Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T18:21:39.451Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceptual Priming and Reading Speed among Fourth Grade Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2014

Maria José Soler*
Affiliation:
Universitat de València (Spain)
Carmen Dasí
Affiliation:
Universitat de València (Spain)
Vicente Bellver
Affiliation:
Servicio Psicopedagógico Escolar de Valencia (SPE-V12). (Spain)
Juan Carlos Ruiz
Affiliation:
Universitat de València (Spain)
*
*Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Maria José Soler. Departamento de Psicología Básica. Facultad de Psicología. Universitat de València. Avda. Blasco Ibáñez, 21. 46010. Valencia (Spain). E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study evaluated the perceptual priming in fourth grade primary school children using a word-fragment completion task. The children were classified into two categories according to their reading speed: high and low. Using several sub-scales of the WISC-IV, their working memory was measured, and their total IQ was estimated, in order to control for their effects on priming. The statistical analyses showed that children with high reading speed were significantly better at word-fragment completion and showed greater priming (p < .01); in other words, the prior processing of the words from which the fragments came produced a greater benefit in the performance of the word-fragment completion task. A regression model was developed to explain reading speed based on the following variables: perceptual priming, working memory and percentage of completed fragments belonging to words not previously processed (adjusted R 2 = 0.64).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arguin, M., Bub, D., & Bowers, J. (1998). Extent and limits of covert lexical activation in letter-by-letter reading. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 5392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/026432998381221 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Betjemann, R. S., & Keenan, J. M. (2008). Phonological and semantic priming in children with reading disability. Child Development, 79, 10861102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01177.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruss, P. J., & Mitchell, D. B. (2009). Memory systems, processes, and tasks: Taxonomic clarification via factor analysis. The American Journal of Psychology, 122, 175189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casalis, S., Dusautoir, M., Colé, P., & Ducrot, S. (2009). Morphological effects in children word reading: A priming study in fourth graders. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 761766. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/026151008X389575 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, J. R., Anderson, V., Rankin, P. M., & MacDonald, J. (2010). An index-based short-form of the WISC-IV with accompanying analysis of the reliability and abnormality of differences. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 235258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466509X455470 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deacon, S. H., Campbell, E., Tamminga, M., & Kirby, J. (2010). Seeing the harm in harmed and harmful: Morphological processing by children in grades 4, 6, and 8. Applied Psycholinguistics, 31, 759775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000238 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, L. B., Rueckl, J., DiLiberto, K., Pastizzo, M., & Vellutino, F. R. (2002). Morphological analysis by child readers as revealed by the fragment completion task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 529535. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196309 Google Scholar
Fuentes, I., Romero, M., Dasí, C., & Ruiz, J. C. (2010). Versión abreviada del WAIS-III para su uso en la evaluación de pacientes con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia [Short version of the WAIS-III for use in the evaluation of patients with schizophrenia]. Psicothema, 22, 202207.Google Scholar
Monaghan, P., & Ellis, A. W. (2010). Modeling reading development: Cumulative, incremental learning in a computational model of word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 506525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.08.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñiz, J., & Fernández-Hermida, J. R. (2010). La opinión de los psicólogos españoles sobre el uso de los tests [The Spanish psychologists view on the use of tests]. Papeles del Psicólogo, 38, 101121.Google Scholar
Naito, M. (1990). Repetition priming in children and adults: Age-related dissociation between implicit and explicit memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 50, 462484. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(90)90081-I Google Scholar
Quémart, P., Casalis, S., & Colé, P. (2011). The role of form and meaning in the processing of written morphology: A priming study in French developing readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 478496. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.008 Google Scholar
Ray, S. (2008). An investigation on time course of category and semantic priming. The Journal of General Psychology, 135, 133148. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/GENP.135.2.133-150 Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. III., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In Spinnler, H. & Boller, F. (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elservier.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Wagner, A. D., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Memory systems of 1999. In Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 627643). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Soler, M. J., Dasí, C., & Ruiz, J. C. (2009). Datos normativos de 269 fragmentos de palabras españolas a partir de la base de Dasí, Soler, & Ruiz (2004) [Normative data of 269 fragments of Spanish words from the Dasi, Soler, and Ruiz' database (2004)]. Psicológica, 30, 91117.Google Scholar
TEA Ediciones (2005). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV [Wechsler Intelligence Scale IV for Children]. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Toro, J., & Cervera, M. (1989). TALE. Test de análisis de lecto-escritura [TALE. Test for the reading and writing]. Madrid, Spain: TEA Ediciones.Google Scholar
Warrington, E. K., & Shallice, T. (1980). Word-form dyslexia. Brain, 103, 99112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/103.1.99 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed