Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-16T17:25:53.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parental work hours and household income as determinants of unhealthy food and beverage intake in young Australian children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 February 2022

Chelsea E Mauch*
Affiliation:
Caring Futures Institute, College of Nursing and Health Sciences, Level 7, SAHMRI building, North Terrace, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA5000, Australia Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood Centre of Research Excellence, Sydney, Australia
Thomas P Wycherley
Affiliation:
Alliance for Research in Exercise, Nutrition and Activity, Allied Health and Human Performance, University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia
Lucinda K Bell
Affiliation:
Caring Futures Institute, College of Nursing and Health Sciences, Level 7, SAHMRI building, North Terrace, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA5000, Australia
Rachel A Laws
Affiliation:
Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood Centre of Research Excellence, Sydney, Australia Institute for Physical Activity and Nutrition, School of Exercise and Nutrition Science, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia
Rebecca Byrne
Affiliation:
Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood Centre of Research Excellence, Sydney, Australia Queensland University of Technology, School of Exercise and Nutrition Sciences, Faculty of Health, Centre for Children’s Health Research, South Brisbane, Australia
Rebecca K Golley
Affiliation:
Caring Futures Institute, College of Nursing and Health Sciences, Level 7, SAHMRI building, North Terrace, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA5000, Australia Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood Centre of Research Excellence, Sydney, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

This study examined parental work hours and household income as determinants of discretionary (energy-dense, nutrient-poor) food and beverage intake in young children, including differences by eating occasion.

Design:

Secondary analysis of cross-sectional data. Three hierarchical regression models were conducted with percentage of energy from discretionary food and beverages across the day, at main meals and at snack times being the outcomes. Dietary intake was assessed by 1 × 24-h recall and 1–2 × 24-h food record(s). Both maternal and paternal work hours were included, along with total household income. Covariates included household, parent and child factors.

Setting:

Data from the NOURISH/South Australian Infants Dietary Intake studies were collected between 2008 and 2013.

Participants:

Participants included 526 mother–child dyads (median (interquartile range) child age 1·99 (1·96, 2·03) years). Forty-one percentage of mothers did not work while 57 % of fathers worked 35–40 h/week. Most (85 %) households had an income of ≥$50 k AUD/year.

Results:

Household income was consistently inversely associated with discretionary energy intake (β = –0·12 to –0·15). Maternal part-time employment (21–35 h/week) predicted child consumption of discretionary energy at main meals (β = 0·10, P = 0·04). Paternal unemployment predicted a lower proportion of discretionary energy at snacks (β = -0·09, P = 0·047).

Conclusions:

This work suggests that household income should be addressed as a key opportunity-related barrier to healthy food provision in families of young children. Strategies to reduce the time burden of healthy main meal provision may be required in families where mothers juggle longer part-time working hours with caregiving and domestic duties. The need to consider the role of fathers and other parents/caregivers in shaping children’s intake was also highlighted.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Excess intakes of energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods and beverages are typical of modern, global dietary patterns(13). This is contributing to the high rates of obesity and non-communicable diseases such as CVD and diabetes(4). Termed ‘discretionary’ foods and beverages in Australia(5), the mean intake of energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods and beverages amongst adults was more than twice the recommended maximum daily serves in 2011–2012(Reference Fayet-Moore, McConnell and Cassettari6). Similarly, the 2001–2003 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey data from the USA found that 96 % of adults consumed excessive energy from solid fats, added sugars and alcohol(Reference Krebs-Smith, Guenther and Subar2). Furthermore, the consumption of excess discretionary foods and beverages begins from as early as the second year of life(1,Reference Krebs-Smith, Guenther and Subar2,Reference Spence, Campbell and Lioret7) and increases over time(Reference Spence, Campbell and Lioret7). As the early years are critical in the development of food preferences(Reference Birch and Doub8), addressing the early intake of discretionary foods and beverages may prevent unhealthy dietary intake in adulthood.

The determinants of dietary intake are broad, including factors at the individual, household and community levels(Reference Zarnowiecki, Dollman and Parlette9,Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski10) . Figure 1 outlines child, parent and household-level factors and their proposed relationship with children’s intake of discretionary foods and beverages and was adapted from similar models of the home and family food environment(Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski10,Reference Vaughn, Ward and Fisher11) . Some of these relationships are well supported by research in young children, such as the role of child eating behaviours(Reference Birch and Fisher12,Reference Birch13) and parental feeding practices(Reference Daniels14), while less is understood about the role of family or household-level factors as determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake. Innate food preferences and child eating behaviours play a key role in children’s acceptance of food and beverages(Reference Birch and Doub8,Reference Wardle, Guthrie and Sanderson15) . However, child preferences and behaviours are set amongst, and shaped by, the home food environment, which includes the resources, structures and behaviours leading to the availability and provision of food to children(Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski10). Parental and household factors such as parental intake and the availability and accessibility of food are strong, consistent determinants of child dietary intake(Reference Pearson, Biddle and Gorely16). Therefore, parents are commonly targeted as key agents of change in interventions addressing the dietary intake of young children(Reference Golley, Hendrie and Slater17).

Fig. 1 Conceptual model of determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake, with child factors in dark blue, parent factors in light blue and external family/household factors in white

According to the COM-B framework of behaviour, a combination of capability (C), opportunity (O) and motivation (M) is required in order to perform a behaviour(Reference Michie, van Stralen and West18). For example, the behaviour ‘parental food provision’ requires nutrition and food knowledge, and cooking skills (capability), a desire, intention or habits that facilitate healthy food provision (motivation) and adequate time, money and other resources to plan, purchase and prepare healthy food and meals (opportunity). Interventions to reduce children’s discretionary food and beverage intake have mainly focused on parental capability and motivation, targeting factors such as child eating behaviour, parental feeding practices, nutrition knowledge and cooking skills, and self-efficacy(Reference Campbell, Lioret and McNaughton19Reference Wen, Baur and Simpson23). The impact of interventions to reduce young children’s intake of discretionary foods and beverages has so far been modest, suggesting a need to expand this focus to encompass opportunity-related determinants of young children’s dietary intake(Reference Johnson, Zarnowiecki and Hendrie21).

Time and money are important determinants of healthy dietary intake in adults(Reference Venn and Strazdins24). Research in school-age children provides evidence of the role of these determinants with respect to discretionary food and beverage intake. A cross-sectional study with 9–13-year-old Australian children found that attitudes, self-efficacy, parental feeding practices and home food availability were important determinants of discretionary food and beverage intake(Reference Zarnowiecki, Parletta and Dollman25). Markers of socio-economic position, such as maternal education, income and employment, moderated the relationship between determinants and intake. Furthermore, the amount of time mothers spent in employment seemed to be more important than maternal occupation(Reference Zarnowiecki, Parletta and Dollman25). These findings suggest that parental work hours and household income play an important role in the discretionary food and beverage intake of school-age children. Their role as determinants of young children’s intake is yet to be investigated however and may be different due to variations in parental influence on child dietary intake over the life course(Reference Li, Akaliyski and Schafer26).

The time and money required for the provision of food may not be consistent across eating occasions. In Australia, the typical eating pattern consists of three main meals, namely breakfast, lunch and the evening meal, and in young children, close to three snacks per d(Reference Fayet-Moore, Peters and McConnell27). The food and beverages consumed at these eating occasions differ(Reference Fayet-Moore, Peters and McConnell27,Reference Sui, Raubenheimer and Rangan28) . For example, snacks commonly feature ready-to-eat foods such as sweet biscuits and salty snacks, which require minimal preparation(Reference Fayet-Moore, Peters and McConnell27,Reference Sui, Raubenheimer and Rangan28) , whereas the evening meal which traditionally incorporates meat, vegetables and grains(Reference Sui, Raubenheimer and Rangan28) may take more time to plan, purchase and prepare. Qualitative evidence suggests that main meals are more time intensive to provide, with low-income employed parents citing time scarcity as a key barrier to healthy evening meal provision(Reference Jabs, Devine and Bisogni29,Reference Pescud and Pettigrew30) . The purchase of fast food after a day at work has been described as a response to time scarcity(Reference Jabs, Devine and Bisogni29), whereas in a discrete choice experiment, time and money were not found to be important to parents in the selection of snacks for their 3–7-year-old children compared with the influence of child resistance and co-parent support(Reference Johnson, Golley and Zarnowiecki31). Sub-group analyses found that cost was more important to parents living in lower socio-economic areas, compared with those living in higher socio-economic areas(Reference Johnson, Golley and Zarnowiecki31). Differences in the determinants of intake across eating occasions would warrant the tailoring of intervention strategies to meal type, although quantitative evidence is required to support this.

This study aimed to examine parental work hours and household income as determinants of discretionary food and beverage intake in young children, and whether their influence differs according to eating occasion. This study will provide evidence regarding opportunity-related determinants of young children’s intake of discretionary foods and beverages, supporting the development of intervention strategies covering all aspects of the COM-B framework(Reference Michie, van Stralen and West18). Finally, the exploration of the role of parental work hours and household income on young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake at different eating occasions will support more targeted and potentially effective intervention strategies.

Methods

Study design, setting and sample

This study was a secondary analysis of cross-sectional data collected as part of the NOURISH and South Australian Infants Dietary Intake studies, conducted in South Australia and Queensland, Australia, between 2008 and 2013. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology-Nutritional Epidemiology (STROBE-nut) statement guides study reporting(Reference Lachat, Hawwash and Ocke32). Recruitment and data collection procedures for both studies have been described in detail previously(Reference Magarey, Mauch and Mallan22,Reference Daniels, Magarey and Battistutta33) . Participants included mother–child dyads, where the infant was born at term with no condition or abnormality affecting development or feeding behaviour, and the mother had facility with the English language. NOURISH participants were randomised to receive a feeding intervention promoting positive infant and toddler feeding practices and the development of healthy food preferences, or usual care. Both NOURISH intervention and control participants were included in the present work. South Australian Infants Dietary Intake participants were recruited at the same time and methodology as per the NOURISH control arm. Both cohorts were followed up over the course of 5 years at various ages.

Data collection

Parent-completed surveys were administered to the same cohort at infant birth, 4–6 months and 2 years and covered parent-reported infant feeding and parenting practices, and maternal and family demographics. The parent involved in data collection was the mother, except n 1, where maternal data were provided at all time points except 2 years where the father became the primary carer. Data collected at infant birth included child gender, maternal age and parental educational attainment. Infant and maternal anthropometrics were measured by study staff. Maternal BMI (kg/m2) was calculated from weight and height data collected at child aged 4–6 months. All other data were collected at child aged 2 years. Child BMI Z-score was calculated using child weight and height measured according to a standardised protocol and the WHO Anthro version 3.0.1 and macros programme (Department of Nutrition for Health and Development, World Health Organization)(34,Reference Daniels, Mallan and Nicholson35) .

One 24-h recall and two 24-h food records were conducted with the primary caregiver at child aged 2 years for the collection of child dietary intake data(Reference Magarey, Mauch and Mallan22). Participants with 2–3 d of intake data were included(Reference Burrows, Martin and Collins36). The 24-h recall utilised a standardised three-pass protocol conducted by trained dietitians via telephone. Food recall and record data included time of consumption, a description of the food and the quantity consumed. Where a primary caregiver felt they could not accurately recall their child’s intake on the day prior, the recall was attempted (unannounced) on another day. An additional food record booklet was provided to be used when the child was being cared for by someone other than the primary caregiver.

Survey data preparation

Multiple-choice questions, including education, household income, other children and marital status, were collapsed to create dichotomous variables (i.e. university educated v. not university educated, less than $50 000 v. $50 000 or more, single child v. multiple child household, partnered v. not partnered). Categories were based on the distribution of the data, whilst ensuring they were meaningful (i.e. University education being considered a high level of educational attainment, and less than $50 k AUD being considered low income for a family at the time)(37). As past research has found non-linear relationships between maternal work hours and children’s weight and weight-related outcomes(Reference Li, Akaliyski and Schafer26,Reference Brown, Broom and Nicholson38) , work hours were grouped into categories and dummy coded for analysis. Maternal working hours (paid employment only) were grouped into four categories, including: not working (reference category), working 1 to <21 h, 21 to <35 h and 35 h or more per week. Paternal working hours (paid employment only) were grouped differently to account for differences in the spread of working hours amongst fathers: not working, working 1–<35 h, 35–40 h (reference category) and greater than 40 h/week.

Thirty-five Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire items were used to calculate scores for four sub-scales of ‘food approach’ (food responsiveness, emotional over-eating, enjoyment of food and desire to drink) and four of ‘food avoid’ (satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, emotional under-eating and food fussiness) eating behaviours(Reference Wardle, Guthrie and Sanderson15). The internal validity and test–retest reliability of these sub-scales have been established in prior research(Reference Wardle, Guthrie and Sanderson15,Reference Mallan, Liu and Mehta39) . Satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating were combined into a single score as they have been shown to be highly correlated(Reference Mallan, Liu and Mehta39). Mean scores were calculated for the remaining sub-scales, with scores between 1 and 5 indicating low to high levels of each eating behaviour.

Items and sub-scales from the Feeding Practices and Structure Questionnaire represented parental feeding practices(Reference Jansen, Mallan and Nicholson40). Predictive validity has been demonstrated against Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire sub-scales, and internal reliability demonstrated with Cronbach’s α values between 0·61 and 0·87(Reference Jansen, Mallan and Nicholson40). Four of the seven sub-scales were included in the present research, namely reward for behaviour, reward for eating, covert restriction and overt restriction, along with a single item to assess family meal setting (Reference Jansen, Mallan and Nicholson40).

Nine (1·7 % of 544) participants had missing data on five or more variables and another nine were missing data that were not at random (1·7 % of 544); all eighteen were therefore excluded from the regression analyses. Of the remaining participants (n 526), six (1·1 %) had missing data for two variables and sixty-two (11·8 %) for one, which were imputed using maximum likelihood estimation. Descriptive statistics were undertaken on the original sample of 544 (with missing data) and on the sample of 526 with imputed data and were found to be similar.

Dietary intake data preparation

Food intake data were entered into FoodWorks Professional Version 9 (Xyris Software Pty Ltd), using energy and nutrient data from the 2007 AUSNUT database(41). Data were exported into SPSS Version 22 (IBM) and merged with 8-digit food codes from the AUSNUT 2007 database. Discretionary foods and beverages were identified using the Australian Bureau of Statistics discretionary food flag(42). Discretionary foods and beverages are defined as those that are not essential for meeting nutrient requirements and are generally energy dense, higher in saturated fat, added sugars, Na and/or alcohol and low in fibre(5). Data were cleaned according to a standard protocol(Reference Magarey, Mauch and Mallan22).

Defined time periods were used to categorise food and beverage intake into eating occasions(Reference Fayet-Moore, Peters and McConnell27,Reference Leech, Worsley and Timperio43) , with all food and beverages consumed during these time periods representing main meals and snacks. The time periods were constructed by plotting the energy content of eating occasions across the day for the whole sample to observe when peaks in intake occurred. Main meals included foods and beverages consumed between 06.00–08.59 hours (breakfast), 11.30–14.29 hours (lunch) and 17.00–19.59 hours (evening meal), while snacks included all food and beverages consumed outside of these times. Foods or meals with no time of consumption recorded were excluded from analysis (18/544 (3·3 %) participants with dietary data, a mean (sd) of 654 (484) kJ per participant).

Data analysis

Analyses were conducted in SPSS version 25 (IBM). Total intake of energy (kilojoules) from discretionary foods and beverages, and discretionary energy consumed at main meals and snacks, was calculated separately for each day of intake and averaged across the number of days reported (n 2 or 3). Descriptive statistics for socio-demographic and intake data included medians and interquartile range for continuous variables and counts and percentages for categorical data. Three hierarchical regression models were conducted with the proportion of total energy consumed from discretionary foods and beverages, and the proportion consumed at main meals and snacks. Variables (total of twenty-nine) were entered in six steps, starting with variables representing: (1) parental work hours (six variables) and household income, followed by; (2) household factors (relationship status, highest level of paternal education and number of children in the household); (3) maternal factors (highest level of maternal education, maternal age at infant birth, maternal BMI); (4) parental feeding practices (reward for behaviour, reward for eating, covert restriction, overt restriction, same food as rest of family and intervention condition); (5) child factors (child gender, child BMI Z-score and child age) and (6) child eating behaviours (food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, satiety responsiveness/slowness in eating, food fussiness, emotional overeating, emotional undereating and desire to drink). Intervention condition was included in the parental feeding practices step, as the intervention focused on directly addressing these practices. The sample size of 526 allowed for eighteen cases per variable, meeting most sample per variable recommendations for regression analyses(Reference Austin and Steyerberg44). There was no multicollinearity, assessed by correlations, tolerance values and variance inflation factor values. Unstandardised beta (β), standard error for the unstandardised beta (se), standardised beta (β) and adjusted R 2 values are presented. Statistical significance was set at P ≤ 0·05.

Results

Figure 2 describes the study participants, and Table 1 presents demographic information for the maximum available sample at child aged 2 years. Seven hundred and nineteen participants provided some data at 2 years, with 654 providing survey data. Mothers were mostly partnered (95 %, n 618/654) with a household income over $50 000 AUD per year (82 %, n 518/631). Just over half were university educated (58 %, n 417/716) and less than half (43 %, n 275/639) were not working, while fathers were mostly working full time (82 %, n 525/640). Participants retained at the 2-year data collection point were older and more likely to hold university qualifications compared with the maximum available sample at baseline (data published elsewhere)(Reference Daniels, Mallan and Battistutta45). Five hundred and forty-four children had 2 (n 10) or 3 (n 534) days of dietary intake data, of which eighteen were excluded from the regression analyses due to missing data, resulting in a final sample size of 526. Compared with the maximum available sample, the regression sample (n 526) included mothers who were slightly older (median (interquartile range) 32 (28–35) years v. 31 (28–35) years), more likely to be partnered (n 513/526, 98 % v. n 618/654, 94 %), university educated (325/526, 62 % v. 417/716, 58 %) and of a higher income (449/526, 85 % v. 518/631, 82 %). Discretionary foods and beverages contributed almost one-fifth of children’s total daily energy intake (19·6 %). Main meals contributed a larger overall proportion of energy intake from discretionary foods and beverages than snacks (554 (313–856) kJ compared with 313 (146–522) kJ).

Fig. 2 Study participants based on survey and dietary intake data availability

Table 1 Child, parental and household characteristics of the maximum sample at child aged 2 years (n 719) and regression sample (n 526)

IQR, interquartile range; EI, energy intake; N/A, dietary data not available; SAIDI, South Australian Infants Dietary Intake.

* Sample size varies between n 631 and n 719 due to missing data, with n 719 providing some data at 2-year data collection, of which 654 provided survey data.

Regression sample includes imputed missing data.

Child Eating Behaviour Questionnaire(Reference Wardle, Guthrie and Sanderson15) sub-scales – scores from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating more of the behaviour.

§ Data collected at recruitment/child birth.

Data collected at time 1/child aged 4–6 months.

Includes n 1 father, who became primary carer before T3 measurements (all other maternal data are from the mother at earlier time points).

** Food Parenting and Structure Questionnaire(Reference Jansen, Mallan and Nicholson40) sub-scales/items – score between 1 and 5, with higher scores indicating more of the parenting practice.

†† n 525 participants as one child was considered a ‘non-consumer’ of snacks.

The final regression models (after all six steps) investigating the relationship between parental work hours, household income and children’s discretionary food and beverage intake are presented in Table 2. Household income showed a consistent, inverse relationship with children’s discretionary food and beverage intake across all three models (β = –0·15, P = 0·002; β = –0·12, P = 0·02 and β = –0·13, P = 0·01 for total discretionary energy intake at main meals and snacks combined, at main meals only and at snacks only, respectively). Children of families with a gross household income below $50 000 AUD/year consumed significantly more energy from discretionary foods and beverages (irrespective of eating occasion) than those with household incomes of $50 000 AUD/year or more.

Table 2 Regression analyses of parental work hours and household income, family, parent and child factors, and proportion of total energy intake from discretionary foods and beverages and at main meals and snacks, in 2-year-old Australian children

Ref, reference category; SAIDI, South Australian Infants Dietary Intake.

* P < 0 05.

Only the final results of the hierarchical models are displayed.

n 1 participant excluded as they were considered a non-consumer of snacks.

§ At recruitment/child birth.

At Time 1/child aged 4–6 months.

** P < 0 01.

*** P < 0 001.

Maternal work hours contributed significantly to the total and main meal models, after controlling for covariates (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Tables 1 and 2). Children with mothers working 21–35 h/week consumed significantly more total energy from discretionary foods and beverages (β = 0·11, P = 0·03) and at main meals (β = 0·10, P = 0·04) than children with mothers who were not working (reference group), whereas children with fathers working greater than 40 h/week had a lower intake of discretionary foods and beverages at main meals (β = –0·11, P = 0·01), compared with their peers with fathers working a standard full-time week of 35–40 h. This was independent of paternal education and household income, both of which were also inversely associated with discretionary intake at main meals (β = –0·12, P = 0·01 and β = –0·12, P = 0·02, respectively).

Although maternal work hours were not associated with the intake of discretionary foods and beverages at snacks, children with fathers not working consumed less discretionary foods and beverages at snacks (β = −0·09, P = 0·047). The association between snack discretionary intake with paternal education was the opposite of that found for main meals, where children with fathers that had a university education consumed more energy from discretionary foods and beverages at snacks than children with fathers without a university education (β = 0·12, P = 0·01). Of the remaining covariates, covert restriction was found to be the most important determinant of children’s discretionary food and beverage intake across all three models (β = –0·16, P < 0·001; β = –0·14, P = 0·001 and β = –0·14, P = 0·003, respectively). Children whose mothers reported using more covert restriction practices consumed a lower total proportion of energy from discretionary foods and beverages, and a lower proportion at main meals and snacks.

Overall, the models accounted for 11·7 % of the variance in children’s total discretionary food and beverage intake (R 2 = 0·117, P < 0·001), 11·4 % in discretionary food and beverage intake at main meals (R 2 = 0·114, P < 0·001) and 5·2 % at snacks (R 2 = 0·052, P = 0·002). The majority of variance was accounted for by the parental work hours and household income (Step 1) and primary carer parenting (Step 4) steps of the regressions (see online supplementary material, Supplemental Tables 13).

Discussion

This study explored parental work hours and household income as opportunity-related determinants of discretionary food and beverage intake in young Australian children and investigated differential associations across eating occasions. Household income had a strong, inverse association with children’s discretionary food and beverage intake across all eating occasions. Maternal and paternal work hours were also key determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake. Maternal work hours had a non-linear relationship with young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake at main meals, with children of mothers working 21–35 h/week consuming more than those of mothers who were not working, whereas children of fathers working more than 40 h/week had a lower intake of discretionary foods and beverages at main meals compared with those working 35–40 h, and children of fathers who were not working consumed less at snacks. These findings suggest that intervention strategies addressing young children’s intake of discretionary foods and beverages should consider opportunity-related determinants of intake such as maternal and paternal time and money and may benefit from tailoring according to eating occasion.

Household income

Consistent with prior research, household income was inversely associated with children’s discretionary food and beverage intake in all three models(Reference Spence, Campbell and Lioret7,Reference Zarnowiecki, Ball and Parletta46) . Children from households with an income of less than $50 000 AUD/year (i.e. the bottom two quintiles for gross household income in Australia in 2015–2016(37)) consumed around 4·6 % more energy from discretionary foods and beverages than those from households with an income of $50 000 AUD or more. Both measurable income and ‘feeling poor’ have been associated with dietary intake in adults, with the effect being stronger with persisting scarcity(Reference Venn and Strazdins24). The mechanisms of this relationship are complex, being that there is no substantial difference between the cost of healthy and unhealthy diets(Reference Lee, Kane and Ramsey47). The cost of diets in line with the dietary guidelines is between 88 and 99 % of the cost of current, unhealthy diets in Australian families(Reference Lee, Kane and Ramsey47). Similarly in some populations in New Zealand, such as those with a higher energy intake, the cost of a healthy diet is lower than that of current diets(Reference Vandevijvere, Young and Mackay48). Furthermore, modelling has shown that even when time cost is taken into account, healthier home-assembled and home-made meals were generally cheaper than takeaway meals(Reference Mackay, Vandevijvere and Xie49). However, families with a low disposable income may be driven to serve acceptable foods that are not rejected and wasted, such as palatable and shelf-stable discretionary foods and beverages(Reference Pescud and Pettigrew30). A low disposable income may also act as a barrier to the purchase of other tools supporting healthy food preparation, such as healthy pre-prepared meals and cooking equipment. Regardless of the mechanism, this research shows that household income is an important opportunity-related determinant of children’s discretionary food and beverage intake and must be considered when planning interventions or policy strategies to address intake.

Parental work hours

Both maternal and paternal work hours were associated with young children’s intake of energy from discretionary foods and beverages. Children with mothers working 21–35 h/week consumed on average 2·8 % more energy from discretionary foods and beverages daily than children with mothers who worked up to 21 h/week, full time (35+ h/week) or were not working. Research in preschool and school-age children has found that greater maternal work hours are associated with lower dietary quality(Reference Datar, Nicosia and Shier50,Reference Gwozdz, Sousa-Poza and Reisch51) . This may be through the impact of work hours on time available for food-related behaviour such as shopping, cooking and eating with children(Reference Cawley and Liu52). In US school-age children, a 20-h increase in maternal work hours was associated with an increased likelihood of consuming fast food at least once per week and consuming sugar-sweetened beverages at least once per day(Reference Datar, Nicosia and Shier50). Similarly in a study of multiple European countries, full-time maternal employment was found to be negatively associated with children’s diet quality, although the effect was relatively small(Reference Gwozdz, Sousa-Poza and Reisch51). The inclusion of a broad range of parent and child covariates, and the younger age of our sample may account for the difference in findings of the present work.

The inclusion of paternal work hours in this study was unique, with prior research not generally accounting for this factor(Reference Li, Akaliyski and Schafer26,Reference Brown, Broom and Nicholson38,Reference Burnett, Worsley and Lacy53) . Fathers work time has tended to be viewed as unimportant in public policy related to parenting, despite their important role in contributing key family resources such as time and money(Reference Strazdins, Baxter and Li54). Children with fathers working greater than 40 h/week consumed less energy in the form of discretionary foods and beverages at main meals, while children of fathers who were not working consumed less at snacks. Although mothers are more frequently the primary caregiver and food provider in Australian households(Reference Burton, Reid and Worsley55), these findings are a reminder that the father or father figure is also a key influencer of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake(Reference Walsh, Cameron and Hesketh56). Whether this is through their direct contribution to food-related tasks or role modelling, or the provision of support to the primary food provider(Reference Jansen, Harris and Rossi57) or through the increased use of external supports such as childcare and/or extended family, the mechanism underlying these findings is unclear and warrants further investigation.

There is no simple explanation for the non-linear findings of this study in relation to maternal and paternal work hours. Past research has similarly demonstrated non-linear relationships between parental work hours and weight and weight-related behaviours(Reference Li, Akaliyski and Schafer26,Reference Brown, Broom and Nicholson38,Reference Burnett, Worsley and Lacy53) . One possible explanation may be that low maternal work hours allow more opportunity for food-related processes, whilst full-time maternal and/or paternal work hours may necessitate a level of organisation and flexibility regarding food-related processes that offer some protection. For example, women working full-time may seek external support with food provision, or outsource other household tasks such as cleaning to allow more time for food provision(Reference Craig, Perales and Vidal58). Furthermore, the enrichment that full-time employment may add to maternal and paternal capability, for example, may outweigh negative effects on time availability for food provision(Reference Li, Akaliyski and Schafer26). However, these relationships may not be due to the effect of work hours on time available for food provision at all and may not be causal. More research, including qualitative research, is needed to understand if these relationships are due to the availability or scarcity of time, or some other mechanism, such as self-efficacy.

The relationship between children’s discretionary food and beverage intake and parental work hours varied by eating occasion. Both maternal and paternal work hours were associated with children’s intake of discretionary foods and beverages at main meals, whilst only paternal work hours were important at snacks. Main meals require more planning and preparation than snacks; thus, work hours may be a more important determinant of children’s discretionary food and beverage intake at main meals compared with snacks. Horning et al. (Reference Horning, Fulkerson and Friend59), in their work investigating mainly mothers’ reasons for purchasing packaged, processed meals, found that those who worked more hours were more likely to report time scarcity as a reason for purchasing convenience foods. By contrast, a discrete choice experiment with mainly mothers of children aged 3–7 years found that time was not a significant factor influencing parental snack choice when weighed up against child acceptance or resistance, co-parent support and home food availability(Reference Johnson, Golley and Zarnowiecki31). This suggests that different intervention approaches may be required at different eating occasions, with time constraints possibly being more relevant when targeting main meals.

Covert restriction

Of the covariates, the parental feeding practices step resulted in the largest increase in variance for all models owing to the parental feeding practice ‘covert restriction’, while child factors such as gender, age, BMI Z-score and eating behaviour were less important in this age group. Covert restriction is the act of restricting a child’s food environment so that they are unaware of it; for example, by avoiding the purchase of discretionary foods and beverages(Reference Jansen, Williams and Mallan60). This contrasts with overt restriction which includes more direct control and restriction of child intake(Reference Zarnowiecki, Parletta and Dollman25,Reference Jansen, Williams and Mallan60) . Similar work in children of various ages confirms the importance of covert restriction in limiting discretionary food and beverage intake(Reference Zarnowiecki, Parletta and Dollman25,Reference Boots, Tiggemann and Corsini61) . This highlights the importance of targeting parental capability with respect to setting up a healthy home food environment, particularly in families with young children.

Strengths and limitations

The incorporation of both maternal and paternal factors was a key strength of this work, as it recognises the influence of both parents on children’s dietary intake, whilst the inclusion of a broad range of covariates ensured that key parent and child factors were adjusted for. This research was however limited by the use of work hours as a proxy for time available for food provision, as work hours do not take into account time commitments outside of work and when and where work takes place (e.g. shift work and working from home). Furthermore, this economic perspective of time does not address the perception of time scarcity, which has been identified as equally important as measurable time(Reference Venn and Strazdins24). Similarly, gross household income does not take into account the availability of money for food and food-related purchases after tax and other essential expenses such as mortgage repayments or rent. Nor does it consider self-assessed poorness, which has been shown to be associated with an increased likelihood of consuming energy from discretionary foods and beverages in adults(Reference Venn and Strazdins24).

As with similar community-based obesity prevention studies(Reference Campbell, Lioret and McNaughton19), NOURISH parents were older, of a higher education and more likely to be partnered than the broader population(Reference Magarey, Mauch and Mallan22). This may explain the lower discretionary food and beverage intake, 20 % in this sample of children compared with 30 % in 2–3-year-olds in the Australian Health Survey(62). The use of parent-reported 24-h food recalls and records was a strength of this work; however, these measures are prone to social desirability bias leading to possible underreporting of children’s discretionary food and beverage intake(Reference Rangan, Allman-Farinelli and Donohoe63). Finally, the amount of variance explained by the models was relatively small, although of a similar magnitude to a study investigating home environment determinants of intake in school-age children (9 and 16 % for sweet and savoury snacks and high-energy beverages, respectively)(Reference Couch, Glanz and Zhou64). Although child, parent and household factors were demonstrated to be important in the present work, there are clearly other determinants at play that were not captured. For example, home food environment factors such as food availability and parent intake(Reference Wyse, Campbell and Wolfenden65), local food environment factors such as supermarkets, food outlets and childcare centres, and food-related policy such as those influencing food pricing and marketing(Reference Rosenkranz and Dzewaltowski10) were not included or adjusted for in the analysis.

Recommendations for future research

Future research in this space would benefit from the inclusion of variables that better represent time and income availability and feelings of scarcity in families of young children, and the use of more socio-economically diverse samples. Analyses that allow the investigation of pathways and interactions between maternal and paternal work hours, and work hours and income, may support a deeper understanding of the interplay between time and money. Intervention development should consider strategies that enable behaviour by increasing means or reducing barriers, by restructuring the physical or social environment or by imparting skills, as these are thought to be effective in addressing opportunity-related determinants of behaviour such as time and money(Reference Michie, van Stralen and West18), although care must be taken to ensure that interventions and programmes are widely accessible and do not increase social inequities between those who can afford or access support and those who cannot(Reference Craig, Perales and Vidal58).

Conclusion

This investigation of opportunity-related determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake through a novel eating occasions lens has provided evidence that can be used to enhance future interventions. Parental work hours and household income were found to be key determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake, along with parental factors such as covert restriction. Household income was a strong and consistent determinant of children’s discretionary food and beverage intake across all eating occasions, meaning that intervention and policy strategies targeting discretionary food and beverage intake in young children should consider the financial implications of dietary change. The maternal work profile of 21–35 h/week was associated with greater child intake of discretionary foods and beverages at main meals, suggesting that this group may require more support to manage the competing demands of work, caregiving and domestic duties, such as evening meal preparation. However, amongst an increasing body of research considering the role of fathers (or father figures) in shaping children’s food intake and preferences(Reference Walsh, Cameron and Crawford66,Reference Williams, de Vlieger and Young67) , this study also suggests a need to consider fathers and other parents or caregivers in future dietary interventions.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: Participants of NOURISH and SAIDI.

Financial support:

CEM was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship, National Health and Medical Research Council postgraduate scholarship (1114194), a top-up scholarship funded by Flinders University and The Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood Centre for Research Excellence (1101675), and a King and Amy O’Malley Trust Postgraduate Scholarship. R.A.L., R.B. and R.K.G. are researchers within the NHMRC Centre for Research Excellence in The Early Prevention of Obesity in Childhood (1101675).

Conflicts of interest:

There are no conflicts of interest.

Authorship:

C.E.M. led the conception and design of the study, under the guidance of T.P.W., L.K.B., R.A.L. and R.K.G., and in consultation with R.B. C.E.M. prepared the data and conducted the analyses, with all co-authors contributing to the interpretation of results. C.E.M. drafted the manuscript, with all co-authors critically reviewing drafts and reading and approving of the final manuscript.

Ethics of human subject participation:

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the Flinders University and Queensland University of Technology Human Research Ethics Committees, and Human Research Ethics Committee’s for each recruitment hospital. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects/patients.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980022000349

References

Australian Institute of Health and Welfare (2018) Nutrition Across the Life Stages. Canberra: Australian Government.Google Scholar
Krebs-Smith, SM, Guenther, PM, Subar, AF et al. (2010) Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. J Nutr 140, 18321838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Public Health England & National Diet and Nutrition Survey (2019) Results from Years 7 and 8 (Combined) of the Rolling Programme (2014/2015 to 2015/2016). UK: Public Health England.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2018) Noncommunicable Diseases Country Profiles 2018. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
National Health & Medical Research Council (2013) Australian Dietary Guidelines – Educator Guide. Canberra: Australian Government.Google Scholar
Fayet-Moore, F, McConnell, A, Cassettari, T et al. (2019) Discretionary intake among Australian adults: prevalence of intake, top food groups, time of consumption and its association with sociodemographic, lifestyle and adiposity measures. Public Health Nutr 22, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, AC, Campbell, KJ, Lioret, S et al. (2018) Early childhood vegetable, fruit, and discretionary food intakes do not meet dietary guidelines, but do show socioeconomic differences and tracking over time. J Acad Nutr Diet 118, 16341643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, LL & Doub, AE (2014) Learning to eat: birth to age 2 years. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 723S728S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarnowiecki, DM, Dollman, J & Parlette, N (2014) Associations between predictors of children’s dietary intake and socioeconomic position: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev 15, 375391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenkranz, RR & Dzewaltowski, DA (2008) Model of the home food environment pertaining to childhood obesity. Nutr Rev 66, 123140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, AE, Ward, DS, Fisher, JO et al. (2016) Fundamental constructs in food parenting practices: a content map to guide future research. Nutr Rev 74, 98117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, LL & Fisher, JO (1998) Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 101, 539549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birch, LL (1999) Development of food preferences. Annu Rev Nutr 19, 4162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, LA (2019) Feeding practices and parenting: a pathway to child health and family happiness. Ann Nutr Metab 74, Suppl. 2, 2942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardle, J, Guthrie, CA, Sanderson, S et al. (2001) Development of the children’s eating behaviour questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42, 963970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, N, Biddle, SJH & Gorely, T (2009) Family correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr 12, 267283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golley, RK, Hendrie, GA, Slater, A et al. (2011) Interventions that involve parents to improve children’s weight-related nutrition intake and activity patterns – what nutrition and activity targets and behaviour change techniques are associated with intervention effectiveness? Obes Rev 12, 114130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michie, S, van Stralen, MM & West, R (2011) The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci 6, 42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, KJ, Lioret, S, McNaughton, SA et al. (2013) A parent-focused intervention to reduce infant obesity risk behaviors: a randomized trial. Pediatrics 131, 652660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Döring, N, Ghaderi, A, Bohman, B et al. (2016) Motivational interviewing to prevent childhood obesity: a cluster RCT. Pediatrics 137, e20153104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, BJ, Zarnowiecki, D, Hendrie, GA et al. (2018) How to reduce parental provision of unhealthy foods to 3- to 8-year-old children in the home environment? A systematic review utilizing the behaviour change wheel framework. Obes Rev 19, 13591370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magarey, A, Mauch, C, Mallan, K et al. (2016) Child dietary and eating behavior outcomes up to 3.5 years after an early feeding intervention: the NOURISH RCT. Obesity 24, 15371545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wen, LM, Baur, LA, Simpson, JM et al. (2015) Sustainability of effects of an early childhood obesity prevention trial over time: a further 3-year follow-up of the healthy beginnings trial. JAMA Pediatr 169, 543551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venn, D & Strazdins, L (2017) Your money or your time? How both types of scarcity matter to physical activity and healthy eating. Soc Sci Med 172, 98106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarnowiecki, DM, Parletta, N & Dollman, J (2016) Socio-economic position as a moderator of 9–13-year-old children’s non-core food intake. Public Health Nutr 19, 5570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J, Akaliyski, P, Schafer, J et al. (2017) Non-linear relationship between maternal work hours and child body weight: evidence from the western Australian pregnancy cohort (Raine) study. Soc Sci Med 186, 5260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fayet-Moore, F, Peters, V, McConnell, A et al. (2017) Weekday snacking prevalence, frequency, and energy contribution have increased while foods consumed during snacking have shifted among Australian children and adolescents: 1995, 2007 and 2011–2012 National Nutrition Surveys. Nutr J 16, 65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sui, Z, Raubenheimer, D & Rangan, A (2017) Exploratory analysis of meal composition in Australia: meat and accompanying foods. Public Health Nutr 20, 21572165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jabs, J, Devine, CM, Bisogni, CA et al. (2007) Trying to find the quickest way: employed mothers’ constructions of time for food. J Nutr Educ Behav 39, 1825.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pescud, M & Pettigrew, S (2014) ‘I know it’s wrong, but…’: a qualitative investigation of low-income parents’ feelings of guilt about their child-feeding practices. Matern Child Nutr 10, 422435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, BJ, Golley, RK, Zarnowiecki, D et al. (2020) Understanding the influence of physical resources and social supports on primary food providers’ snack food provision: a discrete choice experiment. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lachat, C, Hawwash, D, Ocke, MC et al. (2016) Strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology-nutritional epidemiology (STROBE-nut): an extension of the STROBE statement. PLoS Med 13, e1002036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, LA, Magarey, A, Battistutta, D et al. (2009) The NOURISH randomised control trial: positive feeding practices and food preferences in early childhood – a primary prevention program for childhood obesity. BMC Public Health 9, 387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group (2006) WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/Height-for-Age, Weight-for-Age, Weight-for-Length, Weight-for-Height and Body Mass Index-for-Age: Methods and Development. https://www.who.int/childgrowth/standards/technical_report/en/ (accessed July 2020).Google Scholar
Daniels, LA, Mallan, KM, Nicholson, JM et al. (2015) An early feeding practices intervention for obesity prevention. Pediatrics 136, e40e49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burrows, TL, Martin, RJ & Collins, CE (2010) A systematic review of the validity of dietary assessment methods in children when compared with the method of doubly labeled water. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 15011510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2017) 6523.0 – Household Income and Wealth, Australia, 2015–2016. Canberra: Australian Government.Google Scholar
Brown, JE, Broom, DH, Nicholson, JM et al. (2010) Do working mothers raise couch potato kids? Maternal employment and children’s lifestyle behaviours and weight in early childhood. Soc Sci Med 70, 18161824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallan, KM, Liu, WH, Mehta, RJ et al. (2013) Maternal report of young children’s eating styles. Validation of the children’s eating behaviour questionnaire in three ethnically diverse Australian samples. Appetite 64, 4855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, E, Mallan, KM, Nicholson, JM et al. (2014) The feeding practices and structure questionnaire: construction and initial validation in a sample of Australian first-time mothers and their 2-year olds. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 11, 72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Food Standards Australia New Zealand (2008) AUSNUT 2007 – Australian Food Supplement and Nutrient Database for Estimation of Population Nutrient Intakes. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014) Australian Health Survey – Discretionary Food List. https://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/[email protected]/DetailsPage/4363.0.55.0012011-13?OpenDocument (accessed July 2020).Google Scholar
Leech, RM, Worsley, A, Timperio, A et al. (2015) Characterizing eating patterns: a comparison of eating occasion definitions. Am J Clin Nutr 102, 12291337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, PC & Steyerberg, EW (2015) The number of subjects per variable required in linear regression analyses. J Clin Epidemiol 68, 627636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daniels, LA, Mallan, KM, Battistutta, D et al. (2014) Child eating behavior outcomes of an early feeding intervention to reduce risk indicators for child obesity: the NOURISH RCT. Obesity 22, E104E111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zarnowiecki, D, Ball, K, Parletta, N et al. (2014) Describing socioeconomic gradients in children’s diets – does the socioeconomic indicator used matter? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 11, 44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, AJ, Kane, S, Ramsey, R et al. (2016) Testing the price and affordability of healthy and current (unhealthy) diets and the potential impacts of policy change in Australia. BMC Public Health 16, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandevijvere, S, Young, N, Mackay, S et al. (2018) Modelling the cost differential between healthy and current diets: the New Zealand case study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 15, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackay, S, Vandevijvere, S, Xie, P et al. (2017) Paying for convenience: comparing the cost of takeaway meals with their healthier home-cooked counterparts in New Zealand. Public Health Nutr 20, 22692276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Datar, A, Nicosia, N & Shier, V (2014) Maternal work and children’s diet, activity, and obesity. So Sci Med 107, 196204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gwozdz, W, Sousa-Poza, A, Reisch, LA et al. (2013) Maternal employment and childhood obesity – a European perspective. J Health Econ 32, 728742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cawley, J & Liu, F (2012) Maternal employment and childhood obesity: a search for mechanisms in time use data. Econ Hum Biol 10, 352364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burnett, AJ, Worsley, A, Lacy, KE et al. (2019) Moderation of associations between maternal parenting styles and Australian pre-school children’s dietary intake by family structure and mother’s employment status. Public Health Nutr 22, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strazdins, L, Baxter, JA & Li, J (2017) Long hours and longings: Australian children’s views of fathers’ work and family time. J Marriage Fam 79, 965982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burton, M, Reid, M, Worsley, A et al. (2017) Food skills confidence and household gatekeepers’ dietary practices. Appetite 108, 183190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, AD, Cameron, AJ, Hesketh, KD et al. (2015) Associations between dietary intakes of first-time fathers and their 20-month-old children are moderated by fathers’ BMI, education and age. Br J Nutr 114, 988994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, E, Harris, H & Rossi, T (2020) Fathers’ perceptions of their role in family mealtimes: a grounded theory study. J Nutr Educ Behav 52, 4554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, L, Perales, F, Vidal, S et al. (2016) Domestic outsourcing, housework time, and subjective time pressure: new insights from longitudinal data. J Marriage Fam 78, 12241236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horning, ML, Fulkerson, JA, Friend, SE et al. (2017) Reasons parents buy prepackaged, processed meals: it is more complicated than “i don’t have time”. J Nutr Educ Behav 49, 60.e166.e1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jansen, E, Williams, KE, Mallan, KM et al. (2016) The feeding practices and structure questionnaire (FPSQ-28): a parsimonious version validated for longitudinal use from 2 to 5 years. Appetite 100, 172180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boots, SB, Tiggemann, M, Corsini, N et al. (2015) Managing young children’s snack food intake. The role of parenting style and feeding strategies. Appetite 92, 94101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014) 4364.0.55.007 – Australian Health Survey: Nutrition First Results – Foods and Nutrients, 2011–2012. Canberra: Australian Government.Google Scholar
Rangan, A, Allman-Farinelli, M, Donohoe, E et al. (2014) Misreporting of energy intake in the 2007 Australian children’s survey: differences in the reporting of food types between plausible, under- and over-reporters of energy intake. J Hum Nutr Diet 27, 450458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couch, SC, Glanz, K, Zhou, C et al. (2014) Home food environment in relation to children’s diet quality and weight status. J Acad Nutr Diet 114, 1569.e11579.e1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wyse, R, Campbell, E & Wolfenden, L (2011) Associations between characteristics of the home food environment and fruit and vegetable intake in preschool children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 11, 938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, AD, Cameron, AJ, Crawford, D et al. (2016) Dietary associations of fathers and their children between the ages of 20 months and 5 years. Public Health Nutr 19, 20332039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, A, de Vlieger, N, Young, M et al. (2018) Dietary outcomes of overweight fathers and their children in the healthy dads, healthy kids community randomised controlled trial. J Hum Nutr Diet 31, 523532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Conceptual model of determinants of young children’s discretionary food and beverage intake, with child factors in dark blue, parent factors in light blue and external family/household factors in white

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Study participants based on survey and dietary intake data availability

Figure 2

Table 1 Child, parental and household characteristics of the maximum sample at child aged 2 years (n 719) and regression sample (n 526)

Figure 3

Table 2 Regression analyses of parental work hours and household income, family, parent and child factors, and proportion of total energy intake from discretionary foods and beverages and at main meals and snacks, in 2-year-old Australian children

Supplementary material: File

Mauch et al. supplementary material

Mauch et al. supplementary material 1

Download Mauch et al. supplementary material(File)
File 59.8 KB
Supplementary material: File

Mauch et al. supplementary material

Mauch et al. supplementary material 2
Download Mauch et al. supplementary material(File)
File 58 KB