium, 2002–2005 (n 530)