Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T23:21:03.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

School-based Internet-tailored fruit and vegetable education combined with brief counselling increases children's awareness of intake levels

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2007

Resiti T Mangunkusumo
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Johannes Brug
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Harry J de Koning
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Johan van der Lei
Affiliation:
Department of Medical Informatics, Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Hein Raat*
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health, Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam, PO Box 2040, 3000 CA Rotterdam, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Children's fruit/vegetable intake is still below recommended levels. This study applied Internet-tailored advice for schoolchildren and Internet-supported brief dietary counselling (with child and parent) within preventive health care to promote fruit/vegetable intake.

Setting/subjects

The study involved 30 seventh-grade classes (16 in the intervention group and 14 in the control group) with a total of 675 children aged 9–12 years, of whom 495 were allowed to participate.

Design

A cluster-randomised baseline–post-test experimental design was applied. During school hours, all children completed Internet-administered questionnaires on fruit/vegetable intake and related determinants. Children in the intervention group received immediate online individually tailored nutrition feedback. For each child in the intervention group, a nurse received information concerning the assessment of fruit/vegetable intake via the Internet to support a 5 min counselling protocol to promote fruit/vegetable intake. Children completed a similar post-test questionnaire 3 months after the first assessment. Intention-to-treat analyses were conducted using multilevel regression analyses.

Results

A total of 486 children (98% of 495) participated (263 in the intervention group, 223 in the control group); 240 child–parent couples in the intervention group attended the counselling. Awareness of inadequate fruit intake (odds ratio (OR) = 3.0; 95% confidence interval (CI) = 1.8–5.3) and knowledge of recommended vegetable intake levels (OR = 2.7; 95% CI = 1.8–4.1) were significantly more likely at post-test in the intervention group than in the control group. No significant effects were found on intake or other determinants.

Conclusions

A compact, integrated two-component intervention can induce positive changes in knowledge and awareness of intake levels of fruit/vegetables among schoolchildren. To induce changes in intake levels, more comprehensive interventions may be needed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2007

Sufficient intake of fruits and vegetables may contribute to the prevention of cardiovascular diseases, certain cancers and obesityReference Maynard, Gunnell, Emmett, Frankel and Davey Smith1Reference Hung, Joshipura, Jiang, Hu, Hunter and Smith-Warner4. Across Europe, including The Netherlands, current intake levels among schoolchildren are below recommendationsReference Yngve, Wolf, Poortvliet, Elmadfa, Brug and Ehrenblad5. Adopting a health-promoting diet at a young age may help to establish healthy eating in later lifeReference Lien, Lytle and Klepp6, Reference Mikkila, Rasanen, Raitakari, Pietinen and Viikari7.

Various evidence-based interventions are available to encourage healthy dietary habits for schoolchildren, and tailoring of intervention messages is considered a promising approach for promoting healthy eating among young peopleReference Doak8Reference Shepherd, Harden, Rees, Brunton, Garcia and Oliver10. In general (mainly targeting adults), computer-tailored nutrition education is more likely to be read, processed more intensively and appreciated better than more general intervention materialsReference Brug, Campbell and van Assema11Reference Mangunkusumo, Brug, Duisterhout, de Koning and Raat13. It also appears to have a positive impact on behavioural changeReference Brug, Oenema and Campbell12, Reference Kroeze, Werkman and Brug14. Since nurses (i.e. school nurses) already invite children for consultation as current practice in (Dutch) preventive health care, complementing this with computer-tailored nutrition advice and dietary counselling by a nurse may be an efficient approach in promoting a healthy diet. To our knowledge, studies regarding computer-tailored nutritional interventions for schoolchildren as well as the involvement of nurses in such school-based interventions are unreported.

The present study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of a two-component intervention on fruit/vegetable intake by comparing a group receiving the intervention with a control group. It also explored effects on potential determinants of intake levels and self-reported intake as important initial steps towards behavioural change. In addition, we determined the level of exposure to the intervention.

Methods

Two-component intervention

This study applied a school-based intervention among children aged 9–12 years. The intervention consisted of a combination of Internet-tailored nutrition advice for the children followed by Internet-supported brief dietary counselling by the nurse in the presence of at least one parent. This two-component intervention was informed by behavioural change theory and recent evidence that healthy behavioural change is more likely when children are aware of their inadequate intake levels, have knowledge of the recommended intake levels, like to eat fruit/vegetables, and when the fruit/vegetables they like are available and accessibleReference Blanchette and Brug9Reference Bere and Klepp15Reference Wind, De Bourdeaudhuij, Te Velde, Sandvik, Due and Klepp18.

Internet-tailored advice

The Internet-tailored advice specifically aimed to increase knowledge of recommended intake levels, increase awareness of personal intake levels, and stimulate children's liking of fruit/vegetables and their availability in the home.

During school hours the children in both the intervention and control group completed Internet-administered questionnaires on fruit/vegetable intake and related determinants. Children in the intervention group received immediate online feedback about nutrition, individually tailored to each child's answersReference Brug, Oenema and Campbell12, Reference Perez-Rodrigo, Wind, Hildonen, Bjelland, Aranceta and Klepp19. First, the advice explained the recommended intake levels in order to increase knowledge and then compared the child's personal intake with the recommendations in order to increase awareness. The daily recommendations were two pieces of fruit and 150 g (three ‘serving spoons’) of vegetablesReference Yngve, Wolf, Poortvliet, Elmadfa, Brug and Ehrenblad5. Second, the feedback encouraged the child to eat sufficient amounts of fruit/vegetables, e.g. by trying different types of fruit/vegetables and asking their parents to buy/give them the fruit/vegetables that they like.

Children were encouraged to take home a printout of the advice. The teachers supervised the completion of the questionnaire, as well as the printing out of the advice. The assessment took place in either the classroom or in the school's computer area (depending on the school facilities).

Dietary counselling

The dietary counselling focused on increasing the child's knowledge about recommended intake levels, increasing awareness of personal intake levels, and motivating the child to eat sufficient fruit/vegetables and/or discussing any barriers in order to increase self-efficacy for behavioural change. An additional aim of the counselling was to involve the child's home in the process of behavioural change.

To support the counselling, the nurse had access to the data on the child's intake and related determinants that were assessed with the computer-tailored Internet tool; a website provided the nurse with a summary of the child's dietary status and related determinants, the Internet-tailored advice and the child's answer to each item.

About two weeks after the Internet-tailored advice, the child together with one parent attended the brief dietary counselling. The nurse applied a protocol in order to promote the child's fruit/vegetable intake. First, the nurse tried to reinforce knowledge on recommendations and increase awareness of the child's inadequate intake. The focus of the remaining counselling time depended on the child's (and parent's) motivation to eat sufficient fruit/vegetables. If the motivation to change was lacking, then the nurse explained the benefits of adequate intake and tried to dispel misconceptions. If motivation was present, the nurse discussed possible barriers to eating sufficient fruit/vegetables and how to overcome these barriers, in some cases resulting in defining personal goals to achieve behavioural change. This approach is based on the Minimal Intervention Strategy as applied, for example, in counselling to cease smokingReference Wiggers, Oort, Dijkstra, de Haes, Legemate and Smets20.

In any case, the nurse would again give a printout of the Internet-tailored advice to the child and a leaflet on the importance of vegetable consumption to the parents. This counselling (which took about 5 min) occurred at the end of the periodic health examination, which is a routine contact moment of the municipal health services with parents/guardians and their child. Thus children in both the intervention and control groups received the periodic health examination, but only children in the intervention group received the additional dietary counselling. The counselling took place in a private room either at school or at the health service's premises.

Internet tool

The Internet tool of the two-component intervention was developed using PHP (4.0.1 and higher), MySQL (3.22 and higher) and JavaScript (1.3). Access to the questionnaire and advice was password-protected. Data were sent to the server in a scrambled format. Logging out after completing the questionnaire was permitted only after answering all questions. Each nurse received a personal log-in code from the researcher to access the Internet tool and thereby the data for each child.

Study design and sample

A cluster-randomised baseline–post-test experimental design was appliedReference Campbell, Elbourne and Altman21. Two participating municipal health services, one in a rural area (Harderwijk) and one in an urban area (Vlaardingen) in The Netherlands, invited 27 elementary schools from various socio-economic and religious backgrounds to participate. These schools comprised 30 classes from the seventh grade with 675 schoolchildren (aged 9–12 years) and all had adequate computer and Internet facilities. From the Vlaardingen region 14 classes participated, involving two nurses, and from the Harderwijk region 16 classes participated, involving three nurses. In each class all children received counselling from the same nurse, as this is the standard practice for municipal health services. After recruitment, the classes were randomly assigned (‘within’ nurses) to the intervention group with tailored nutrition advice and counselling (16 classes) or the control group (14 classes). This clustering was done to minimise contamination.

The parents and children, blinded to which group they belonged to, received information about the project and parents were allowed several weeks to actively give written consent for participation. This resulted in 495 (73%) children with permission to participate. The Medical Ethical Committee of the Erasmus MC–University Medical Center Rotterdam approved the study.

Evaluation measures

All children completed (during school hours) an Internet-administered baseline questionnaire and about three months later a similar post-test questionnaire on potential determinants and intake related to fruit/vegetables, as well as questions on gender, age, type of family, and countries of birth of the child and parents to determine ethnic backgroundReference Keij22. The baseline questionnaire was also the questionnaire used for the assessment of the Internet-tailored advice. After receiving the Internet-tailored advice, the children completed a brief evaluation form to assess the level of exposure to the intervention. The nurses did the same after giving the counselling.

Primary outcome measures

The primary outcome measures were fruit/vegetable intake. Children completed the fruit/vegetable intake questions that were derived from, and thus not exactly the same as (e.g. different categories), the fruit/vegetable food-frequency questionnaire (FFQ) validated by Van Assema et al. Reference Van Assema, Brug, Ronda, Steenhuis and Oenema23. The relative validity of the FFQ was assessed by comparison with 7-day dietary records. The correlations of the validity estimates ranged between 0.29 and 0.59 for citrus fruit and other fruit intake, and between 0.22 and 0.35 for total vegetable intake (heated and raw/salad, last week).

In the present questionnaire children were asked about the number of days on which they ate fruit (such as apples, mandarins, pears, or oranges) during the week before FFQ administration and the number of pieces of fruit on such days; multiplying the frequency and quantity and dividing this by seven assessed the ‘fruit frequency last week’ in servings per day. A pre-coded 24-hour recall assessed vegetable intake of the day before administration. The recall was divided into three parts: heated vegetables, salad, and vegetables in mixed dishes. Each part started with a general question such as ‘Did you eat heated vegetables (such as broccoli, French beans, and carrots) yesterday?’ (yes/no), and if confirmed, followed by ‘How much of such vegetables did you eat yesterday?’ Answers ranged from one serving spoon to three or more serving spoons. Standards were defined for the units in spoons: one serving spoon of heated vegetables equalled 50 g, one serving spoon of salad equalled 20 g, and one serving spoon of vegetables in mixed dishes equalled 15 g. Summing the grams of these three types of vegetables assessed the ‘24-hour recall vegetable intake’. Finally, confirmation of two dichotomous items, ‘Do you eat fruit everyday during schooldays?’ and ‘Do you eat fruit every Saturday and Sunday?’, established the number of children reporting ‘usual daily intake’ for fruit. This was also done for vegetables.

Secondary outcome measures

As secondary outcome measures, potential behavioural determinants of dietary intake were assessed for fruit and vegetables separately. One question measured whether the child thought that he/she ate a sufficient amount of fruit (in yes/no format). In combination with the child's actual intake (see above) being adequate or not, this determined the child's ‘awareness’ of inadequate intake for fruit in the previous week; the same was done for vegetable intake during the previous day. To assess ‘knowledge’ of recommended intake levels, children were asked (on an 8-point scale) how much fruit they should eat to have a healthy eating pattern. Response options ranged from no fruit to five pieces a day or more. This was subsequently re-coded into a dichotomous variable (amount too low vs. correct/higher amount); the same was done for recommended vegetable intake levels, dichotomised from response options ranging from no vegetables to five serving spoons (~250 g) a day or more. Asking the children if they thought eating fruit every day was achievable or not assessed ‘self-efficacy’. This was dichotomised from a 5-point Likert scale (negative or neutral scores = 0; positive scores = 1); the same was done for vegetable intake. A single item assessing the child's ‘liking’ of fruit was dichotomised from a 3-point Likert scale to the child reporting liking many types (negative or neutral scores = 0; positive scores = 1); the same was done for liking vegetables. The ‘availability’ of fruit was assessed by an item on how often fruit was available at home. Response options were dichotomised from a 3-point Likert scale to the child reporting they always had fruit at home (negative or neutral scores = 0; positive scores = 1). Finally, the number of types of vegetables available determined the ‘availability’ of vegetables at dinner on a 4-point scale. Response options ranged from usually having no vegetables to more than two types of vegetables. This was subsequently recoded into a dichotomous variable (less than two types vs. two or more types).

Measures to assess exposure to the intervention

After the Internet-tailored advice, the children completed an additional item on how much they had read of the advice, with four possible answers ranging from having read nothing to having read both the fruit and vegetable advice completely. After the counselling session, the nurses completed a form to assess the child's attendance at the counselling. Additionally, the nurses noted which of four topics they concluded the counselling session with: i.e. increasing motivation to change, increasing self-efficacy, setting goals, or sustaining/taking action to eat sufficiently.

Statistical analysis

To test for equality of groups at baseline, multiple logistic regression of treatment condition (yes/no) was conducted using SPSS version 11.0.1. Baseline measures and sociodemographic characteristics were the independent variables. The same regression was done for dropouts (yes/no), with baseline measures, sociodemographic characteristics and treatment condition as independent variables. This allowed us to identify potential correlates of dropout and thereby potential biases in post-test data analyses.

In all effect analyses, we included children with a missing post-test value due to dropout by replacing the missing post-test value with the baseline value (i.e. the last observation carried forward method, ‘intention-to-treat analyses’Reference Twisk24).

Due to the study design, children were grouped within nurses and classes with a high probability of interdependence between children of the same class and/or of the same nurse. To take this into account, multilevel analyses with random intercepts were conducted in MLwiN 2.02Reference Rasbash, Steele, Browne and Prosser25 to assess the effects of the intervention on behaviour and determinants of behaviour, while controlling for region (i.e. municipal health service), gender, age and baseline value of the outcome measures (except for knowledge and self-efficacy, for which no baseline measure had been assessed). The regression coefficient (β) or odds ratio (OR) was used to describe effect size, with the control group as the reference category. An α-level of 0.05 (two-tailed) or 95% confidence interval (CI) was used to determine statistical significance.

The current dataset was large enough to show a significant difference for individuals (while not taking the nurse and class levels into account) of, for example, 8% for awareness of inadequate fruit intake and 0.17 for the mean servings of fruit intake per day, with a power of 80%.

Results

Participants

Of the 495 children with consent, 486 children (98%) participated in the baseline measurement including the Internet-tailored advice, of which 263 children belonged to the intervention group and 223 children to the control group. The post-test measurement included 469 children (97% of baseline participation). The main reason for absence was illness.

Table 1 presents the characteristics of the children at baseline. There were no significant differences between the intervention and control groups regarding sociodemographic characteristics and baseline measures (P ≥ 0.05). Neither did the children who dropped out at post-test differ on sociodemographic characteristics, baseline measures and study group, compared with the children included at post-test (P ≥ 0.05).

Table 1 Characteristics of the children at baseline by study group

* Mean (standard deviation).

Both parents born in The Netherlands.

Outcome measures at post-test

Fruit and vegetable intake did not differ significantly between the intervention and control groups (Tables 2 and 3). Of the secondary outcome measures, children in the intervention group were three times more likely to be aware of their personal inadequate fruit intake level at post-test than children in the control group (Table 2). Children in the intervention group were almost three times more likely to know the recommended vegetable levels at post-test than children in the control group (Table 3). Awareness of inadequate vegetable intake and knowledge of the fruit recommendation showed ORs in favour of the intervention group, but these were not significant. All remaining behavioural determinants of fruit and vegetable intake showed OR close to one.

Table 2 Self-reported fruit intake and determinants at post-test compared between the intervention and control groups

OR – odds ratio; CI – confidence interval; NA – not applicable.

* OR derived from multilevel logistic regression analyses adjusted for region, gender, age and baseline measure (except for knowledge and self-efficacy) and allowing a random intercept for nurse and class level.

Unstandardised regression coefficient derived from multilevel linear regression analyses adjusted for region, gender, age and baseline measure (except for knowledge and self-efficacy) and allowing a random intercept for nurse and class level.

Mean (standard deviation).

Table 3 Self-reported vegetable intake and determinants at post-test compared between the intervention and control groups

OR – odds ratio; CI – confidence interval; NA – not applicable.

* OR derived from multilevel logistic regression analyses adjusted for region, gender, age and baseline measure (except for knowledge and self-efficacy) and allowing a random intercept for nurse and class level.

Unstandardised regression coefficient derived from multilevel linear regression analyses adjusted for region, gender, age and baseline measure (except for knowledge and self-efficacy) and allowing a random intercept for nurse and class level.

Mean (standard deviation).

Exposure to intervention

After receiving the Internet-tailored advice, 210 of the 263 children in the intervention group registered whether or not they had read the advice concerning fruit/vegetables. This resulted in 84% reporting that they had read both sets of advice completely, 8% had read part of the advice, 5% had read only the fruit or only the vegetable advice, and 3% had read nothing. Based on the nurses' registrations, 240 (91% of 263) children and one of their parents received at least some of the counselling. The nurses indicated that most of the counselling sessions concluded with encouraging the child's self-efficacy to change behaviour (about 42%) and with stimulating goal setting (about 21%). This indicates that for most of the children and parents in the intervention group at least knowledge of recommendations, awareness of personal intake, motivation to change and barriers to increase self-efficacy had been discussed.

Discussion

This cluster-randomised study indicated that an intervention combining Internet-tailored nutrition advice with dietary counselling by a nurse among elementary-school children resulted in better knowledge of the recommended vegetable intake levels and awareness of inadequate fruit intake, but no effects were found regarding actual intake levels. We note that, although effects were found for awareness, there were in fact some (non-significant) differences in awareness at baseline between the intervention and control groups.

We are not aware of any other controlled studies on similar interventions. A non-controlled study on a similar intervention among adolescents in primary care settings in the USA reported indications of increased fruit and vegetable intakesReference Patrick, Sallis, Prochaska, Lydston, Calfas and Zabinski26.

Our two-component intervention was compact and embedded in the existing practice of the preventive health care at schools, offering the benefit of a large reach and increasing the chance of future implementation. Counselling the child in the presence of one parent enabled involvement of the family; this is important because the social environment is a recognised facilitator for dietary behaviourReference Blanchette and Brug9, Reference Shepherd, Harden, Rees, Brunton, Garcia and Oliver10, Reference Vereecken, Keukelier and Maes27. The study had a high response rate (after receiving consent to participate from the parents) and had a high level of exposure to the intervention. Potentially, once taken out of the research mode and implemented in standard practice, there may be an even higher participation rate since a less strict informed consent procedure would be applied than in our study. Our sample was a representative study population, as it does not differ substantially in sociodemographic characteristics (gender, family status and ethnicity) from the general Dutch adolescent population when compared with nationwide data on schoolchildren in elementary schools28.

A possible limitation concerning the measures was the application of child self-reports. Self-administered questionnaires can be subject to response bias (socially desirable answers), thereby resulting in overreporting of healthy behaviour or positive beliefs. Unfortunately, the use of more reliable and valid measures often incurs greater costs and longer time investment, as well as increasing the burden for the participants (see also further below).

That only one outcome measurement (i.e. 3 months after the baseline measurement) was applied may be considered a limitation. Because our two-component intervention was very compact, perhaps only short-term effects of intake levels were present, which we did not measure. In future studies outcome measurements should be made at several points in time, including short-term outcome measurements.

We now discuss some possible explanations for the absence of effects of our two-component intervention regarding dietary intake, and make suggestions for improvement. Although Patrick et al. Reference Patrick, Sallis, Prochaska, Lydston, Calfas and Zabinski26 added mail and telephone contact to the two-component approach, this did not result in improved intakes. In contrast to that study, multi-component interventions with longer duration and more contact hours generally achieve better results than shorter programmesReference Blanchette and Brug9, Reference Shepherd, Harden, Rees, Brunton, Garcia and Oliver10. The current Internet-based two-component intervention may be regarded as an initial part of a broader approach, involving repetition of the current components or the addition of other componentsReference Doak8, Reference Shepherd, Harden, Rees, Brunton, Garcia and Oliver10. Furthermore, to improve effectiveness regarding intake levels, the current intervention components may need improvement. For instance, relying on the vegetable intake of the previous day is generally regarded as valid, but may have some limitations. For example, assessments that took place on Mondays might have resulted in lower reports of vegetable intake; in The Netherlands, Sundays are ‘easy or no cooking days’, as confirmed by Wind et al. Reference Wind, Bobelijn, De Bourdeaudhuij, Klepp and Brug17. When parents or children ‘feel’ that their assessment and therefore their personal advice is not representative, the advice may lack a positive effect. More comprehensive assessment may improve this, but this will also increase the burden on the respondents. Another solution may be to focus more on the advice and counselling concerning the relevance of eating sufficient vegetables ‘every day’. Additionally, the nurses reported that some children of this age may have a poor recognition of vegetables, e.g. not knowing whether the food was a vegetable or some other type of food, or to which category the type of vegetable in question belonged to (e.g. heated, salad or mixed). This has also been indicated in previous studiesReference Shepherd, Harden, Rees, Brunton, Garcia and Oliver10, Reference Edwards and Hartwell29. Adding pictures and more explanation about the questions in the assessment as well as improvement regarding the wording of questions might avoid this. An alternative approach might be more advice on how to recognise vegetablesReference Perez-Rodrigo, Wind, Hildonen, Bjelland, Aranceta and Klepp19. More research is needed to test whether these suggestions would indeed improve the effect on vegetable intake.

In conclusion, this study was unique for its school-based intervention integrated within preventive health care. The dual approach of advice and counselling guaranteed a repetition of the main health message, combined written and oral feedback, and involved both the school and home environment. Linking the two components by means of the Internet allowed automatic support of the dietary counselling, thus enhancing efficiency. The study showed that this compact, integrated two-component intervention induced positive changes in knowledge and awareness of dietary intake levels among schoolchildren. To induce changes in intake levels, more comprehensive interventions may be needed.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank the employees of the municipal health services in Vlaardingen and Harderwijk, the schools, the schoolchildren and their parents/guardians for their enthusiastic participation; Halime Kaya-Dag for data collection and other supportive tasks; and Saskia te Velde for helping with the analyses. The Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (ZonMw) #4010.0011 funded this research.

References

1Maynard, M, Gunnell, D, Emmett, P, Frankel, S, Davey Smith, G. Fruit, vegetables, and antioxidants in childhood and risk of adult cancer: the Boyd Orr cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 2003; 57(3): 218–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Paolini, M, Sapone, A, Canistro, D, Antonelli, MA, Chieco, P. Diet and risk of cancer. Lancet 2003; 361(9353): 257–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3World Health Organization (WHO). Diet, Nutrition and the Prevention of Chronic Diseases. Technical Report Series No. 916. Geneva: WHO, 2003; i–viii, 1149.Google Scholar
4Hung, HC, Joshipura, KJ, Jiang, R, Hu, FB, Hunter, D, Smith-Warner, SA, et al. . Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of major chronic disease. Journal of National Cancer Institute 2004; 96(21): 1577–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Yngve, A, Wolf, A, Poortvliet, E, Elmadfa, I, Brug, J, Ehrenblad, B, et al. . Fruit and vegetable intake in a sample of 11-year-old children in 9 European countries: The Pro Children Cross-sectional Survey. Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism 2005; 49(4): 236–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
6Lien, N, Lytle, LA, Klepp, KI. Stability in consumption of fruit, vegetables, and sugary foods in a cohort from age 14 to age 21. Preventive Medicine 2001; 33(3): 217–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7Mikkila, V, Rasanen, L, Raitakari, OT, Pietinen, P, Viikari, J. Longitudinal changes in diet from childhood into adulthood with respect to risk of cardiovascular diseases: The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2004; 58(7): 1038–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Doak, C. Large-scale interventions and programmes addressing nutrition-related chronic diseases and obesity: examples from 14 countries. Public Health Nutrition 2002; 5(1A): 275–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Blanchette, L, Brug, J. Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 2005; 18(6): 431–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10Shepherd, J, Harden, A, Rees, R, Brunton, G, Garcia, J, Oliver, S, et al. . Young people and healthy eating: a systematic review of research on barriers and facilitators. Health Education Research 2006; 21(2): 239–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Brug, J, Campbell, M, van Assema, P. The application and impact of computer-generated personalized nutrition education: a review of the literature. Patient Education and Counseling 1999; 36(2): 145–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12Brug, J, Oenema, A, Campbell, M. Past, present, and future of computer-tailored nutrition education. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 2003; 77(Suppl. 4): 1028S–34S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13Mangunkusumo, RT, Brug, J, Duisterhout, JS, de Koning, HJ, Raat, H. Feasibility, acceptability, and quality of Internet-administered adolescent health promotion in a preventive-care setting. Health Education Research 2006 May 19; Epub ahead of print.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14Kroeze, W, Werkman, A, Brug, J. A systematic review of randomized trials on the effectiveness of computer-tailored education on physical activity and dietary behaviors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 2006; 31(3): 205–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15Bere, E, Klepp, KI. Correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among Norwegian schoolchildren: parental and self-reports. Public Health Nutrition 2004; 7(8): 991–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16Bere, E, Klepp, KI. Changes in accessibility and preferences predict children's future fruit and vegetable intake. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 2005; 2: 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17Wind, M, Bobelijn, K, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, Klepp, KI, Brug, J. A qualitative exploration of determinants of fruit and vegetable intake among 10- and 11-year-old schoolchildren in the low countries. Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism 2005; 49(4): 228–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Wind, M, De Bourdeaudhuij, I, Te Velde, S, Sandvik, C, Due, P, Klepp, KI, et al. . Correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption among 11-year-old Belgian-Flemish and Dutch schoolchildren. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behaviour 2006; 38(4): 211–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Perez-Rodrigo, C, Wind, M, Hildonen, C, Bjelland, M, Aranceta, J, Klepp, KI, et al. . The pro children intervention: applying the intervention mapping protocol to develop a school-based fruit and vegetable promotion programme. Annals of Nutrition & Metabolism 2005; 49(4): 267–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20Wiggers, LC, Oort, FJ, Dijkstra, A, de Haes, JC, Legemate, DA, Smets, EM. Cognitive changes in cardiovascular patients following a tailored behavioral smoking cessation intervention. Preventive Medicine 2005; 40(6): 812–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Campbell, MK, Elbourne, DR, Altman, DG. CONSORT statement: extension to cluster randomised trials. British Medical Journal 2004; 328(7441): 702–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Keij, I. Aantallen allochtonen volgens verschillende definities [Number of Foreigners According to Different Definitions]. Voorburg/Heerlen: Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (Statistics Netherlands), 2000.Google Scholar
23Van Assema, P, Brug, J, Ronda, G, Steenhuis, I, Oenema, A. A short Dutch questionnaire to measure fruit and vegetable intake: relative validity among adults and adolescents. Nutrition and Health 2002; 16(2): 85106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24Twisk, JWR, ed. Missing data in longitudinal studies. In: Applied Longitudinal Data Analysis for Epidemiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2003; 202–24.Google Scholar
25Rasbash, J, Steele, F, Browne, W, Prosser, B. A User's Guide to MLwiN Version 2.0. Bristol: University of Bristol, 2005.Google Scholar
26Patrick, K, Sallis, JF, Prochaska, JJ, Lydston, DD, Calfas, KJ, Zabinski, MF, et al. . A multicomponent program for nutrition and physical activity change in primary care: PACE+ for adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 2001; 155(8): 940–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Vereecken, CA, Keukelier, E, Maes, L. Influence of mother's educational level on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite 2004; 43(1): 93103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Statistics Netherlands. Statline [online]. Available at http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb/start.asp?lp=Search/Search. Accessed 18 October 2005.Google Scholar
29Edwards, JS, Hartwell, HH. Fruit and vegetables – attitudes and knowledge of primary school children. Journal of Human Nutrition and Dietetics 2002; 15(5): 365–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Characteristics of the children at baseline by study group

Figure 1

Table 2 Self-reported fruit intake and determinants at post-test compared between the intervention and control groups

Figure 2

Table 3 Self-reported vegetable intake and determinants at post-test compared between the intervention and control groups