Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T10:45:00.411Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pathways to health: A longitudinal examination of protective factors in children with and without preschool anxiety

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2024

Esmeralda Navarro*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Megan Davis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Sophia Martin
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Lauren V. Butler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Helen Egger
Affiliation:
Little Otter, Inc., San Francisco, CA, USA
Kimberly L.H. Carpenter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
William E. Copeland
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA
Margaret A. Sheridan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
*
Corresponding author: Esmeralda Navarro; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Preschool anxiety is highly prevalent and well known to predict risk for future psychopathology. The present study explores whether a diagnosis of an anxiety disorder in preschool interacts with (a) social skills and (b) cognitive ability to longitudinally predict psychopathology, two well-known protective factors, among a sample of 207 children measured at preschool (Mage = 4.34 years) and early childhood (Mage = 6.61 years). To assess social skills and cognitive ability, we utilized the Social Skills Rating Scale and the Differential Abilities Scale, respectively. To assess psychopathology, we utilized the parent report of the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment. Hierarchical linear regression models revealed significant interactions between both social skills and cognitive ability with preschool anxiety. We observed that social skills protected against emergent psychopathology for both children with and without anxiety, although this association was stronger for children with preschool anxiety. Contrastingly, cognitive ability served as a protective factor against future psychopathology primarily among children without preschool anxiety. Results from this study identify targets for future intervention and inform our understanding of how preschool anxiety, a common disorder among young children, shapes future psychopathology risk in childhood.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Early onset psychopathology has long-lasting effects, with impairment continuing into adolescence and adulthood (Finsaas et al., Reference Finsaas, Kessel, Dougherty, Bufferd, Danzig, Davila, Carlson and Klein2020; Reef et al., Reference Reef, van Meurs, Verhulst and van der Ende2010). Anxiety disorders, in particular, are among the most common disorders present in early childhood (Costello et al., Reference Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler and Angold2003). Childhood anxiety disorders not only predict future anxiety but also other forms of psychopathology, including conduct disorder, depression, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (Bittner et al., Reference Bittner, Egger, Erkanli, Jane Costello, Foley and Angold2007; Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Shanahan, Costello and Angold2009, Reference Copeland, Angold, Shanahan and Costello2014). Epidemiology studies examining the prevalence of childhood anxiety have reported that symptoms of anxiety can arise as early as preschool age, with 19% of preschool children meeting the criteria for an anxiety disorder (Franz et al., Reference Franz, Angold, Copeland, Costello, Towe-Goodman and Egger2013). Early preschool anxiety disorders and their symptoms are highly distressing for both children and their families; these anxiety symptoms are also some of the first signs of psychopathology observed by caregivers (Towe-Goodman et al., Reference Towe-Goodman, Franz, Copeland, Angold and Egger2014). Yet, preschool anxiety largely goes untreated (Chavira et al., Reference Chavira, Stein, Bailey and Stein2004). One reason for this lack of treatment may be because the preschool period is a time of rapid development along almost every dimension, including physical and mental (Poulou, Reference Poulou2015). This rapid pace of development makes it difficult to identify enduring risk relative to stochastic variation in emotional reactivity, leading to disorders in early childhood being considered less consequential than those identified in adolescence and adulthood (Egger & Angold, Reference Egger and Angold2006). However, given that early onset anxiety is a transdiagnostic risk factor for multiple forms of psychopathology, identifying pathways of risk and resilience from this early period is imperative. A better understanding of how risk and protective factors function in early childhood, in interaction with existing risk processes such as the presence of a disorder like anxiety, can help with novel intervention development and more targeted application of positive interventions.

There is substantial literature identifying the characteristics, skills, and assets that are beneficial for overall childhood development. Among these, social skills and cognitive ability have continuously emerged as important early intervention targets for children, as they are essential developmental tasks for this age group (Masten et al., Reference Masten, Burt and Coatsworth2015). These two factors also serve as protective factors (i.e., factors that predict positive outcomes; Masten et al., Reference Masten, Cutuli, Herbers, Reed, Lopez and Snyder2009). Multiple research studies have documented the positive impacts possessing strong social skills and cognitive skills have on development. However, to date, little research has examined whether preschool anxiety impacts the positive associations observed between well-established protective factors and psychopathology (Crews et al., Reference Crews, Bender, Cook, Gresham, Kern and Vanderwood2007; Huber et al., Reference Huber, Plötner and Schmitz2019a). Accordingly, the current study seeks to evaluate the potential that these two well-established protective factors will function similarly for children with and without preschool anxiety.

Social skills and psychopathology

Social competence is a broad term used to describe the degree to which one effectively navigates social interactions and uses appropriate means to achieve personal and social goals (Huber et al., Reference Huber, Plötner and Schmitz2019b). Social skills, which include behaviors such as cooperation, assertion, and responsibility, enable individuals to display social competence and successfully interact with others in their environment. Importantly, developing social competence is necessary for normative development. As children move from childhood into adulthood and form relationships outside of the primary caregiver household, social skills become increasingly important. They aid in the acquisition of social acceptance, network formation, and social support. Among other outcomes, these are key factors in mitigating the effects of future stressors (Thompson & Goodvin, Reference Thompson and Goodvin2016).

Consistent with this important developmental role, early social impairment is considered a risk factor for psychopathology. Cross-sectional studies document associations between social skill deficits and psychological disorders transdiagnostically (Dupaul et al., Reference Dupaul, Mcgoey, Eckert and Vanbrakle2001; Gadow & Nolan, Reference Gadow and Nolan2002; Halls et al., Reference Halls, Cooper and Creswell2015). Multiple theoretical models posit that poor social skills contribute to the development of psychopathology and are central to the etiology and maintenance of multiple disorders (Huber et al., Reference Huber, Plötner and Schmitz2019b). For children with anxiety disorders specifically, poor social skills can both give rise to and be a consequence of anxiety. Communication difficulties at age 7, for example, have been found to predict social anxiety symptoms at age 13 (Pickard et al., Reference Pickard, Rijsdijk, Happé and Mandy2017). Similarly, lower social competence scores at age four are associated with more internalizing and externalizing behaviors at age 10 and 14, respectively (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Hahn and Haynes2010).

While social deficits are associated with psychopathology, social skills can be protective against poor well-being outcomes. In the face of adversity, for example, children with strong social skills show better academic, work, and relationship outcomes (Mihalec-Adkins & Cooley, Reference Mihalec-Adkins and Cooley2020). Similarly, social skills moderate the relationship between stress and depression, displaying a protective effect for children undergoing life stressors (Jaureguizar et al., Reference Jaureguizar, Garaigordobil and Bernaras2018). Further, possessing strong social skills has positive downstream consequences, as stronger social skills aid in the development of social networks that protect against psychopathology (Thompson & Goodvin, Reference Thompson and Goodvin2016). In summary, current evidence indicates that strong social skills in childhood protect against emergent psychopathology and increase future well-being. How social skills mitigate risk for children with preschool anxiety, a group of individuals who may already struggle in this domain, has yet to be tested.

Cognitive ability and psychopathology

Cognitive ability, a heterogeneous construct that includes memory, logical reasoning, attention, and abstract thinking, is necessary for day-to-day functioning. The ability to use critical reasoning is required for successfully achieving goal-directed behavior and meeting task demands. As observed with social skills, deficits in cognitive ability are associated with risk for psychopathology. For example, a global measure of cognitive ability, IQ, is negatively associated with psychopathology (Castellanos-Ryan et al., Reference Castellanos-Ryan, Brière, O’Leary-Barrett, Banaschewski, Bokde, Bromberg, Büchel, Flor, Frouin, Gallinat, Garavan, Martinot, Nees, Paus, Pausova, Rietschel, Smolka, Robbins, Whelan, Schumann and Conrod2016; Mahony et al., Reference Mahony, Tu, Rau, Liu, Lalonde, Alexander-Bloch, Satterthwaite, Shinohara, Bassett, Milham and Raznahan2023). In addition, longitudinal studies document negative associations between childhood IQ and adult psychopathology (Koenen et al., Reference Koenen, Moffitt, Roberts, Martin, Kubzansky, Harrington, Poulton and Caspi2009). Deficits in other measures of cognitive ability, including working memory, cognitive control, and language, are also associated with increased transdiagnostic psychopathology risk among children and adults (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2020).

Like social skills, cognitive ability is a protective factor in the context of high risk. High IQ, for example, has been found to protect against psychopathology among children who have experienced adversity (Batty et al., Reference Batty, Mortensen and Osler2005; Koenen et al., Reference Koenen, Moffitt, Roberts, Martin, Kubzansky, Harrington, Poulton and Caspi2009). Individuals with stronger cognitive abilities, as measured by aspects of cognitive control including inhibitory control and attention, show associations with better relative well-being outcomes in the context of environmental risk (Bardeen et al., Reference Bardeen, Stevens, Clark, Lahti and Cropsey2015; Maciejewski et al., Reference Maciejewski, Brieant, Lee, King-Casas and Kim-Spoon2020; Wade et al., Reference Wade, Wright and Finegold2022). Given this literature, cognitive abilities may allow for adaptive functioning despite risk through an increased ability to problem-solve, manage, and mitigate challenges. Thus, cognition may serve as a valuable resource for children to rely on when faced with stressors that increase the risk of psychopathology. In the context of anxiety, findings between IQ and anxiety are mixed, with a body of research finding positive associations between IQ and anxiety symptoms in adults (Penney et al., Reference Penney, Miedema and Mazmanian2015). The role cognitive ability plays in the development of psychopathology in young children with and without anxiety, however, remains untested.

The present study

Identifying whether well-known risk and protective factors like social skills and cognition function equality in the context of early anxiety risk using longitudinal designs is a primary way current research can contribute to identifying targets for early intervention efforts. Despite strong evidence that social skills and cognition may serve as protective factors, research has not investigated whether these protective factors are disrupted by the presence of a preschool anxiety disorder. Here we explore the impact of preschool anxiety diagnosis on future risk for psychopathology across early childhood and examine whether social skills and cognitive ability, factors known to protect against psychopathology risk in other populations, mitigate the risk conferred by early anxiety diagnosis. We hypothesized that preschool anxiety would be associated with transdiagnostic risk for psychopathology across early childhood but that this association would interact with social skills and cognitive ability. Specifically, we predicted that social skills and cognitive ability would moderate the association between preschool anxiety and later psychopathology symptoms, such that higher levels of social skills and cognitive ability would be more protective for children with preschool anxiety compared to those without preschool anxiety. Given the high prevalence of preschool-onset anxiety, its association with impairment and distress in the preschool period, and its potential for impairment into adulthood, it is imperative to identify potential protective factors to best support children and their families who experience anxiety early in development.

Methods

Participants and procedures

Participants were originally part of the Duke Preschool Anxiety Study. Children from the Duke Preschool Anxiety Study were recruited from local primary care clinics in Durham, North Carolina. Caregivers completed a brief child anxiety screening questionnaire which was used to create an initial sample enhanced for anxiety. A total of 917 caregivers and their children completed an initial home visit. During this visit, caregivers completed a semi-structured interview of child psychopathology (psychopathology at Time 1). A subset (n = 502) of the 917 children were recruited to participate in a laboratory visit where children completed measures on cognitive ability and parents reported on social skills. The laboratory visit study utilized a case–control design in which 251 children had at least one anxiety disorder (generalized anxiety, social phobia, or separation anxiety); the other 251 children in the laboratory visit were age and gender matched to the anxiety sample. Following the lab visit, participants completed a secondary psychopathology measure (psychopathology at time two). Of the subset, 207 had usable cognitive ability or social skills data from the laboratory visit; thus, the present analysis is comprised of these N = 207 participants. Children in the analysis sample (N = 207) were more likely to be older in age, t (914) = 7.23, p < .001 than the full sample (N = 917). There were also more Black (χ 2 = 6.47, p < .011) and less Asian (χ 2 = 4.96, p < .026) participants in the analysis sample than the full sample. Additional details about recruitment and enrollment procedures for all phases of the data have been detailed elsewhere (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Baranek, Copeland, Compton, Zucker, Dawson and Egger2019; Franz et al., Reference Franz, Angold, Copeland, Costello, Towe-Goodman and Egger2013). All study procedures were approved by Duke University’s Institutional Review Board. A reliance agreement between the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and Duke University was established prior to data analysis.

Measures

Anxiety and psychopathology

Psychopathology at Time 1 and Time 2 was measured via caregiver report on the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA; Egger & Angold, Reference Egger and Angold2004). During the PAPA interviews, caregivers reported on children’s symptoms of psychological disorders present in the past 3 months. Caregivers provided information about the onset, duration, and level of impairment of symptoms at both timepoints. Based on this information, interviewers assigned a score of 0 (not present) or 1 (present) for each symptom. Preschool anxiety was defined as the presence or absence of an anxiety disorder at Time 1, in accordance with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-IV. Psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 were summed across the following disorders to calculate an overall psychopathology symptom score for each participant: depression, generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, oppositional defiant disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and conduct disorder. Higher symptom scores are indicative of a greater number of symptoms. We chose to assess symptoms of psychopathology generally, as previous research indicates that early anxiety is a transdiagnostic risk factor for multiple disorders (Reef et. al., 2009). To test the robustness of effects, separate analyses were also conducted with an internalizing (depression, separation anxiety, generalized anxiety, social phobia) and externalizing (oppositional defiance disorder, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder) symptom score.

Social skills

Social skills were measured using the social skills scale of the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) at Time 2 (Gresham & Elliott, Reference Gresham and Elliott1990). The SSRS includes 38 items asking caregivers to report on their child’s social skills and problem behavior. The social skills scale is composed of four subscales: assertion, responsibility, cooperation, and self-control. Items are answered on a 3-point Likert scale (“never,” “sometimes,” and “very often”). Higher scores are indicative of greater social skills. In this sample, the reliability of the SSRS was good (alpha = 0.87). The SSRS has been previously used in children as young as 3 years old (Maleki et al., Reference Maleki, Mardani, Mitra Chehrzad, Dianatinasab and Vaismoradi2019; Mohamed, Reference Mohamed2018; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Sandall, Davis and Thomas2011).

Cognitive ability

Cognitive ability was measured using the Differential Ability Scales at Time 2 (Elliott, Reference Elliott1990). The Differential Ability Scales is comprised of three indicator scores: verbal ability, nonverbal reasoning, and spatial ability. The verbal ability scale reflects oral processing and language expression and consists of the Word Definitions and Similarities subtests. The nonverbal reasoning index reflects complex reasoning and abstract thinking abilities and consists of the Matrix Reasoning and Quantitative Reasoning subtests. The spatial ability index reflects the perception of spatial orientation and consists of the Recall of Designs and Pattern Construction subtests. Scores across the three indices are used to obtain a measure of General Conceptual Ability. The Differential Ability Scales can be administered to children as young as 2.5 years old and has previously been used to examine associations with psychopathology in preschool and early childhood (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Guthrie, Coffing and Lord2011; Hughes & McIntosh, Reference Hughes and McIntosh2002; Reddy et al., Reference Reddy, Braunstein and Dumont2008).

Data analytic plan

All statistical analyses were conducted in the SPSS program version 28. Data was missing in 5.83% of cases on average across variables (range of missing 0%–10.6%). Little’s MCAR test was not significant χ 2 = 759.92, p = .063, indicating that data was missing completely at random. Thus, we used multiple imputation to account for missing data. Multiple imputation has been validated to produce unbiased estimates for analyzing data missing at random (Schafer & Graham, Reference Schafer and Graham2002). Multiple imputation was conducted using fully conditional specification, an iterative Markov chain Monte Carlo procedure. In this procedure, a model including all other variables is created for each variable to predict imputed values; multiple datasets using this procedure were created over 20 iterations.

Following multiple imputation, stepwise linear regression analyses were used to examine the contributions of social skills, preschool anxiety, and cognition to psychopathology at time 2 (between 6 and 9 years of age). Three models were constructed. Model 1 was entered in three steps: (a) demographic variables and covariates (age, time between study visits, sex at birth, parent-reported race, parent-reported caregiver education, and parent-reported household composition) and preschool anxiety, (b) social skills, and (c) the interaction term of preschool anxiety and social skills. Model 2 was similarly entered in three steps: (a) demographics and preschool anxiety, (b) cognitive ability, and (c) the interaction term of preschool anxiety and cognitive ability. Model 3 tested the three-way interaction between cognitive ability, social skills, and preschool anxiety, and was entered using four steps: (a) demographics and preschool anxiety, (b) social skills and cognitive ability, (c) both interaction terms, and (d) the three-way interaction between social skills, cognitive ability, and preschool anxiety. Total psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 were entered as the dependent variable in all models. In Model 3, the three-way interaction was not significant; thus, we do not present results from this model in the main text (Supplement Table 1).

Table 1. Cohort description

Note. PAPA = Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment.

We opted to have a binary anxiety variable predict a continuous psychopathology outcome, as we were interested in exploring how meeting diagnostic criteria for preschool anxiety diagnosis impacts the future experience of psychopathology symptoms. Additional sensitivity analyses were conducted using (a) internalizing symptoms as the dependent variable, (b) externalizing symptoms as the dependent variable, (c) total number of disorders as the dependent variable, and (4) a continuous preschool anxiety symptom variable as the predictor.

Results

Bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics

Demographic variables are presented in Table 1. The mean age of children (54.6 % female, 45.4% male) in this sample at the time of the first psychopathology assessment (Time 1) was 4.34 years old (2.00–6.17). The mean age of children at the time of the second psychopathology assessment (Time 2) was 6.61 years old (6.00–8.75). The mean time between Time 1 and Time 2 was 2.26 years (0.17–5.58). Eighty children (38.6%) met the criteria for an anxiety disorder in preschool at Time 1, whereas 127 children (61.4%) did not meet the criteria. Parents of children in the sample identified their children as Black (55.6%), White (39.1%), Asian or Asian-American (1%), Native American (3.9%), or another race (0.5%). Caregiver education was coded as 0 or 1 to indicate the presence or absence of at least one caregiver in the household with a 4-year college education. Similarly, household composition was coded as 0 or 1 to indicate the presence or absence of a two-caregiver household. Most children in this study came from a two-caregiver household (62.8%). Slightly less than half of caregivers in our sample (42.7%) had received a 4-year college education.

Tables 2 and 3 present bivariate correlations and descriptive statistics among cognitive ability, social skills, and total psychopathology symptoms. In this sample, psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 were negatively correlated with cognitive ability and social skills. Cognitive ability was not significantly correlated with social skills (r = .06).

Table 2. Bivariate correlations

*p < .05, **p < .01.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics among variables of interest

Hierarchical regression analysis results

Results from Model 1 are presented in Table 4. The first step, with demographic variables, covariates, and preschool anxiety entered, was statistically significant (F = 3.61, p = .001) with an R2 of 0.121, indicating that the entered variables accounted for 12.1% of the total variance in psychopathology symptoms at Time 2. In the second step, social skills accounted for an additional 18.2% of the variance (F = 9.87, p < .001; R2 = 0.303). Finally, in the last step, the interaction between preschool anxiety and social skills was entered; this interaction term accounted for an additional 11.3% of the total variance in psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 (F = 14.35, p < .001; R2 = .416). In this final step, the interaction between social skills and preschool anxiety was statistically significant (B = .062, p = <.001). Examination of simple slopes revealed that preschool anxiety was significantly associated with total psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 when social skills were one standard deviation below the mean (B = 0.252, p = .005). When social skills were one standard deviation above the mean, this association was not significant (B = 0.109, p = 0.199). Figure 1 plots the simple slopes of these interactions.

Figure 1. Social skills moderate the association between preschool anxiety diagnosis and childhood psychopathology symptoms.

Table 4. Social skills regression model

Note. Household = household composition; parent ed = parent education; SS = social skills; PA = preschool anxiety.

The bold values are in the p column, signifying their significant value. For example, this value was significant, p = 0.025.

Results from Model 2 are presented in Table 5. The first step, with demographic variables, covariates, and preschool anxiety entered, was statistically significant (F = 3.61, p = .001) with an R 2 of 0.121, indicating that the entered variables accounted for 12.1% of the total variance in psychopathology symptoms at Time 2. In the second step, cognitive ability accounted for an additional 0.5% of the variance (F = 3.29, p = .002; R2 = 0.126). Finally, in the last step, the interaction between preschool anxiety and cognitive ability was entered; this interaction term accounted for an addition 24.4% of the total variance in psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 (F = 11.83, p < .001; R2 = 0.370). In this final step, the interaction between cognitive ability and preschool anxiety was statistically significant (B = .075, p = <.001). Examination of simple slopes revealed that preschool anxiety was significantly associated with total psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 when cognitive ability was one standard deviation above the mean (B = 0.312, p = .001). When cognitive ability was one standard deviation below the mean, this association was not significant (B = 0.135, p = 0.161). Figure 2 plots the simple slopes of these interactions.

Figure 2. Cognitive ability moderates the association between preschool anxiety diagnosis and childhood psychopathology symptoms.

Table 5. Cognitive ability regression model

Note. Household = household composition; parent ed = parent education; CA = cognitive ability; PA = preschool anxiety.

Sensitivity analyses

When internalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms, and the total number of disorders at Time 2 were entered as the dependent variable, the findings were identical to the main findings presented (Supplement Tables 25; 89); thus, only results exploring associations with total psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 are presented. When anxiety symptoms, measured continuously, were used in our model, only main effect associations remained consistent (Supplement Tables 67).

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine whether preschool anxiety predicted transdiagnostic risk for psychopathology in early childhood and whether this risk was mitigated by well-known protective factors: social skills and cognitive ability, which promote positive development in adulthood and adolescence. Here, we use a longitudinal design to examine associations between early anxiety, protective factors, and psychopathology across time. Consistent with previous work, we observed that meeting the criteria for an anxiety disorder in preschool longitudinally increased the risk for both internalizing and externalizing symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhood. In addition, we observed that having high social skills protected against emergent psychopathology for both children with and without anxiety; this association was stronger for children with preschool anxiety. In contrast, higher cognitive ability was associated with fewer psychopathology symptoms for children without preschool anxiety only. Together, these findings demonstrate that preschool anxiety is a risk factor for the development of psychopathology in childhood, and while protective factors are important avenues for intervention in ameliorating the development of psychopathology across early childhood, their impact may differ by this existing vulnerability.

Social skills moderated the association between preschool anxiety and later psychopathology. Specifically, having higher social skills was associated with fewer psychopathology symptoms for both children with and without an anxiety disorder during preschool. Having poorer social skills, however, was more strongly associated with greater psychopathology symptoms in children with an anxiety disorder. These results are consistent with other longitudinal evidence indicating that children who have stronger social skills experience fewer psychopathology symptoms later in life, with previous studies showing these associations lasting into adolescence and adulthood (Bornstein et al., Reference Bornstein, Hahn and Haynes2010; Burt et al., Reference Burt, Obradović, Long and Masten2008; Jones et al., Reference Jones, Greenberg and Crowley2015). Possessing social skills early in development sets the stage for positive behavioral and emotional functioning. Because positive social behaviors are reinforced by others, employing social skills increases engagement in positive experiences and allows individuals to gain social support from those around them. Children who show difficulty using social skills, contrastingly, may not receive this reinforcement, resulting in fewer positive experiences and increased difficulty forming relationships necessary for psychological well-being. For children with anxiety, a disorder characterized by excessive worry and fear, social skills may be especially helpful, as garnering positive social experiences may counteract anxiety-driven cognitions and worries.

Social skills interventions have been found to be effective in reducing psychopathology among children. A meta-analysis of intervention programs aimed at enhancing the social skills of children and adolescents reported that participants demonstrated significant increases in positive social behaviors and academic achievement, and significant decreases in problem behaviors after receiving social skills training (Durlak et al., Reference Durlak, Weissberg and Pachan2010). Other randomized control trials (Flook et al., Reference Flook, Goldberg, Pinger and Davidson2014; Schonert-Reichl et al., Reference Schonert-Reichl, Oberle, Lawlor, Abbott, Thomson, Oberlander and Diamond2015) show similar improvements. Our study demonstrates that associations between social skills and emergent psychopathology are present as early as 6 years of age and point to the opportunity for early intervention to mitigate the progression of psychopathology even in early childhood. Further, in our study, the protective role of social skills extended across both internalizing and externalizing disorders. While a meta-analysis investigating longitudinal associations between social skills and psychopathology reported stronger evidence for externalizing symptoms versus internalizing symptoms (Huber et al., Reference Huber, Plötner and Schmitz2019b), our results suggest that in early childhood, social skills are protective across both dimensions of psychopathology and in particular for children with early internalizing symptoms.

Cognitive ability moderated the association between preschool anxiety and total psychopathology symptoms. Higher cognitive ability was associated with fewer psychopathology symptoms at Time 2 among children without preschool anxiety only, a finding reported in older children and adults (Blok et al., Reference Blok, Schuurmans, Tijburg, Hillegers, Koopman-Verhoeff, Muetzel, Tiemeier and White2022). Cognitive ability encompasses multiple domains, including working memory, verbal language, and perceptive reasoning. These skills are needed for self-regulation and goal-directed behavior, processes that are disrupted in many psychological disorders (Strauman, Reference Strauman2017). The relationship observed among children without anxiety is therefore consistent with previous literature finding associations between cognitive deficits and psychopathology (Abramovitch et al., Reference Abramovitch, Short and Schweiger2021).

Interestingly, and contrary to our hypothesis, having higher cognitive ability was not protective for children with preschool anxiety. While it is theorized that cognitive ability results in greater capacity to employ the skills needed for self-regulation and goal-directed behavior, cognitive ability may not serve this function for children with preschool anxiety. Instead, increased cognitive ability may contribute to or exacerbate anxiety symptoms. Children with increased cognitive ability may be better able to understand complex situations, remember events with greater detail, and form conclusions about these events. For children with anxiety, this greater understanding of their environment might result in more rumination and worry. Among adults with generalized anxiety disorder, for example, worry and rumination over past events have been positively correlated with cognitive ability (Coplan et al., Reference Coplan, Hodulik, Mathew, Mao, Hof, Gorman and Shungu2012; Penney et al., Reference Penney, Miedema and Mazmanian2015). Since worry and rumination are cognitive processes, individuals with higher cognitive ability may also have more cognitive resources to engage in maladaptive thinking, increasing the risk for further psychopathology. Future research, however, is needed to identify the mechanisms underlying these associations.

Of note, when we conducted our analyses using a continuous measure of preschool anxiety (i.e., number of anxiety symptoms), we did not find any significant interactions between social skills and cognitive ability. This difference in results points to the importance of diagnostic evaluation at the preschool age. While some symptoms of anxiety are considered a normal part of development and may be exhibited by many children, not all will exhibit the impairment and duration necessary to meet the criteria for an anxiety diagnosis (Muris et al., Reference Muris, Merckelbach, Mayer and Prins2000). Our findings support that identifying those who do meet the criteria for an anxiety disorder in preschool is imperative to understanding how other individual-level factors ameliorate or exacerbate future psychopathology symptoms.

Several limitations should be noted. Social skills and cognition were not tested during the first timepoint in our study, and thus, we are unable to examine how these protective factors changed between preschoolage and age 6. Measuring potential protective factors during the preschool age would allow for a greater understanding of how these skills develop longitudinally and are shaped by emerging psychopathology. Similarly, it may also allow us to examine whether, and how, social skills and cognitive ability contribute to the development of psychopathology. Further, while we chose to focus on preschool anxiety as a moderator of psychopathology outcomes due to its documented impact on families in the first few years of a child’s life, future research may consider exploring how other forms of psychopathology moderate the relationship between protective factors and future outcomes.

Conclusion

The present study explored how social skills and cognitive ability interact with preschool anxiety to shape risk for psychopathology. Our findings identify that, while social skills serve as a protective factor for children with preschool anxiety, cognitive ability functions differentially among children with and without preschool anxiety. These results inform our understanding of how important developmental processes, such as developing social skills and strong cognitive abilities, interact with preschool anxiety to shape future psychopathology. Given that early intervention is essential for mitigating risk, fostering the development of potential protective factors as early as the preschool age serves as an important avenue of interventions for young children, particularly among those with preschool anxiety.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001652.

Funding statement

This study was funded by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health grants R01MH120314 (to MAS, WC, and KC), R01MH115004 (to MAS), R01MH081025 (to HE), and R01MH075766 (HE).

Competing interests

Dr Egger has reported being the cofounder and chief medical and scientific officer of Little Otter Health.

References

Abramovitch, A., Short, T., & Schweiger, A. (2021). The C factor: Cognitive dysfunction as a transdiagnostic dimension in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardeen, J. R., Stevens, E. N., Clark, C. B., Lahti, A. C., & Cropsey, K. L. (2015). Cognitive risk profiles for anxiety disorders in a high-risk population. Psychiatry Research, 229(0), 572576. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.05.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batty, G. D., Mortensen, E. L., & Osler, M. (2005). Childhood IQ in relation to later psychiatric disorder: Evidence from a Danish birth cohort study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 187(2), 180181. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.187.2.180 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, S. L., Guthrie, W., Coffing, M., & Lord, C. (2011). Convergent validity of the mullen scales of early learning and the differential ability scales in children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal On Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116(5), 331343. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-116.5.331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bittner, A., Egger, H. L., Erkanli, A., Jane Costello, E., Foley, D. L., & Angold, A. (2007). What do childhood anxiety disorders predict? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 11741183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01812.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blok, E., Schuurmans, I. K., Tijburg, A. J., Hillegers, M., Koopman-Verhoeff, M. E., Muetzel, R. L., Tiemeier, H., & White, T. (2022). Cognitive performance in children and adolescents with psychopathology traits: A cross-sectional multicohort study in the general population. Development and Psychopathology, 35(2), 926940. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579422000165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Haynes, O. M. (2010). Social competence, externalizing, and internalizing behavioral adjustment from early childhood through early adolescence: Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology, 22(4), 717735. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000416 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burt, K. B., Obradović, J., Long, J. D., & Masten, A. S. (2008). The interplay of social competence and psychopathology Over 20 Years: Testing transactional and cascade models. Child Development, 79(2), 359374. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01130.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, K. L. H., Baranek, G. T., Copeland, W. E., Compton, S., Zucker, N., Dawson, G., & Egger, H. L. (2019). Sensory over-responsivity: An early risk factor for anxiety and behavioral challenges in young children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 10751088. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0502-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castellanos-Ryan, N., Brière, F. N., O’Leary-Barrett, M., Banaschewski, T., Bokde, A., Bromberg, U., Büchel, C., Flor, H., Frouin, V., Gallinat, J., Garavan, H., Martinot, J.-L., Nees, F., Paus, T., Pausova, Z., Rietschel, M., Smolka, M. N., Robbins, T. W., Whelan, R., Schumann, G., Conrod, P., & The IMAGEN Consortium (2016). The structure of psychopathology in adolescence and its common personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(8), 10391052. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000193 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chavira, D. A., Stein, M. B., Bailey, K., & Stein, M. T. (2004). Child anxiety in primary care: Prevalent but untreated. Depression and Anxiety, 20(4), 155164. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20039 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Angold, A., Shanahan, L., & Costello, E. J. (2014). Longitudinal patterns of anxiety from childhood to adulthood: The great smoky mountains study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(1), 2133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Shanahan, L., Costello, E. J., & Angold, A. (2009). Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 764772. https://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coplan, J., Hodulik, S., Mathew, S., Mao, X., Hof, P., Gorman, J., & Shungu, D. (2012). The relationship between intelligence and anxiety: An association with subcortical white matter metabolism. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(8), 837844. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crews, S. D., Bender, H., Cook, C. R., Gresham, F. M., Kern, L., & Vanderwood, M. (2007). Risk and protective factors of emotional and/or behavioral disorders in children and adolescents: A mega-analytic synthesis. Behavioral Disorders, 32(2), 6477. https://doi.org/10.1177/019874290703200201 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupaul, G. J., Mcgoey, K. E., Eckert, T. L., & Vanbrakle, J. (2001). Preschool children with attention-deficit/Hyperactivity disorder: Impairments in behavioral, social, and school functioning. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(5), 508515. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200105000-00009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., & Pachan, M. (2010). A meta-analysis of after-school programs that seek to promote personal and social skills in children and adolescents. American Journal of Community Psychology, 45(3-4), 294309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10464-010-9300-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2004). The preschool age psychiatric assessment (PAPA): A structured parent interview for diagnosing psychiatric disorders in preschool children. In Handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment (pp. 223243). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(3-4), 313337. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01618.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, C. D. (1990). The differential ability scales: Introductory and technical handbook. Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Finsaas, M. C., Kessel, E. M., Dougherty, L. R., Bufferd, S. J., Danzig, A. P., Davila, J., Carlson, G. A., & Klein, D. N. (2020). Early childhood psychopathology prospectively predicts social functioning in early adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49(3), 353364. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1504298 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flook, L., Goldberg, S., Pinger, L., & Davidson, R. (2014). Promoting prosocial behavior and self-regulatory skills in preschool children through a mindfulness-based kindness curriculum. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 4451. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038256 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, L., Angold, A., Copeland, W., Costello, E. J., Towe-Goodman, N., & Egger, H. (2013). Preschool anxiety disorders in pediatric primary care: Prevalence and comorbidity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(12), 12941303.e1. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.09.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gadow, K. D., & Nolan, E. E. (2002). Differences between preschool children with ODD, ADHD, and ODD+ADHD symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 191201. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). The social skills rating system. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Halls, G., Cooper, P. J., & Creswell, C. (2015). Social communication deficits: Specific associations with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 3842. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.040 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, L., Plötner, M., & Schmitz, J. (2019a). Social competence and psychopathology in early childhood: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 443459. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1152-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, L., Plötner, M., & Schmitz, J. (2019b). Social competence and psychopathology in early childhood: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(4), 443459. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1152-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, T. L., & McIntosh, D. E. (2002). Differential ability scales: Profiles of preschoolers with cognitive delay. Psychology in the Schools, 39(1), 1929. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.10001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaureguizar, J., Garaigordobil, M., & Bernaras, E. (2018). Self-concept, social skills, and resilience as moderators of the relationship between stress and childhood depression. School Mental Health, 10(4), 488499. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-018-9268-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D. E., Greenberg, M., & Crowley, M. (2015). Early social-emotional functioning and public health: The relationship between kindergarten social competence and future wellness. American Journal of Public Health, 105(11), 22832290. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2015.302630 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenen, K. C., Moffitt, T. E., Roberts, A. L., Martin, L. T., Kubzansky, L., Harrington, H., Poulton, R., & Caspi, A. (2009). Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: A test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(1), 5057. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030343 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maciejewski, D., Brieant, A., Lee, J., King-Casas, B., & Kim-Spoon, J. (2020). Neural cognitive control moderates the relation between negative life events and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 49(1), 118133. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2018.1491005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahony, B. W., Tu, D., Rau, S., Liu, S., Lalonde, F. M., Alexander-Bloch, A. F., Satterthwaite, T. D., Shinohara, R. T., Bassett, D. S., Milham, M. P., & Raznahan, A. (2023). IQ modulates coupling between diverse dimensions of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 62(1), 5973. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2022.06.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maleki, M., Mardani, A., Mitra Chehrzad, M., Dianatinasab, M., & Vaismoradi, M. (2019). Social skills in children at home and in preschool. Behavioral Sciences, 9(7), Article–7. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9070074 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, A. S., Burt, K. B., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2015). Competence and psychopathology in development. In Developmental psychopathology (pp. 696738). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd, https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939406.ch19 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, A. S., Cutuli, J. J., Herbers, J. E., & Reed, M.-G. J. (2009). Resilience in development. In Lopez, S. J., & Snyder, C. R. (Ed.), The oxford handbook of positive psychology. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0012 Google Scholar
Mihalec-Adkins, B. P., & Cooley, M. E. (2020). Examining individual-level academic risk and protective factors for foster youth: School engagement, behaviors, self-esteem, and social skills. Child & Family Social Work, 25(2), 256266. https://doi.org/10.1111/cfs.12681 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohamed, A. H. H. (2018). Gender as a moderator of the association between teacher-child relationship and social skills in preschool. Early Child Development and Care, 188(12), 17111725. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1278371 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Mayer, B., & Prins, E. (2000). How serious are common childhood fears? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(3), 217228. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00204-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penney, A. M., Miedema, V. C., & Mazmanian, D. (2015). Intelligence and emotional disorders: Is the worrying and ruminating mind a more intelligent mind? Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 9093. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickard, H., Rijsdijk, F., Happé, F., & Mandy, W. (2017). Are social and communication difficulties a risk factor for the development of social anxiety? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(4), 344351.e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.01.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulou, M. S. (2015). Emotional and behavioural difficulties in preschool. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(2), 225236. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-013-9828-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, L. A., Braunstein, D. J., & Dumont, R. (2008). Use of the differential ability scales for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. School Psychology Quarterly, 23(1), 139148. https://doi.org/10.1037/1045-3830.23.1.139 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reef, J., van Meurs, I., Verhulst, F. C., & van der Ende, J. (2010). Children’s problems predict adults’ DSM-IV disorders across 24 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(11), 11171124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.08.002 Google ScholarPubMed
Schafer, J. L., & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 147177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., & Diamond, A. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social-emotional development through a simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school children: A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 5266. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038454 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauman, T. J. (2017). Self-regulation and psychopathology: Toward an integrative translational research paradigm. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13(1), 497523. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A., & Goodvin, R. (2016). Social support and developmental psychopathology. In Developmental psychopathology: Risk, resilience, and intervention. (vol. 4, 3rd ed. pp. 86135). John Wiley & Sons, Inc, https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy403 Google Scholar
Towe-Goodman, N. R., Franz, L., Copeland, W., Angold, A., & Egger, H. (2014). Perceived family impact of preschool anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(4), 437446. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.12.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, M., Wright, L., & Finegold, K. E. (2022). The effects of early life adversity on children’s mental health and cognitive functioning. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-02001-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H.-T., Sandall, S. R., Davis, C. A., & Thomas, C. J. (2011). Social skills assessment in young children with autism: A comparison evaluation of the SSRS and PKBS. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(11), 14871495. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1175-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P. D. (2020). Executive function and psychopathology: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 431454. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072319-024242 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Cohort description

Figure 1

Table 2. Bivariate correlations

Figure 2

Table 3. Descriptive statistics among variables of interest

Figure 3

Figure 1. Social skills moderate the association between preschool anxiety diagnosis and childhood psychopathology symptoms.

Figure 4

Table 4. Social skills regression model

Figure 5

Figure 2. Cognitive ability moderates the association between preschool anxiety diagnosis and childhood psychopathology symptoms.

Figure 6

Table 5. Cognitive ability regression model

Supplementary material: File

Navarro et al. supplementary material

Navarro et al. supplementary material
Download Navarro et al. supplementary material(File)
File 48.2 KB