Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T13:05:23.479Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A school-based supplementary food programme in rural Kenya did not reduce children's intake at home

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2012

Constance A Gewa*
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition & Food Studies, College of Health & Human Services, George Mason University, 10340 Democracy Lane MSN 1F8, Fairfax, VA 22030, USA
Suzanne P Murphy
Affiliation:
University of Hawaii Cancer Center, Honolulu, HI, USA
Robert E Weiss
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Charlotte G Neumann
Affiliation:
Departments of Community Health Sciences and Pediatrics, Schools of Public Health and Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

To examine changes in energy intake along with markers of dietary quality (animal-source energy and protein intakes) among household members in the presence of supplementary school feeding in rural Kenya.

Design

A 2-year, longitudinal, randomized controlled feeding intervention study.

Setting

Kyeni South Division, Embu District, Kenya.

Subjects

A total of 182 schoolchildren and selected household members.

Results

There was no evidence that schoolchildren who received supplementary snacks at school experienced reduced intakes at home or that intakes by other family members were increased at the expense of the schoolchild's intake.

Conclusions

This analysis highlights a number of factors useful in planning for supplementary feeding interventions in rural Kenya and similar communities.

Type
Interventions
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Food distribution and childcare practices that favour particular gender and age groups have been reported among different populations(Reference Haddad, Pena and Nishida1Reference Sudo, Sekiyama and Maharjan4), but little is known about the effect of participation in supplementary feeding programmes that only target selected household members. A concern has been the likelihood of reallocation of foods/nutrients from programme targets to non-targets within the household. It has been shown that programme foods may be distributed to non-target household members when foods are distributed in the home(Reference Engle and Nieves5). Relatively few studies have assessed the dietary impact of school-based feeding among populations in Africa(Reference Adelman, Gilligan and Lehrer6). Studies that have assessed the dietary impact of school-based feeding on programme recipients in India, Bangladesh and the Philippines have shown that the total daily energy intake among target schoolchildren increased by about 75–100 % of the school feeding energy on the days that the children participated in the programme(Reference Adelman, Gilligan and Lehrer6Reference Afridi9). However, very few studies have assessed the dietary impact of school-based feeding programmes on non-target household members. Furthermore, a study conducted among African children did not study the impact of school feeding on recipients’ diet(Reference Kazianga, de Walque and Alderman10). Reallocation of foods consumed within the home can happen in two possible ways: programme foods may be distributed to non-target household members or non-programme foods previously provided to target members may be reallocated to other household members.

In the present analysis we examine changes in the amount of food consumed (total energy intake) and dietary quality (total protein intake, animal-source (AS) energy intake and AS protein intake) among household members in the presence of supplementary feeding of schoolchildren in rural Kenya. The Child Nutrition Project (CNP) provided three types of snacks to schoolchildren (vegetarian, meat and milk snacks), providing an opportunity to assess whether changes in intakes within the household differed by type of snack provided. The snacks were served at 09.00 hours so as not to interfere with the schoolchildren's usual dietary behaviour, and thus we hypothesized that: (i) there would be no statistically significant decreases in either the quantity or quality of food from home sources for schoolchildren in the intervention groups receiving snacks; and (ii) there would be no changes in intakes for other household members in these groups. No changes were expected for any of the household members in the control group.

Materials and methods

Data were collected as part of the CNP, a group-randomized controlled feeding intervention study carried out in Kyeni South Division of Embu District in Eastern Province, Kenya from 1998 to 2000(Reference Neumann, Bwibo and Murphy11). The study area is rural and subsistence farming is the primary occupation. All children (n 554) enrolled in grade 1 (median age 7·4 years) from twelve selected primary schools participated in the CNP study. The schools were randomized to one of four snack groups: (i) control (no food supplement provided); (ii) vegetarian supplement (a feeding based on a traditional local dish (githeri) of maize, beans and vegetables); (iii) milk supplement (githeri plus a glass (250 ml) of whole cows’ milk); and (iv) meat supplement (githeri cooked with 85 g of minced beef). The intervention period was 24 months, starting in August 1998 and ending in July 2000. Schools were in session for 3-month terms, with a 1-month holiday between each term with no feeding. Snacks were provided on all school days during the six school terms. The school snacks were approximately isoenergetic and contained an estimated energy content of ∼1046 kJ (∼250 kcal)/serving during the first school term of the intervention (September to November 1998) and ∼1339 kJ (∼320 kcal)/serving thereafter. School snacks were analysed every 3 months to ensure prescribed energy content; micronutrient content was analysed initially and twice annually thereafter. Project-trained feeding assistants served the snacks to the children in their classrooms, collected information on attendance and collected and measured the snack leftovers. All project enumerators and testers were not blinded to the snack group assignments but were blinded to the CNP study hypotheses. The schoolchildren's parents were aware of the snack group assignments. However, they were asked not to alter the children's home intake. An assessment of percentage of school days attended during the study period showed that mean attendance rate was higher among the meat group compared with the vegetarian, milk and control groups with a difference of 2·97 %, 2·78 % and 3·93 %, respectively(Reference Mukudi, Neumann and Bwibo12). Supplement consumption levels were high, with supplements eaten completely in over 99 % of the child-feeding sessions across all snack groups and across all the terms that the snacks were provided(Reference Gewa, Murphy and Bwibo13). A detailed description of the study area and design has been published elsewhere(Reference Neumann, Bwibo and Murphy11).

One of the three schools within each snack group was randomly selected by the project statistician to participate in the intra-household food allocation study, altogether yielding a total of 182 schoolchildren (Fig. 1). The goal was to include the mother, father, schoolchild and a sibling of the schoolchild within the age range of 2–15 years. There were no siblings within ages 2–15 years in 13 % (24/182) of the households while 17 % (31/182) of the fathers worked outside the study area and were not included. Data used for the intra-household food allocation study were collected at baseline in May–August 1998 and two years later in March–May 2000. Human Subjects Approval was obtained for the research study from the University of California, Los Angeles, the University of Nairobi, School of Medicine, Kenya and the Office of the President, Government of Kenya. Local and district authorities helped facilitate implementation of the study. The community was informed in detail about the aim and procedures of the intervention. Informed verbal consent by parents of study children was obtained before the study.

Fig. 1 Flow diagram displaying participants recruited into the intra-household food distribution study

Food intake

Dietary information consisted of three non-consecutive 24 h recalls for each of the household members at baseline and again during the follow-up period. Information on daily intake for each of the four household members was obtained, with the mothers being the main respondents. Each respective household member was asked to assist with the interview when present. Women with experience in dietary data collection methods from a previous study(Reference Neumann, Bwibo and Sigman14) were retrained to conduct 24 h recall interviews at home. The enumerators did not work over the weekend, so the dietary data collected reflect what was consumed on any one day from Sunday to Thursday. Procedures followed in determining nutrient intake have been previously described(Reference Murphy, Gewa and Liang15). In summary, mothers were asked to estimate amounts of foods consumed by household members and the ingredients used in mixed dishes prepared in the home. Common portion measures and food models were used, and later converted to gram weights using a database developed for this purpose. Respondents were also asked to describe foods consumed outside the home, including the primary ingredients for any out-of-home mixed dishes. Standard recipes were used to help determine the nutrient intake from these mixed dishes. The standard recipes were based on dietary information that had been collected in a previous study in the same study area(Reference Neumann, Bwibo and Sigman14) and were updated to reflect any changes in recipe content that had taken place over the years. Over 90 % of schoolchildren, siblings and mothers and 76 % of fathers had complete sets of three days of data collection at baseline. Mean number of available intake days at baseline were 2·87 (sd 0·34), 2·81 (sd 0·50), 2·84 (sd 0·41) and 2·64 (sd 0·65) for schoolchildren, siblings, mothers and fathers, respectively. Over 90 % of schoolchildren, siblings and mothers and 83 % of fathers had complete sets of three days of data collection at follow-up. Mean number of available intake days at follow-up were 2·93 (sd 0·29), 2·84 (sd 0·41), 2·86 (sd 0·40) and 2·73 (sd 0·60) for schoolchildren, siblings, mothers and fathers, respectively.

Nutrient intake

We calculated the schoolchildren's daily nutrient intake from home foods, excluding the CNP snack intake. Nutrient intakes were calculated for each individual using an international food composition table adapted for use in the present study(Reference Calloway, Murphy and Bunch16). The database contains complete nutrient values for the common foods consumed in rural Kenya. Nutrient contents of less common foods were estimated from similar foods. Nutrients of interest for all household members included a measure of the quantity of food consumed, total energy and three measures of dietary quality (AS energy, total protein and AS protein).

Socio-economic and demographic status of the family

Household socio-economic status (SES) was ascertained through interviews with the lead female/male in each household. The SES questionnaire was based on the SES ranking criteria identified by the area community leaders(Reference Neumann, Bwibo and Murphy11) which included possessions as well as economic and social status. A composite SES score was developed; a higher score represents a higher level of SES. Information on household members’ dates of birth, ages and gender was collected as part of the household census interviews conducted with the parents. A dichotomous indicator was developed to reflect whether the schoolchild was younger or older than his/her corresponding sibling.

Data analysis

We used ANOVA to check for group-based differences in ages of various household members, household SES and household sizes. CNP snack intake was not included in the schoolchildren's daily intakes. Participants were included in their treatment group as randomized. Because the regression models handle missing data, all children, including fourteen (three from vegetarian group, three from meat group, seven from milk group and one from control group) who were lost to follow-up, were included in the analyses. If a schoolchild left the school or moved to another school with a different treatment, then his/her data after the move and those of other household members were omitted. All available data prior to the move were included in the analyses. For the nutrients of interest (total energy, AS energy, total protein and AS protein), we plotted baseline and follow-up mean nutrient intake levels. Original nutrient intake values were natural log-transformed to accommodate skewness. Schoolchildren's and siblings’ data over time were analysed in a single paired longitudinal random-effects model that was fit with Restricted Maximum Likelihood estimation(Reference Weiss17) in the SAS mixed procedure (SAS statistical software package version 9; SAS Institute) to assess changes in intake across time and differences between groups (vegetarian, meat, milk and control). Similarly, parents’ data over time were fit together in a single paired longitudinal random-effects model. Additional difference of difference assessment was conducted to assess if any nutrient intake changes were significantly different across groups. Covariates included days categorized into three categories (baseline, school days at follow-up and non-school days at follow-up), groups (vegetarian, meat, milk and control), sex (male, female), household member indicator (schoolchild, sibling, mother or father), schoolchild–sibling age status indicator (schoolchild younger than sibling, schoolchild older than sibling), household size and SES. A total of twenty-four schoolchildren did not have siblings, so the age status indicator variable is not defined and they were omitted from the children's analyses. Household SES information was not available for one household. Complete data were available for the remaining covariates in the analysis. As a result of these values, twenty-five schoolchildren were not included in the regression analysis. The regression model adjusts for these missing values. Separate sets of analyses were carried out for children and for parents. The schoolchild–sibling age status indicator was omitted from parents’ analyses.

Our analysis followed a strategy that examined simple to more complex models that included two-way and three-way variable interactions. We first considered models with only main effects. Next, two-way interactions were created using those main effects that were significant. Significant two-way interactions were combined into three-way interactions and included. None of the three-way interactions was significant, leading to all three-way interactions being eliminated from the analysis. Our model kept all two-way interactions and associated main effects. Despite not being statistically significant, sex was included as a main effect in the final model. Significant two-way interactions were schoolchild–sibling age status × household member indicator, schoolchild–sibling age status × days, schoolchild–sibling age status × group and group × days. For parents, the group × days interaction was the only significant two-way interaction. Because the household member × days interaction was found to be non-significant, a decision was made to combine schoolchildren and siblings as ‘children’ and to combine mothers and fathers as ‘parents’ in final sets of analysis and in reporting. All regression analysis adjusted for household size and SES. Our interest lies in change in nutrient intake over time and we were therefore particularly interested in the interaction between feeding group and days. The interaction captured change in the nutrient intake slope associated with each treatment group. Because days was coded as a three-group categorical variable that distinguished baseline days, follow-up school days and follow-up non-school days, we could compare baseline intakes with school day intakes at follow-up and with the weighted average of five school days and two non-school day average intakes at follow-up. School day intakes were Monday–Thursday intakes during the school year while non-school days included Sundays during the school year and Sunday to Thursday during school vacations. An α level of 0·05 was used to indicate statistical significance.

Results

Fifty-one and fifty-four per cent of the schoolchildren and siblings, respectively, were male. Schoolchildren's ages ranged from 6 to 11 years while that of the siblings ranged from 2 to 15 years. No significant age differences were noted between male and female schoolchildren and between male and female siblings. Mothers were significantly younger than fathers (34·0 (sd 7·8) v. 41·1 (sd 9·7) years; Table 1). Between-group comparisons showed that schoolchildren in the meat group were older than children in the other groups and that they came from larger households compared with those in the milk group. There were no differences in household SES score across the snack groups.

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants by snack groupFootnote *: schoolchildren and selected household members participating in the Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

SES, socio-economic status.

a,b Mean values within a row with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).

* Sample sizes given in Fig. 1.

School days as a percentage of intake days at baseline.

School days as a percentage of intake days at follow-up.

The percentage of schoolchildren with recalls for at least one school day during the baseline period ranged from 93 % among the milk group to 100 % for the rest of the groups. On average, 64 % of each child's baseline intake recall days (∼2 of 3 d) were carried out on school days. The percentage of schoolchildren with recalls for at least one school day during the follow-up period ranged from 74 % among the milk group to 100 % among the control group. On average, 49 % of each child's baseline intake recall days (1–2 of 3 d) days were carried out on school days.

Baseline intake

Schoolchildren's daily energy and protein intakes at baseline, before the school feeding programme began, were similar across the four snack groups. Siblings’ daily energy intake at baseline was similar across all four snack groups (Table 2). Meat group siblings’ protein intake mean estimates were significantly lower than those of siblings in the milk group. However, this difference was not sustained after adjusting for other covariates (Table 3). Meat group children had significantly lower AS energy and AS protein intakes compared with those enrolled in the vegetarian, milk and control groups (Tables 2 and 3). The differences noted in AS energy and AS protein were sustained after controlling for other covariates in the regression analysis (Table 3).

Table 2 Daily nutrient intakes at baseline by snack groupFootnote *Footnote : schoolchildren and selected household members participating in the Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

a,b Mean values within a row with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).

* Sample sizes given in Fig. 1.

Reported values are not adjusted for any covariates.

Parents’ baseline intake estimates followed a pattern similar to that of schoolchildren: daily energy and protein intake estimates similar across all four groups and AS energy and AS protein intake estimates significantly lower among meat group parents compared with vegetarian, milk and control group parents. Group differences in AS energy and AS protein intakes at baseline were maintained even after adjusting for other covariates (Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3 Children's log-transformed daily nutrient intakes (baseline and follow-up period) and change estimates by snack groupFootnote *Footnote Footnote , Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

a,b For each nutrient, mean values within a column with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).

* Sample sizes: n 37 for vegetarian group; n 47 for meat group; n 45 for milk group; n 29 for control group.

Results are estimates from regression analyses on natural log-transformed nutrient intake values.

Analysis adjusted for schoolchild–sibling age status, household socio-economic status, household size and sex.

§ Weighted average of school and non-school days at follow-up.

Significantly different from baseline (P < 0·05).

Change significantly different from that of the control group (P < 0·05).

Table 4 Parents’ log-transformed daily nutrient intakes (baseline and follow-up period) and change estimates by snack groupFootnote *Footnote Footnote , Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

a,b For each nutrient, mean values within a column with unlike superscript letters were significantly different (P < 0·05).

* Sample sizes given in Fig. 1.

Results are estimates from regression analyses on natural log-transformed nutrient intake values.

Analysis adjusted for household socio-economic status, household size and sex.

§ Weighted average of school and non-school days at follow-up.

Significantly different from baseline (P < 0·05).

Change significantly different from that of the control group (P < 0·05).

Changes in intake at home between baseline and follow-up

Children in the meat group experienced significant increases in their energy and protein intakes at follow-up, with the follow-up intake values being significantly higher than those noted at baseline (Table 3). Children from the vegetarian, milk and control groups did not experience any significant changes in their energy and protein intakes at follow-up. The changes experienced by the meat group children were not significantly different from those of the control group. These results did not vary by schoolchild–sibling age status.

There was a significant decline in the vegetarian group parents’ total energy and protein intakes on follow-up school days and during the overall follow-up period (Table 4). Although parents from the remaining three groups experienced some decline in their energy and protein intake values across time, the declines were not statistically significant. The vegetarian group's energy and protein intake declines were significantly different from those noted among meat and milk group parents. The declines were not significantly different from those experienced by the control group parents.

Meat and control group parents experienced significant declines in AS energy and AS protein on follow-up school days and the overall follow-up period, while the vegetarian group parents experienced a significant decline in AS protein intake during the overall follow-up period. Milk group parents experienced only slight non-significant declines in dietary quality measures across time. Meat and control group dietary quality declines were significantly different from those noted among the milk group parents. No other differences were noted across groups.

Discussion

These findings allow an evaluation of the changes in household members’ intakes over the two years of the CNP feeding study. Changes in the food consumed at home (excluding the school snack for the schoolchild) differed by household member (parent v. child) and by treatment group. Changes associated with total protein exhibited similar patterns to those noted for total energy, an indication that total protein may be more of a measure of food quantity and not quality for this population group in rural Kenya. Changes associated with AS protein exhibited similar patterns to those noted for AS energy.

The study's results support our first hypothesis: the home-based energy, protein, AS energy and AS protein intakes of snack-receiving schoolchildren did not decrease across the study period. Siblings experienced changes that were similar to those of schoolchildren, thus lending support to our second hypothesis to a large extent. Despite the regression analysis showing no significant differences in children's energy and protein intakes at baseline (Table 3), the meat group children's energy and protein intakes increased during the study period. A possible explanation for these increases may lie in meat group schoolchildren's increased appetite as a result of improved Zn intake from the meat-containing snacks(Reference Prasad18). This increase may also be as a result of increased muscle growth associated with the meat-receiving schoolchildren(Reference Grillenberger, Neumann and Murphy19). However, this does not explain the similar energy and protein intake increases noted among the group's siblings. It is possible that the schoolchildren's increased intake may have had a residual effect on overall children's intake in the respective households. However our data do not allow us to assess such a hypothesis. Control group children did not experience any significant change in energy, protein, AS energy or AS protein intake across time, clearly showing support for our third hypothesis with respect to children's diet quantity and quality.

However, the study results showed minimal support for the second and third hypotheses with respect to parent's intakes. There was a general decline among parents’ energy and protein and AS energy and AS protein intakes indicating a decline in food quantity and quality. This decline may have been as a result of crop failure associated with failed ‘long rains’ between March and May 2000(20). However, as noted within the Results section, the declines were significant only for vegetarian, meat and control group parents but not for the milk group, indicating that the magnitude of food quantity and quality decline was different among parents. Such differences in response were not anticipated and more research into the possible reasons is needed.

These results demonstrate that irrespective of whether the schoolchildren received snacks at school or not, the children's home-based dietary intake seemed to have been given priority and protected from any significant decline over time. On the other hand, most parents did not receive similar protection and experienced declines in their dietary quantity and quality over time, suggesting that there may be differences in ways that households and their members respond to changes in food amounts and food quality, especially in times of food scarcity. These differences may need to be further assessed. Even though children's home-based intake appear to be protected during times of food scarcity, feeding programmes such as the CNP are important in addressing inadequacies that are present in recipients’ diets and physical growth status. Analysis of CNP's schoolchildren's nutritional status reported inadequate dietary intake and physical growth at baseline(Reference Murphy, Gewa and Liang15, Reference Grillenberger, Neumann and Murphy19).

Strengths and limitations of the study

One strength of the present study lies in the use of dietary data collected multiple times (three days at baseline and three days at follow-up), providing an approximation of usual intakes among the household members. The use of the 24 h recall has been shown to be appropriate in this population(Reference Kigutha21, Reference Gewa, Murphy and Neumann22). Although the weighment method is considered a better estimate for dietary intake, its use is limited in large studies due to the costs involved. Weighed records may also result in study participants altering their dietary behaviour, especially if enumerators are present. Another limitation lies in including only one of three schools in each of the study groups. We would like to note that although the randomization process generally resulted in comparable measures at baseline (Table 1), the meat group schoolchildren were slightly older and they came from significantly larger households, which may explain their lower intakes of AS energy and protein at baseline. Larger household sizes place a strain on limited household resources resulting in relatively lower food intake levels among household members(Reference Pelto, Urgello and Allen23). Additionally, older children may receive relatively lower amounts of food compared with younger children in times of food scarcity. An assessment among Indian schoolchildren reported that being an older target recipient, larger household size and poorer household were significantly associated with higher transfer of energy from the school meal recipient(Reference Afridi9). Our analyses adjusted for important factors that have been shown to influence dietary intake: schoolchild–sibling age indicator, sex, SES and household size. Thus, the reported impact is independent of the children's age and sex and household SES and size.

Conclusions

Results from this analysis indicate a number of factors that may be useful in planning for supplementary feeding interventions in Kenya and similar communities. There was no evidence that schoolchildren who received supplementary snacks at school experienced reduced intakes at home or that intakes by other family members were increased at the expense of the schoolchild's intake. Even when excluding the nutrient contributions from the school snack, children were generally protected from a decline in nutrient intake levels compared with adult members of the family.

Acknowledgements

Preliminary results of these analyses were presented at the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology (FASEB) conference, New Orleans, USA, 18-22 April 2009 (presentation title: Impact of supplementary feeding on intra-household food distribution in rural Kenya). This work was carried out as part of the CNP study, which was supported by the Global Livestock Collaborative Research Support Program, University of California at Davis, through the US Agency for International Development (grant number PCE-G-00-98-00036-00). There are no conflicts of interest. C.A.G. was the Field Nutritionist for CNP, conducted the research, performed the statistical analysis and wrote the paper. S.P.M. was the Co-Investigator for CNP, conducted the research, helped guide the statistical analysis and wrote the paper. R.E.W. provided guidance on the statistical analysis and edited numerous drafts. C.G.N. was the Principal Investigator for CNP, was responsible for the study design, oversaw overall research activities, and helped guide the statistical analysis and writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. The authors would like to thank all participants and workers for their collaboration in the CNP.

References

1.Haddad, L, Pena, C, Nishida, Cet al. (1996) Food Security and Nutrition Implications of Intrahousehold Bias: A Review of Literature. Washington, DC: International Food Policy Research Institute.Google Scholar
2.Kramer, EM, Peterson, KE, Rogers, BLet al. (1997) Intrahousehold allocation of energy intake among children under five years and their parents in rural Bangladesh. Eur J Clin Nutr 51, 750756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Luo, W, Zhai, F, Jin, Set al. (2001) Intrahousehold food distribution: a case study of eight provinces in China. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 10, Suppl., S19S28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Sudo, N, Sekiyama, M, Maharjan, Met al. (2006) Gender differences in dietary intake among adults of Hindu communities in lowland Nepal: assessment of portion sizes and food consumption frequencies. Eur J Clin Nutr 60, 469477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Engle, PL & Nieves, I (1993) Intra-household food distribution among Guatemalan families in a supplementary feeding program: behavior patterns. Soc Sci Med 36, 16051612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6.Adelman, SW, Gilligan, DO & Lehrer, K (2008) How Effective are Food for Education Programs? A Critical Assessment of Evidence from Developing Countries. Washington DC: International Food Policy and Research Institute; available at http://www.ifpri.org/sites/default/files/pubs/pubs/fpreview/pv09/pv09.pdfGoogle Scholar
7.Jacoby, HG (2002) Is there an intrahousehold ‘flypaper effect’? Evidence from a school feeding programme. Econ J 112, 196221.Google Scholar
8.Ahmed, AU (2004) Impact of Feeding Children in School: Evidence from Bangladesh. Washington, DC: International Food Policy and Research Institute.Google Scholar
9.Afridi, F (2005) Public Transfers and Intra-Household Resource Allocation: Evidence from a Supplementary School Feeding Program. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan ; available at http://www.fordschool.umich.edu/edts/pdfs/afridi_paper.pdfGoogle Scholar
10.Kazianga, H, de Walque, D, Alderman, H (2009) Educational and Health Impacts of Two School Feeding Schemes: Evidence from Randomized Trial in Rural Burkina Faso. Impact Evaluation Series no. 30, Policy Research Working Paper no. 4976. Washington, DC: The World Bank Development Research Group; available at http://www-wds.worldbank.org/external/default/WDSContentServer/IW3P/IB/2009/07/15/000158349_20090715163511/Rendered/PDF/WPS4976.pdfGoogle Scholar
11.Neumann, CG, Bwibo, NO, Murphy, SPet al. (2003) Animal source foods improve dietary quality, micronutrient status, growth and cognitive function in Kenyan school children: background, study design and baseline findings. J Nutr 133, 11 Suppl. 2, 3941S3949S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Mukudi, EO, Neumann, C & Bwibo, NO (2011) Effects of a school feeding intervention on school attendance rates among elementary school children in rural Kenya. Nutrition 27, 188193.Google Scholar
13.Gewa, C, Murphy, SP, Bwibo, NOet al. (2005) Assessment of Snack Acceptance During a School Feeding Intervention in Rural Kenya. Global Livestock CRSP Research Brief no. 05-03-CNP. Davis, CA: Global Livestock Collaborative Research Support Program.Google Scholar
14.Neumann, C, Bwibo, NO & Sigman, M (1987) Diet Quantity and Quality. Functional Effects on Rural Kenyan Families. Kenya Project Final Report. Phase II – 1989–1992. Human Nutrition Collaborative Research Support Program. Los Angeles, CA: UCLA School of Public Health.Google Scholar
15.Murphy, SP, Gewa, C, Liang, Let al. (2003) School snacks containing animal source foods improve dietary quality for children in rural Kenya. J Nutr 133, 11 Suppl. 2, 3950S3956S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Calloway, DH, Murphy, SP, Bunch, Set al. (2004) World Food Dietary Assessment System User's Guides. Berkley, CA: University of California.Google Scholar
17.Weiss, RE (2005) Modeling Longitudinal Data. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
18.Prasad, AS (1991) Discovery of human zinc deficiency and studies in an experimental human model. Am J Clin Nutr 53, 403412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19.Grillenberger, M, Neumann, CG, Murphy, SPet al. (2003) Food supplements have a positive impact on weight gain and the addition of animal source foods increases lean body mass of Kenyan school children. J Nutr 133, 11 Suppl. 2, 3957S3964S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20.Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations (2000) Special Report: Crop and Food Supply Situation in Kenya. Rome: FAO; available at http://www.fao.org/docrep/004/x7697e/x7697e00.htmGoogle Scholar
21.Kigutha, HN (1997) Assessment of dietary intake in rural communities in Africa: experiences in Kenya. Am J Clin Nutr 65, 4 Suppl., 1168S1172S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Gewa, CA, Murphy, SP & Neumann, CG (2009) A comparison of weighed and recalled intakes for schoolchildren and mothers in rural Kenya. Public Health Nutr 12, 11971204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Pelto, GH, Urgello, J, Allen, NHet al. (1991) Household size, food intake and anthropometric status of school-age children in a highland Mexican area. Soc Sci Med 33, 11351140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow diagram displaying participants recruited into the intra-household food distribution study

Figure 1

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics of the participants by snack group*: schoolchildren and selected household members participating in the Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

Figure 2

Table 2 Daily nutrient intakes at baseline by snack group*†: schoolchildren and selected household members participating in the Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

Figure 3

Table 3 Children's log-transformed daily nutrient intakes (baseline and follow-up period) and change estimates by snack group*†‡, Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000

Figure 4

Table 4 Parents’ log-transformed daily nutrient intakes (baseline and follow-up period) and change estimates by snack group*†‡, Child Nutrition Project (CNP), rural Kenya, 1998–2000