Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T15:12:47.878Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parenting practices are associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in pre-school children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 June 2009

Teresia M O’Connor*
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Sheryl O Hughes
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Kathy B Watson
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Tom Baranowski
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Theresa A Nicklas
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Jennie O Fisher
Affiliation:
Center for Obesity Research and Education, Department of Public Health, Temple University, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Alicia Beltran
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Janice C Baranowski
Affiliation:
USDA/ARS Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine, 1100 Bates Street, Houston, TX 77030, USA
Haiyan Qu
Affiliation:
Department of Health Services Administration, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
Richard M Shewchuk
Affiliation:
Department of Health Services Administration, University of Alabama at Birmingham, Birmingham, AL, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Parents may influence children’s fruit and vegetable (F&V) consumption in many ways, but research has focused primarily on counterproductive parenting practices, such as restriction and pressure to eat. The present study aimed to assess the association of diverse parenting practices to promote F&V and its consumption among pre-school children.

Design

An exploratory analysis was performed on cross-sectional data from 755 Head Start pre-school children and their parents collected in 2004–5. Data included parent practices to facilitate child F&V consumption (grouped into five categories); parent-reported dietary intake of their child over 3 d; and a number of potential correlates. K-means cluster analysis assigned parents to groups with similar use of the food parenting practice categories. Stepwise linear regression analyses investigated the association of parent clusters with children’s consumption of F&V, after controlling for potential confounding factors.

Results

A three-cluster solution provided the best fit (R2 = 0·62), with substantial differences in the use of parenting practices. The clusters were labelled Indiscriminate Food Parenting, Non-directive Food Parenting and Low-involved Food Parenting. Non-directive parents extensively used enhanced availability and teachable moments’ practices, but less firm discipline practices than the other clusters, and were significantly associated with child F&V intake (standardized β = 0·09, P < 0·1; final model R2 = 0·17) after controlling for confounders, including parental feeding styles.

Conclusions

Parents use a variety of parenting practices, beyond pressuring to eat and restrictive practices, to promote F&V intake in their young child. Evaluating the use of combinations of practices may provide a better understanding of parental influences on children’s F&V intake.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2009

High fruit and vegetable (F&V) consumption has been associated with a lower prevalence of obesity, type 2 diabetes and CHD in adults(Reference Bazzano, Serdula and Liu1). In children, higher vegetable(Reference Cullen, Baranowski, Klesges, Watson, Sherwood, Story, Zakeri, Leachman-Slawson and Pratt2) and F&V consumption(Reference Roseman, Yeung and Nickelsen3) were associated with lower BMI. However, most US children and adults are not eating the recommended amount of F&V(Reference Guenther, Dodd, Reedy and Krebs-Smith4). US pre-school children consumed approximately 80 % of the recommended daily fruit servings, but only 25 % of the recommended daily vegetable servings(Reference Dennison, Rockwell and Baker5). Identifying strategies to promote F&V consumption in young children is critical to promoting healthy nutrition and potentially lowering their risk of obesity and chronic diseases.

Parents likely influence children’s behaviour, including F&V consumption, in many ways. General parenting style has been defined as parents’ attitudes about childrearing combined with the emotional climate created by the parents through which parental practices are expressed(Reference Darling and Steinberg6). Parenting style specific to the feeding context has been termed ‘feeding style’(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7). Both general parenting style and its derivative, parental feeding style, have been related to children’s F&V consumption(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7Reference Patrick, Nicklas, Hughes and Morales9) and weight status(Reference Rhee, Appugliese, Kaciroti and Bradley10). For example, pre-school children whose parents used an authoritative feeding style consumed more vegetables than children whose parents used an authoritarian feeding style(Reference Patrick, Nicklas, Hughes and Morales9).

Parenting style has been differentiated from parenting practices, which are defined as specific parental actions or behaviours designed to influence children’s behaviour(Reference Darling and Steinberg6). Since it may be difficult to change parenting or feeding styles, targeting food parenting practices may offer more promise for future obesity prevention programmes. However, most of this literature has been cross-sectional and focused on counterproductive parental feeding practices; for example, parental control and pressure to eat were both negatively associated with children’s F&V consumption(Reference Fisher, Mitchell, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch11, Reference Wardle, Carnell and Cooke12). Greater understanding of food parenting practices that promote F&V consumption is needed. In addition, parents likely do not use only one type of food parenting practice when trying to influence their children’s intake. Therefore it is important to investigate how different types of parenting practices work in combination in promoting F&V consumption in young children.

Previous research on food parenting(Reference Vereecken, Keukelier and Maes13Reference Cullen, Baranowski, Rittenberry, Cosart, Hebert and de Moor15) and feeding styles(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7Reference Patrick, Nicklas, Hughes and Morales9) has utilized primarily an etic research method. The etic and emic approaches to research have been described in anthropology and psychology, and refer to the perspective taken to better understand cultures or behaviours(Reference Headland16Reference Morris, Leung, Ames and Lickel18). An etic approach relies upon an outsider’s perspective and in food-related parenting would be exemplified by having the investigator impose theory- or expert-based constructs to characterize and categorize feeding styles and food parenting practices. Alternatively, an emic approach(Reference Headland16Reference Morris, Leung, Ames and Lickel18) studies groups from within and allows the subjects (or insiders) to describe these phenomena from their own point of view. In food-related parenting, an emic approach would therefore involve parents self-identifying and categorizing parenting practices for promoting F&V intake in their pre-school children. The present study used an emic approach to: (i) cluster parents based on similarities of their reported use of feeding practices; (ii) describe the clusters based on parent and child characteristics; and (iii) relate children’s F&V consumption with parent cluster membership.

Methods

Design

An exploratory secondary analysis was performed on cross-sectional data, originally designed to investigate facilitators and barriers to F&V consumption among pre-school children attending Head Start centres in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama(Reference Hughes and Shewchuk19). The study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Boards at Baylor College of Medicine and the University of Alabama at Birmingham. Participants were recruited at Head Start centres before and after school in 2004–5. Staff members explained the purpose of the study, confidentiality was assured, and informed consent was obtained. The original study was conducted in two phases. During Phase I, parents participated in eight focus groups with eight to ten parents per group using the nominal group technique (NGT)(Reference Miller, Shewchuk, Elliot and Richards20) to identify behaviours parents used (parenting practices) to promote healthy eating such as F&V consumption in their 3- to 5-year-old children. NGT is a structured focus group method that uses a round-robin approach to give each member equal chance of participation to contribute potential F&V parenting practices. Participants were then asked to nominate the three parenting practices that were most successful for them in getting their child to eat healthily, in order to provide a rank-ordered list per meeting. An additional NGT was conducted with nine experts in the field to supplement the previously identified practices. After considering the substantive similarities among the items generated across meetings, a final list of thirty-three F&V-focused practices was identified, five which were provided only by the experts (items 2, 6, 10, 16 and 20). In Phase II a second sample of parents (n 761) completed a cross-sectional assessment, including a card sort to judge the similarity and distinctions among the thirty-three F&V parenting practices. Parents were instructed to group the parenting practices into two to ten piles, according to how alike they seemed. Following the card sorts, the parents were interviewed and asked if they used each of the thirty-three F&V parenting practices, using a dichotomous response (used/not used) for each practice. Parents were also asked to complete a demographic survey and a set of questionnaires described below. The current analysis uses Phase II data.

Anthropometric measures

Parents’ and children’s height and weight were obtained by trained staff following structured procedures(Reference Lohman, Roche and Martorell21). Height and weight measures for the children were converted to age- and gender-specific BMI Z-scores using the revised 2000 growth charts from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention(Reference Kuczmarski, Ogden, Guo, Grummer-Strawn, Flegal, Mei, Wei, Curtin, Roche and Johnson22). Parents’ height and weight were used to calculate BMI as (weight in kg)/(height in m)2.

Parent and child’s fruit and vegetable consumption

Parents’ and children’s dietary consumption was assessed by parent report using dietary recalls on three non-consecutive days (two weekdays and one weekend day). Parents were interviewed in-person or over the telephone by a trained dietitian using the US Department of Agriculture multiple-pass protocol, and were asked to report all food and beverages consumed in the past 24 h and what their child consumed while with the parent (excluding food consumed while away from the parent such as while at Head Start, since the parents could not report this accurately)(Reference Baranowski, Sprague, Baranowski and Harrison23). The family was provided with two-dimensional food models to be used in the telephone interviews. Dietary data were collected and analysed using the Nutrient Data System, University of Minnesota(Reference Lytle, Murray, Perry and Eldridge24). Child F&V intake was operationalized as the average servings of F&V consumed by the child after 15.00 hours over 3 d, excluding fried vegetables (such as fried potatoes) and fruit juices. Parent F&V intake was operationalized as the average servings of F&V consumed in a 24 h period over 3 d, excluding fried vegetables and fruit juices. F&V servings were analysed as a combined variable since the formative work identifying the parenting practices addressed them together. While partial day recalls do not fully reflect the child’s daily dietary intake, they do reflect the child’s intake that parents could directly influence. The reliability intra-class correlation coefficient across the three days of F&V consumption was 0·49 (95 % CI 0·43, 0·56) for children and 0·57 (95 % CI 0·51, 0·62) for parents.

Questionnaire measures

Caregiver’s Feeding Styles Questionnaire

It is important to differentiate the effect of specific parenting practices from parental feeding styles. The Caregiver’s Feeding Styles Questionnaire (CFSQ) was developed to measure general feeding styles in low-income parents of pre-school children(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7), consistent with a developmental parenting paradigm(Reference Maccoby and Martin25). The CFSQ includes two sub-scales: a Demandingness sub-scale of nineteen items and a Responsiveness sub-scale, calculated as a ratio of seven child-centred items relative to the mean of the total items(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7). A cross-classification of high and low scores based on median splits on the two sub-scales identified the four feeding styles: (i) Authoritative (high on both); (ii) Authoritarian (high demandingness, low responsiveness); (iii) Indulgent (low demandingness, high responsiveness); and (iv) Uninvolved (low on both). Confirmatory factor analyses supported the factorial integrity of this measure(Reference Hughes, Anderson, Power, Micheli, Jaramillo and Nicklas26). Coefficient α values for parent-centred and child-centred feeding were 0·86 and 0·71 in a previous Head Start sample(Reference Hughes, Power, Orlet Fisher, Mueller and Nicklas7) and 0·83 and 0·67 in the present sample.

Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire

Child temperament has been associated with the weight status of young children(Reference Agras, Hammer, McNicholas and Kraemer27, Reference Carey, Hegvik and McDevitt28) as well as picky eating in pre-school children(Reference Jacobi, Agras, Bryson and Hammer29), and could be a confounding variable. Therefore child temperament was measured by the Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire (CBQ), Very Short Form. It assesses child temperament, defined as constitutionally based, individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation(Reference Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey and Fisher30). The three CBQ sub-scales (Negative Affectivity, Effortful Control and Extraversion/Surgency) showed adequate internal consistency, with previously reported α values ranging from 0·66 to 0·70 for negative affectivity, from 0·62 to 0·78 for effortful control and from 0·70 to 0·76 for extraversion/surgency(Reference Putnam and Rothbart31). The factor structure was invariant across age groups and cultures. Convergent validity included parental agreement and prediction of social and laboratory behaviour patterns(Reference Rothbart, Ahadi, Hershey and Fisher30). Values of α in the current sample were 0·65 for negative affectivity, 0·74 for effortful control and 0·63 for extraversion/surgency.

Home fruit and vegetable availability questionnaire

Home availability of F&V has been shown to be a strong predictor of F&V consumption in school-aged children and adolescents(Reference Cullen, Baranowski, Owens, Marsh, Rittenberry and de Moor32Reference Jago, Baranowski and Baranowski34) and therefore a potential confounder in the present analysis. Home availability of F&V was assessed by a questionnaire with seventeen fruit and eighteen vegetable items. Previous studies reported α of 0·77 among parents of African-American girls(Reference Cullen, Klesges, Sherwood, Baranowski, Beech, Pratt, Zhou and Rochon35) and validation with home-inventory checks with parents of 4th and 6th graders(Reference Marsh, Cullen and Baranowski36). To maintain consistency between measured consumption and availability, high-fat vegetables were not included in the analysis resulting in a thirty-one-item F&V availability scale. The value of α in the current study sample was 0·79.

Marlow–Crowne social desirability scale, short version

A short form (ten items) of the Marlow–Crowne Social Desirability Scale(Reference Strahan and Gerbasi37) was used to measure and account for participating parents’ response bias towards socially desirable replies. The original research obtained factor loadings ranging from 0·28 to 0·54 with reliability coefficients of 0·49 to 0·75 in four separate populations(Reference Strahan and Gerbasi37). Internal consistency of the scale in the present study sample was 0·44.

Food parenting practice categories

Thirty-three parenting practices were grouped using a systematic cognitive mapping approach that involved a card sort, multidimensional scaling and hierarchical cluster analysis procedures, which have been used in previous studies(Reference Shewchuk and O’Connor38Reference Shewchuk, O’Connor, Williams and Savage41). Five parent-derived practice (PP) categories were generated: teachable moments (five items), practical methods (nine items), firm discipline (four items), restriction of junk foods (five items) and enhanced availability and accessibility (ten items; Table 1). Internal consistencies of PP category items varied from 0·41 to 0·58.

Table 1 Parent-generated feeding practice categories with proportion of reported use: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

F&V, fruit and vegetables.

Statistical analysis

Parents were clustered based on similar use of PP categories to create profiles of how parents used these practices in combination. Standardized PP category scores were subjected to a K-means cluster analysis to identify groups (clusters) of parents who used similar practices based on the PP category scores. K-means clustering minimizes the sum of squared distances between a cluster centre and all the points within the cluster. The model R 2 value and the relevance of the clusters(Reference Koehly, Arabie, Bradlow and Hutchinson42) were used to determine the optimal number of clusters. Clusters were labelled to provide meaningful interpretations of their parenting practice profiles.

Tests of independence and ANOVA were used to assess differences among the K-means PP clusters by demographic, anthropometric, psychosocial, and F&V availability and consumption characteristics. Post hoc comparisons for categorical variables were established by conducting additional tests of independence on all possible cluster pairs. Post hoc analyses for ANOVA were conducted using Tukey’s HSD (honestly significant difference). The level of significance for post hoc testing was adjusted accordingly. Pearson product moment correlations were used to investigate bivariate relationships among anthropometric, psychosocial, F&V availability and consumption characteristics of the study sample.

Two sets of stepwise linear regression analyses were used to investigate the association of (i) the individual PP category scores (i.e. teachable moments, practical methods, firm discipline, restriction of junk foods and enhanced availability/accessibility) and (ii) the combination of parenting practices (i.e. the clusters) with children’s consumption of F&V, while controlling for potential confounding factors. Statistical significance was set at P < 0·10, given the exploratory nature of these analyses(Reference Baghi, Noorbaloochi and Moore43).

Results

Six parents did not complete any portion of the demographic survey or set of questionnaires. The remaining 755 parents provided data for determining PP cluster membership. The rest of the analyses utilized the 662 parent/child dyads that provided complete data. Analyses revealed no significant differences between parents providing complete and incomplete data (Tables 2 and 3). The majority of the parents/guardians were female (95 %); 290 (43·8 %) were African-American, 197 (29·8 %) were Hispanic and 175 (26·4 %) white. The average child age was 4·5 (sd 0·6) years and the average child BMI Z-score was 0·8 (sd 1·5). Approximately 60 % of the children were normal weight (5th percentile ≤ BMI > 85th percentile for age and gender), 15 % were overweight (85th percentile ≤ BMI > 95th percentile for age and gender) and 25 % were obese (BMI ≥ 95th percentile for age and gender)(Reference Barlow44).

Table 2 Demographic characteristics and parental feeding styles by parenting practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

F&V, fruit and vegetables.

Note: No significant differences between Total (included) and Excluded; missing category not included in analyses.

†Significant association (χ 2(4) = 14·15, P = 0·007) between cluster and race/ethnicity. Follow-up χ 2 tests of independence by cluster pairs yielded significant (P < 0·0167) differences between the distributions of the Low-Involved Food Parenting cluster and the Non-directive Food Parenting cluster.

‡Significant association (χ 2(6) = 83·90, P < 0·001) between cluster and feeding style. Post hoc χ 2 tests of independence by cluster pairs yielded significant (P < 0·0167) differences between the distributions for all pairwise comparisons.

Table 3 Mean, standard deviation and ANOVA results of participant characteristics by parent practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

F&V, fruit and vegetables.

Note: No significant global effects between those included and excluded from the analyses.

†Significant (P < 0·05) global effect; post hoc testing revealed a significant difference between Low-Involved Food Parenting and Indiscriminate Food Parenting clusters (P < 0·0167).

‡Significant (P < 0·05) global effect; post hoc testing showed no significant difference at P < 0·0167.

Identification of parent practice clusters

The model R 2 values for the K-means cluster analysis were 0·48, 0·62 and 0·69 for the two-, three- and four-cluster models, respectively (data not shown). Although the four-cluster model showed slight improvement in variation, given the marginal improvement in model fit of the four-cluster model coupled with the finding that one cluster accounted for less than 10 % of the parents, the three-cluster model was deemed to exhibit the best fit. Post hoc analyses demonstrated that use of the feeding practices was significantly different for the three PP clusters of parents (Fig. 1). At the extremes, parents in cluster 1 (Low-involved Food Parenting) reported low use of all practices intended to promote F&V intake (range 42–70 %), while parents in cluster 3 (Indiscriminate Food Parenting) reported using at least 75 % of practices in each PP category. A high proportion of cluster 2 (Non-directive Food Parenting) parents reported the use of enhanced availability and teachable moments to promote F&V intake in their children (92 % and 84 %, respectively), but reported lower use of practices in the firm discipline category (30 %).

Fig. 1 Reported use (proportion) of each parenting practice category by K-means derived parenting practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

Correlates of the parent practice clusters

The three parenting clusters revealed significant differences by ethnicity (χ 2(4) = 14·15, P = 0·007) and feeding styles (χ 2(6) = 83·90, P < 0·001; Table 2). Post hoc analysis demonstrated a significantly different distribution by ethnicity for Low-involved Food Parenting and the Non-directive Food Parenting groups (P < 0·0167), with proportionally higher representation of African-Americans in the Low-Involved Parenting cluster. Feeding styles were significantly different for all pairwise distributions (P < 0·0167) with the Low-involved Food Parenting group having the highest representation of uninvolved feeding style (35·7 %); the Non-directive Food Parenting group having the highest representation of indulgent feeding (47·9 %); and the Indiscriminate Food Parenting group having high representation of authoritarian feeding (40·6 %). ANOVA of differences among PP clusters demonstrated significant (P < 0·05) global effects for parental age, child age and child BMI Z-score (Table 3). Post hoc analyses showed that mean parental age of 29·6 (sd 6·4) years and child age of 4·3 (sd 0·7) years among the Low-involved Food Parenting cluster were significantly (P < 0·0167) less than the mean parental age of 32·8 (sd 9·2) years and child age of 4·5 (sd 0·6) years among the Indiscriminate Food Parenting cluster. Post hoc testing yielded no pairwise differences in child BMI Z-scores. No other significant differences were detected across PP clusters.

Correlations of parent practice categories and parent practice clusters

Practical methods was the only PP category that was bivariately correlated with child F&V intake (r = 0·08, P < 0·05). Other variables that were significantly correlated with child F&V intake were parent F&V intake (r = 0·50, P < 0·01), home availability of F&V (r = 0·20, P < 0·01), child’s age (r = 0·15, P < 0·01), parent’s BMI (r = −0·08, P < 0·05) and child’s negative affect (r = −0·08, P < 0·05; Table 4). All five of the parenting practice categories were significantly inter-correlated, ranging from 0·12 to 0·45 (Table 4), suggesting that parents did not use practices in isolation but rather in combination.

Table 4 Correlations among BMI status, age and psychosocial model covariates: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5 (n 662)

*Correlation is significant at the 0·05 level (two-tailed).

**Correlation is significant at the 0·01 level (two-tailed).

Association of parent practice categories and clusters with child’s fruit and vegetable consumption

Stepwise linear regression analysis with the child’s home F&V consumption as the dependent variable and the PP categories as the independent variable, while accounting for potential confounders, resulted in a final model with R 2 of 0·17 (data not shown). None of the five PP categories was significantly associated with the child’s F&V consumption. Analysis with PP clusters replacing PP category scores in block 4 resulted in a final model with R 2 of 0·17 (Table 5). Prior to controlling for feeding style (block 4), both the Non-directive (standardized β = 0·09, P < 0·1) and the Indiscriminate Food Parenting clusters (standardized β = 0·09, P < 0·1) were associated with children’s F&V intake. When adding the feeding style to the model (block 5), the Non-directive Food Parenting cluster (standardized β = 0·09, P < 0·1) continued to be significantly associated with child F&V consumption. When the parent’s F&V intake was included in block 1 of the stepwise regression model presented in Table 5 (data not shown), there was no longer a significant relationship between the parent clusters and child F&V intake.

Table 5 Results from stepwise linear regression analysis of demographics characteristics, psychosocial correlates, feeding styles and parent practices clusters on child F&V intake: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5 (n 662)

F&V, fruit and vegetables; Std β, standardized beta coefficient.

*Significant at P < 0·10.

**Significant at P < 0·05.

Discussion

To our knowledge, the present study is the first to use an emic approach to understand low-income parents’ perspective of how they try to get their pre-school children to consume F&V and how their own food parenting behaviours relate to one another. This approach included: (i) engaging parents to generate a list of feeding practices they typically used; (ii) asking parents to organize these feeding practices into PP categories; (iii) testing whether parents could be clustered into patterns of use of the parenting categories; and (iv) investigating how these PP categories and clusters were associated with children’s F&V intake. This approach is different from previous feeding practice research, which has focused primarily on understanding the effects of restrictive and controlling practices on F&V consumption in children(Reference Fisher, Mitchell, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch11, Reference Wardle, Carnell and Cooke12). It is noteworthy that some of the parenting practices which on the surface may appear to be similar were placed in different categories. However, the resulting parenting practice categories represent how parents collectively view the similarities and relationships of these F&V parenting practices.

There was no significant association of the five PP categories with children’s F&V consumption. Alternatively, when combined into clusters, the Non-directive Food Parenting cluster retained a significant positive association with children’s home F&V consumption (Table 5) even after controlling for various other factors that might influence child F&V consumption, including parental feeding style. This association was lost if parental F&V consumption was included in the model. Given that parental and child F&V intake are likely correlated for several reasons – such as a shared home availability and accessibility of F&V (typically determined by the parent), parental role-modelling of dietary behaviours, and shared genetic and behavioural-based preferences for F&V – its role within the framework of F&V parenting practices may be difficult to interpret. In addition, the parents reported both their own and their child’s dietary intake, inducing a likely significant correlation by the common reporting bias/error for the parent and the child’s intake.

Parents in the Non-directive Food Parenting cluster were different from parents in the other two clusters because they tended to use parenting practices in the enhanced availability/accessibility and teachable moments categories more frequently, with lower use of practices in the firm discipline category (Fig. 1). Parenting practices in the firm discipline category rely on controlling practices that are highly directive and external to the child (such as threats, rewards, pressure to eat and restriction)(Reference Reynolds, Baranowski, Bishop, Farris, Binkley, Nicklas and Elmer46). Several of these have been shown to be counterproductive in getting children to eat F&V(Reference Fisher, Mitchell, Smiciklas-Wright and Birch11, Reference Wardle, Carnell and Cooke12). On the other hand, teachable moments may also be considered a type of control, but is non-directive and allows for child autonomy(Reference Reynolds, Baranowski, Bishop, Farris, Binkley, Nicklas and Elmer46). Parenting practices that increase the home availability of F&V may be considered a way to structure the feeding environment to maximize the likelihood that children will consume F&V(Reference Hughes, O’Connor and Power45). Home F&V availability has consistently been shown to be a predictor of F&V consumption in school-aged children and adolescents(Reference Cullen, Baranowski, Owens, Marsh, Rittenberry and de Moor32Reference Jago, Baranowski and Baranowski34) and was a predictor of child F&V intake in our models.

The concept of authoritative food parenting grew from the general parenting style literature. Hughes et al. proposed that warmth, control and structure are dimensions that provide a more comprehensive framework to identify authoritative food parenting practices and investigate parental influences on children’s eating behaviours(Reference Hughes, O’Connor and Power45). Authoritative food parenting practices likely include those that are warm (nurturing), provide proactive structure for healthy eating, and use more non-directive controlling practices to promote F&V intake in children. The emic approach used in the current study identified additional F&V parenting practices and broadens parenting practices to include both positive and negative practices that may fall into these three dimensions. The exploratory analysis found the combination of F&V parenting practices that provide structure through practices such as enhanced home availability/accessibility and non-directive control through teachable moments with low use of highly controlling directive practices, such as firm discipline, may be a constellation of authoritative practices that promote F&V consumption in young children. These relationships need to be investigated utilizing etic approaches to further develop the authoritative food parenting framework and validate measures of these three dimensions of warmth, structure and type of control and test them in longitudinal observational and experimental study designs.

The analysis also found ethnicity to be significantly associated with F&V consumption in children. Hispanic ethnicity was associated with greater F&V consumption in children in our sample, which supports ethnic differences in F&V consumption among children(Reference Reynolds, Baranowski, Bishop, Farris, Binkley, Nicklas and Elmer46) and adults(Reference Thompson, Midthune, Subar, McNeel, Berrigan and Kipnis47) identified in other studies. Further research is needed to clarify the role that culture has on children’s F&V consumption.

The strengths of the present research are the large sample from two regions of the country and data that reflect the perspective of the parent in trying to encourage their child to eat F&V. In addition, the parent/child dyads who participated in this study were recruited from Head Start centres and represent a low-income sample with high participation of ethnic minorities. Alternatively, several limitations include the generalizability of these results to other populations of families; and all of the behavioural data were based on self-report, which may have reporting biases. A measure was used in order to statistically correct for possible social desirability reporting bias, but it had low internal consistency in our sample. Other research subsequently found problems with brief measures of social desirability(Reference Barger48). Further research will need to assess whether using longer, better measures of social desirability increases correlations of interest. Information about the frequency and consistency of use of these parenting practices were not obtained. Parents who frequently and consistently used these practices likely may have different effects on their child’s behaviour than parents who infrequently or inconsistently used these parenting practices.

It will be important to replicate these findings with refined measurement tools in other families with young children of varying socio-economic and ethnicity/racial backgrounds. Preferably, this would be done in a longitudinal sample to assess how the PP categories and PP clusters are related to young children’s F&V consumption over time. Parenting practices that have an effect on the child’s immediate consumption of certain foods (such as vegetables) may not be effective or even have the opposite effect in the long term. If these relationships of food parenting clusters to child consumption are replicated in future studies, experimental studies would be warranted to test the effect of training parents to use the authoritative feeding practices, while avoiding the use of others, to facilitate child F&V consumption.

Conclusions

The exploratory analyses presented here suggest that evaluating parents’ use of combinations of F&V parenting practices may provide richer understanding of parental influences on children’s nutrition behaviours. Further research is needed to verify that using a combination of F&V parenting practices that provide structure and non-directive control, with avoidance of over-controlling parenting practices, is associated with higher F&V consumption in children by replicating these findings using more refined measurement techniques in longitudinal samples and in controlled experimental designs.

Acknowledgements

Sources of funding: The present research was supported by funds from the National Cancer Institute (Grant R01 CA102671). This work is also a publication of the US Department of Agriculture/Agricultural Research Service (USDA/ARS) Children’s Nutrition Research Center, Department of Pediatrics, Baylor College of Medicine (Houston, TX) funded in part by the USDA/ARS (Cooperative Agreement 6250-51000). The contents of this publication do not necessarily reflect the views or policies of the USDA, nor does the mention of trade names, commercial products or organizations imply endorsement from the US government. Conflict of interest declaration: We have no conflict of interest to report. Contribution of authors: R.M.S., T.A.N. and S.O.H. collaborated on the initial grant examining F&V intake in Head Start children. R.M.S. and H.Q. performed the cognitive mapping of the card sorts to identify the parenting practice categories. K.B.W. performed all other statistical analysis. All authors contributed to the conceptualization and interpretation of the analysis. T.M.O. drafted and coordinated the review of the manuscript. All authors critically reviewed drafts of the manuscript and approved the final version of the manuscript. Acknowledgements: The authors wish to thank Frank Franklin, Professor and Chair, Department of Maternal and Child Health, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Public Health and the Principal Investigator at the Birmingham site of the larger grant examining F&V intake in Head Start children; as well as Michelle Feese, the Project Coordinator. The authors also thank the children and parents of Head Start who participated in the study.

References

1.Bazzano, LA, Serdula, MK & Liu, S (2003) Dietary intake of fruits and vegetables and risk of cardiovascular disease. Curr Atheroscler Rep 5, 492499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Klesges, LM, Watson, K, Sherwood, NE, Story, M, Zakeri, I, Leachman-Slawson, D & Pratt, C (2004) Anthropometric, parental, and psychosocial correlates of dietary intake of African-American girls. Obes Res 12, Suppl., 20S31S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Roseman, MG, Yeung, WK & Nickelsen, J (2007) Examination of weight status and dietary behaviors of middle school students in Kentucky. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 11391145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Guenther, PM, Dodd, KW, Reedy, J & Krebs-Smith, SM (2006) Most Americans eat much less than recommended amounts of fruits and vegetables. J Am Diet Assoc 106, 13711379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Dennison, BA, Rockwell, HL & Baker, SL (1998) Fruit and vegetable intake in young children. J Am Coll Nutr 17, 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Darling, N & Steinberg, L (1993) Parenting style as context: an integrative model. Psychol Bull 113, 487496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
7.Hughes, SO, Power, TG, Orlet Fisher, J, Mueller, S & Nicklas, TA (2005) Revisiting a neglected construct: parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite 44, 8392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8.Kremers, SP, Brug, J, de Vries, H & Engels, RC (2003) Parenting style and adolescent fruit consumption. Appetite 41, 4350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Patrick, H, Nicklas, T, Hughes, T & Morales, M (2005) The benefit of authoritative feeding style: caregiver feeding styles and children’s food consumption. Appetite 44, 243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Rhee, K, Appugliese, D, Kaciroti, N & Bradley, R (2006) Parenting styles and overweight in first grade. Pediatrics 117, 20472054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Fisher, JO, Mitchell, DC, Smiciklas-Wright, H & Birch, LL (2002) Parental influences on young girls’ fruit and vegetable, micronutrient, and fat intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 102, 5864.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12.Wardle, J, Carnell, S & Cooke, L (2005) Parental control over feeding and children’s fruit and vegetable intake: how are they related? J Am Diet Assoc 105, 227232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Vereecken, CA, Keukelier, E & Maes, L (2004) Influence of mother’s educational level on food parenting practices and food habits of young children. Appetite 43, 93103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.van der Horst, K, Kremers, S, Ferreira, I, Singh, A, Oenema, A & Brug, J (2007) Perceived parenting style and practices and the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages by adolescents. Health Educ Res 22, 295304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Rittenberry, L, Cosart, C, Hebert, D & de Moor, C (2001) Child-reported family and peer influences on fruit, juice and vegetable consumption: reliability and validity of measures. Health Educ Res 16, 187200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Headland, T (2008) Chapter 1 Introduction: A dialogue between Kenneth Pike and Marvin Harris on Emics and Etics. In Emics and Etics: The Insider/Outsider Debate. http://www.sil.org/~headlandt/ee-intro.htm (accessed October 2008).Google Scholar
17.Pike, KL (1990) On the emics and etics of Pike and Harris. In Emics and Etics: The Insider/Outsider Debate, pp. 2847. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
18.Morris, MW, Leung, K, Ames, D & Lickel, B (1999) Views from the inside and outside: integrating emic and etic insights about culture and justice judgment. Acad Manag Rev 24, 781796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
19.Hughes, SO & Shewchuk, RM (2006) The differential impact of feeding strategies on children’s intake and BMI among a tri-ethnic sample of Head Start families. Poster presented at the Obesity Society’s Annual Scientific Meeting. Boston, Massachusetts. Obesity 14, A85.Google Scholar
20.Miller, D, Shewchuk, R, Elliot, TR & Richards, S (2000) Nominal group technique: a process for identifying diabetes self-care issues among patients and caregivers. Diabetes Educ 26, 305310, 312, 314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Lohman, TG, Roche, AF & Martorell, M (1988) Anthropometric Standardization Reference Manual. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
22.Kuczmarski, R, Ogden, C, Guo, S, Grummer-Strawn, LM, Flegal, KM, Mei, Z, Wei, R, Curtin, LR, Roche, AF & Johnson, CL (2002) 2000 CDC Growth charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat 11 246, 1190.Google Scholar
23.Baranowski, T, Sprague, D, Baranowski, JH & Harrison, JA (1991) Accuracy of maternal dietary recall for preschool children. J Am Diet Assoc 91, 669674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Lytle, L, Murray, D, Perry, C & Eldridge, A (1998) Validating fourth-grade students’ self-report of dietary intake: results from the 5 a day power plus program. J Am Diet Assoc 98, 570572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
25.Maccoby, E & Martin, J (1983) Socialization in the context of the family: parent–child interaction. In Handbook of Child Psychology, pp. 1101 [PH Mussen, editor]. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
26.Hughes, SO, Anderson, CB, Power, TG, Micheli, N, Jaramillo, S & Nicklas, TA (2006) Measuring feeding in low-income African-American and Hispanic parents. Appetite 46, 215223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Agras, WS, Hammer, LD, McNicholas, F & Kraemer, HC (2004) Risk factors for childhood overweight: a prospective study from birth to 9.5 years. J Pediatr 145, 2025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Carey, WB, Hegvik, RL & McDevitt, SC (1988) Temperamental factors associated with rapid weight gain and obesity in middle childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr 9, 194198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Jacobi, C, Agras, WS, Bryson, S & Hammer, LD (2003) Behavioral validation, precursors, and concomitants of picky eating in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 42, 7684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30.Rothbart, MK, Ahadi, SA, Hershey, K & Fisher, P (2001) Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Child Dev 72, 13941408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Putnam, SP & Rothbart, MK (2006) Development of short and very short forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J Pers Assess 87, 102112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
32.Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Owens, E, Marsh, T, Rittenberry, L & de Moor, C (2003) Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100 % fruit juice, and vegetables influence children’s dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav 30, 615626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Neumark-Sztainer, D, Wall, M, Perry, C & Story, M (2003) Correlates of fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents. Findings from Project EAT. Prev Med 37, 198208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.Jago, R, Baranowski, T & Baranowski, JC (2007) Fruit and vegetable availability: a micro environmental mediating variable? Public Health Nutr 10, 681689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Cullen, KW, Klesges, LM, Sherwood, NE, Baranowski, T, Beech, B, Pratt, C, Zhou, A & Rochon, J (2004) Measurement characteristics of diet-related psychosocial questionnaires among African-American parents and their 8- to 10-year-old daughters: results from the Girls’ health Enrichment Multi-site Studies. Prev Med 38, Suppl., S34S42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Marsh, T, Cullen, K & Baranowski, T (2003) Validation of a fruit, juice, and vegetable availability questionnaire. J Nutr Educ Behav 35, 9396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37.Strahan, R & Gerbasi, KC (1972) Short, homogeneous versions of the Marlowe–Crowne Social Desirability Scale. J Clin Psychol 28, 191193.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38.Shewchuk, R & O’Connor, SJ (2002) Using cognitive concept mapping to understand what health care means to the elderly: an illustrative approach for planning and marketing. Health Mark Q 20, 6988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
39.Shewchuk, RM, Franklin, FA, Harrington, KH, Davies, SL & Windle, M (2004) Using cognitive mapping procedures to develop a community-based family intervention. Am J Health Behav 28, 4353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
40.Shewchuk, RM, Rivera, PA, Elliott, TR & Adams, AM (2004) Using cognitive mapping to understand problems experienced by family caregivers of persons with severe physical disabilities. J Clin Psychol Med Settings 11, 141150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41.Shewchuk, RM, O’Connor, SJ, Williams, ES & Savage, GT (2006) Beyond rankings: using cognitive mapping to understand what health care journals represent. Soc Sci Med 62, 11921204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Koehly, L, Arabie, P, Bradlow, E & Hutchinson, W (2001) How do I choose the optimal number of clusters in cluster analysis? In Methodological and Statistical Concerns of the Experimental Behavioral Researcher [D Iacobucci, editor]. J Consum Psychol 10, 102104.Google Scholar
43.Baghi, H, Noorbaloochi, S & Moore, J (2007) Statistical and nonstatistical significance: implications for health care researchers. Qual Manag Health Care 16, 104112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44.Barlow, SE & Expert Committee (2007) Expert committee recommendations regarding the prevention, assessment, and treatment of child and adolescent overweight and obesity: summary report. Pediatrics 120, Suppl. 4, S164S192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Hughes, SO, O’Connor, TM & Power, TG (2008) Parenting and children’s eating patterns: examining control in a broader context. Int J Child Adolesc Health 1, 323330.Google Scholar
46.Reynolds, KD, Baranowski, T, Bishop, DB, Farris, RP, Binkley, D, Nicklas, TA & Elmer, PJ (1999) Patterns in child and adolescent consumption of fruit and vegetables: effects of gender and ethnicity across four sites. J Am Coll Nutr 18, 248254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Thompson, FE, Midthune, D, Subar, AF, McNeel, T, Berrigan, D & Kipnis, V (2005) Dietary intake estimates in the National Health Interview Survey, 2000: methodology, results, and interpretation. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 352363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48.Barger, S (2002) The Marlowe–Crowne affair: short forms, psychometric structure, and social desirability. J Pers Assess 79, 286305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Parent-generated feeding practice categories with proportion of reported use: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

Figure 1

Table 2 Demographic characteristics and parental feeding styles by parenting practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

Figure 2

Table 3 Mean, standard deviation and ANOVA results of participant characteristics by parent practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

Figure 3

Fig. 1 Reported use (proportion) of each parenting practice category by K-means derived parenting practices cluster: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5

Figure 4

Table 4 Correlations among BMI status, age and psychosocial model covariates: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5 (n 662)

Figure 5

Table 5 Results from stepwise linear regression analysis of demographics characteristics, psychosocial correlates, feeding styles and parent practices clusters on child F&V intake: Head Start pre-school children and their parents in Houston, Texas and northern Alabama, 2004–5 (n 662)