In The Netherlands, as in most European countries, few evidence-based interventions are available to encourage healthful dietary habits. At the time of writing, no comprehensive studies into the behavioural effects of healthful diet interventions for Dutch children and adolescents had been completed. The availability of such healthful diet interventions is important, since unhealthy eating habits, such as low intake of fruit and high intake of saturated fat, are very common in youngsters and may contribute to the high incidence of cardiovascular diseases, diabetes and certain types of cancer at a later age1–3. The diet-related prevalence of adolescent overweight and obesity is an additional rapidly growing problemReference Hirasing, Fredriks, Van Buren, Verloove-Van Horick and Wit4, 5. It is particularly important to develop effective interventions for young people, as healthful eating at an early age may be an important determinant of dietary choices in later lifeReference Kelder, Perry, Klepp and Lytle6, Reference Lytle, Seifert, Greenstein and McGovern7.
There are examples of evidence-based programmes for youngsters available especially in the United StatesReference Contento, Randell and Basch8–Reference Perez-Rodrigo and Aranceta11. More recently, results from a few school-based interventions in Europe have been published, e.g. Bere et al.Reference Bere, Veierod, Bjelland and Klepp14. An examination of studies evaluating these programmes revealed a number of elements that are helpful in developing a successful nutrition education programmeReference Hoelscher, Evans, Parcel and Kelder9–Reference Roos, Lahelma, Virtanen, Prattala and Pietinen18. For instance, a behaviourally focused approach that targets specific behaviours was found to increase the likelihood of behavioural changes more than interventions focused on nutrition in generalReference Hoelscher, Evans, Parcel and Kelder9–Reference Contento, Balch, Bronner, Lytle, Maloney and Olson12. Nutrition education interventions should also provide opportunities for adolescents to taste and enjoy a wide variety of healthful foods in positive social-affective contexts, and increase the availability of healthful foodsReference Contento, Michela, Goreczny and Hersen10, Reference Klepp, Perez Rodrigo, De Bourdeaudhuij, Due, Elmadfa and Haraldsdottir15. Furthermore, interventions should include self-evaluation or self-assessment with a personal feedback componentReference Contento, Michela, Goreczny and Hersen10, Reference Perez-Rodrigo and Aranceta11, Reference Blanchette and Brug13. Other features that have been found to contribute to effectiveness are adequate time for the intervention and adequate intensity, as programmes with longer duration and more contact hours achieve better results than shorter programmesReference Hoelscher, Evans, Parcel and Kelder9–Reference Perez-Rodrigo and Aranceta11, Reference Klepp, Perez Rodrigo, De Bourdeaudhuij, Due, Elmadfa and Haraldsdottir15
This article describes the results of a trial to assess the effects on dietary behaviours of the Dutch healthful diet promotion programme called Krachtvoer for 12–14-year-old students of lower vocational schools (preparatory secondary vocational education). The programme focused on this age group because it is especially when children transfer from primary school to secondary school that their eating habits are likely to change and become less healthfulReference Lytle, Seifert, Greenstein and McGovern7, Reference Contento, Balch, Bronner, Lytle, Maloney and Olson12. Intervening at this age may therefore help to prevent unhealthy eating behaviour becoming habitual. The programme targeted students of lower vocational schools, since they tend to belong to groups with lower socio-economic position, which are known to have less-favourable diets than groups with higher socio-economic positionReference Blanchette and Brug13, Reference Bere, Veierod, Bjelland and Klepp14. The programme focused on the intake of fruit, high-fat snacks and breakfast, because these behaviours are under the students’ volitional control and can be influenced during school hours or are important for school performance. The programme built upon earlier international attempts to improve adolescents’ dietsReference Contento, Randell and Basch8–Reference Contento, Balch, Bronner, Lytle, Maloney and Olson12, which were adapted to the Dutch situation.
Method
Intervention
In the development of the programme, some key aspects of intervention mapping were usedReference Bartholomew, Parcel, Kok and Gottlieb19. Intervention mapping is a framework for effective decision-making at each step in the process of developing, implementing and evaluating theory-based and evidence-based health education programmes. As recommended by intervention mapping, we systematically used the available theories and evidence to make decisions on programme goals and target group, the behaviour change methods and strategies, the programme design, and implementation strategies. We conducted a thorough needs assessment to gain further insight into specific personal and situational determinants of the target behaviours and opinions on nutrition and nutrition education in our specific target population, i.e. Dutch adolescents in lower vocational schoolsReference Martens, Wind, Van Assema and Brug20, Reference Martens and Brug21. The overall aim of the programme was to increase the consumption of fruit and fruit juice, to decrease the consumption of high-fat snacks and to increase breakfast frequency and quality. We defined programme objectives by breaking down the dietary behaviours under study into specified performance objectives (Table 1). The programme consisted of eight school lessons lasting 50 min each. A combination of materials was used: various so-called `task forms' for the lessons, postcards and posters, a ‘survival kit’ (a lunchbox with three healthful food items, viz. a piece of fruit, a health food snack and a breakfast product, and a flyer), a magazine, a video, a website, a take-home bag with a newsletter and food items for the parents, taste-testing of various products and a manual to instruct the teacher in providing the different lessons.
Study design and sample
The study used a cluster-randomised pre-test–post-test experimental design. Twenty-five lower vocational schools were recruited in the southern and central parts of The Netherlands. To avoid too large a variation between the schools in the study, inclusion criteria were (1) a maximum of 50% students of foreign origin and (2) implementation of one of the three most commonly used nutrition education curricula in The Netherlands. Each participating school was asked to select a number of classes in such a way so as to have 50 to 150 students in the targeted age group participating in the study in each school. After recruitment, the schools were randomly assigned to the experimental condition (13 schools) or the control condition (12 schools). Control schools kept using their usual nutrition education curriculum and experimental schools replaced their usual curriculum with the Krachtvoer programme.
Measures
Students were requested to complete a baseline questionnaire in the classroom (September 2002) and a post-test questionnaire 3 months later (December 2002). The teachers supervised the completion of the questionnaires. The questionnaire was a classroom activity, but students were told that they could perform another task in case they objected to the completion of the questionnaire. Baseline and post-test questionnaires were matched on the basis of school, class and date of birth.
Dietary intakes
Dietary intakes were assessed by using food frequency questions and by using a self-administered 24-hour recall. Consumption of fruit was assessed using the fruit questions from a validated fruit and vegetable food-frequency questionnaireReference Van Assema, Brug, Ronda, Steenhuis and Oenema22. In two separate items, students indicated on how many days a week they consumed fruit and fruit juice and the number of servings of fruit and fruit juice they consumed on these days. Frequency and quantity were multiplied to obtain estimates of mean consumption of fruit in servings per day, and of fruit juice in glasses per day. Two items measuring snack consumption were derived from a validated fat-intake questionnaireReference Van Assema, Brug, Ronda and Steenhuis23. Students were asked to indicate the number of days a week they usually consumed sweets and savoury snacks, as well as the amount of sweets and savoury snacks they consumed on these days. Frequency and quantity were multiplied to obtain estimates of the mean consumption of sweets and savoury snacks in servings per day.
In one item, students were asked on how many days per week they normally ate breakfast. Students also wrote down yesterday’s fruit and fruit juice consumption and snack consumption, and today’s breakfast consumption. Nutrient intakes were calculated using the Becel nutrition programme (2002) and Dutch food codes and weightsReference Donders-Engelen, Van der Heijden and Hulshof24, i.e. total fat and saturated fat for yesterday’s snack consumption, and total fat, saturated fat, carbohydrates, protein, vitamin C and sugar for today’s breakfast consumption.
Analysis
Analyses were performed using SPSS (version 11; SPSS, 2001), unless mentioned otherwise.
Descriptive statistics (percentages and means) were used to describe key participant characteristics and variable scores for the experimental and control groups, both at baseline and post-test. Multiple logistic regression of dropout (yes/no) on baseline variables and treatment condition was done to identify causes of dropout and thereby also as potential bias in analyses of post-test data. This dropout analysis was done twice: once with all schools of which baseline questionnaires were received, and once with all schools of which baseline and post-test questionnaires were received. An α = 0.05 two-tailed test was used.
In all effect analyses, persons with a missing post-test value due to school, class or individual dropout were included by replacing the missing post-test value with the baseline value (i.e. the last observation carried forward method). From this point, these analyses will be referred to as intention-to-treat analyses. Since the fruit and snack frequencies variables were not normally distributed, the data were transformed by taking the square root of the frequency scores, which improved the variance and the normality. The breakfast frequency variable was extremely skewed and was therefore dichotomised based on the Dutch breakfast recommendation (i.e. at least 5 days a week vs. fewer days).
Post-test differences between study conditions were first analysed with a fixed linear or logistic regression. Each post-test measure of behaviour was regressed on the study group, gender, age, baseline value of the outcome at hand and interaction terms of the study group with gender, age and baseline value. To simplify the interpretation of study group effects in the possible presence of interactions, the baseline value was always centred (i.e. the overall sample mean was subtracted from each individual baseline value such that the baseline mean became zero), and gender and age were centred if they interacted with treatment. Due to this centring, the B-value and significance of the treatment effect can be interpreted as average treatment effect in the presence of interaction with covariate. Non-significant interactions were removed from the model one by one. In case of a significant interaction with the baseline value, the sample was stratified into three approximately equally large subgroups based on the baseline value. Also, multilevel (mixed) regression analyses were conducted to verify the behavioural outcomes that were significant in the fixed regression analyses, by taking intra-class correlation into accountReference Snijders and Bosker25. Mixed logistic regression analyses were run in Mlwin 1.10.0007Reference Rasbash, Browne, Goldstein, Yang, Plewis and Draper26. More specifically, the fixed regression model was extended with a random school effect and a random class effect. Since schools were randomised, no mixed interaction of treatment by school or class could be tested. In all analyses, an α = 0.05 two-tailed test was used. An α = 0.05 one-tailed test was used for testing intra-class correlations of the primary outcomesReference Snijders and Bosker25, Reference Verbeke and Molenberghs27.
Results
Response rates, attrition and respondents
Before the actual start of the baseline measurements, one experimental school and two control schools withdrew because of time problems and because one of the teachers left the school. The baseline questionnaires of another experimental school and two control schools were never received, even though two schools claimed to have sent the completed questionnaires to the study centre. One experimental school was excluded during the study, as its staff did not implement the programme and had the questionnaires completed by students who were not supposed to be in the study. The final sample for the intention-to-treat analyses thus consisted of 10 experimental schools with 13 teachers, 37 classes and 879 students, and eight control schools with 10 teachers, 31 classes and 734 students (total n = 1613). Post-test questionnaires were received from 10 experimental schools with 13 teachers, 37 classes and 781 students, and six control schools with eight teachers, 21 classes and 469 students. The main reasons for not receiving post-test questionnaires from schools and classes were postal delivery problems. Of all participating schools and classes, 98 individual experimental students and 107 control students did not complete the post-test. The main reason was absence during the hours when they were supposed to complete the questionnaire. None of the students objected to completing the questionnaire. Multiple logistic regression of dropout on baseline variables showed that girls, older students and control group students dropped out significantly more often than boys, younger students and intervention group students, respectively. The treatment group and age effect on dropout remained significant and the gender effect disappeared when the dropout analyses were limited to the schools of which baseline and post-test questionnaires were received.
Boys and girls were equally represented in the sample. In the final sample, 32% of the students were aged 12 years or younger, 53% were 13 years old and 15% were 14 years or older. The majority of the students had parents who had both been born in The Netherlands (83%). Mean variables scores at baseline and post-test with individuals as unit of analysis are presented in Table 2. Very similar mean values were found using schools instead of individuals as unit of analysis.
SD – standard deviation.
* Adjusted for age, gender and baseline, corresponding with the unstandardised regression weight of the binary predictor study group (0 = control condition and 1 = intervention condition). One interaction with age (yesterday's total fat consumption B-value = −1.01, P = 0.04) was found and one interaction effect with gender (fruit frequency B-value = −0.12, P = 0.00) was found.
† Only reported when a significant interaction between study group and centred baseline value was found. The mean B-value of the subgroups can differ from the total B-value because it was not always possible to stratify the sample in three equal tertiles.
‡ B as based on logistic regression.
1P = 0.000 two-tailed.
2P ≤ 0.01 two-tailed.
3P ≤ 0.05 two-tailed.
Dietary intakes at post-test
The results of the effect analyses of the dietary intakes are summarised in Table 2. Note that the centring of baseline values allows the main effect of the study group (i.e. regression coefficient or B-value) to be interpreted as an average treatment effect even in the presence of treatment-by-baseline interaction. No convincing interaction effects of condition with age or gender were found.
Fruit intake at post-test
At post-test, respondents in the experimental condition reported a higher fruit frequency and higher fruit consumption during the previous day (main treatment effects) than the control group. The main treatment effect on fruit frequency was due to an effect in the subgroups of students with a medium baseline frequency value. The main treatment effect on fruit consumption during the previous day was due to an effect in the subgroup of students with the least desirable baseline values.
With respect to the outcomes of fruit juice frequency and fruit juice consumption during the previous day, we found no main effect, but an interaction effect in the sense that the intervention had a significant but adverse effect among the students with the most desirable baseline values.
The mixed regression analyses yielded a class effect regarding fruit consumption during the previous day (P < 0.05 one-tailed, intra-class correlation 0.14), and fruit juice consumption during the previous day (P < 0.05 one-tailed, intra-class correlation 0.17). No convincing differences in B-values were found compared to the fixed analyses, but the effects on fruit frequency and fruit consumption during the previous day were no longer significant in the mixed analyses (P for fruit frequency = 0.23 and P for fruit consumption during the previous day = 0.06).
Snack intake at post-test
With respect to snack frequency and total fat intake from snacks consumed during the previous day, we found an interaction effect in the sense that the intervention had a beneficial effect among students with the least desirable baseline intakes. As regards the number of snacks consumed during the previous day, the intervention had an adverse effect among students with the most desirable baseline value, but a beneficial effect among students with the least desirable baseline value.
Mixed regression analysis for snack behaviour did not show any school or class effects and no convincing differences in B- or P-values were found compared to the fixed regression analysis.
Breakfast intake at post-test
Two significant interaction effects of study group by baseline value were found. In terms of vitamin C intake at breakfast on the day before questionnaire administration, we found a significant beneficial effect among students in the middle intake tertile at baseline. We found a significant beneficial intervention effect among students with the least desirable values at baseline for total saturated fat intake during the previous day.
Mixed regression analysis for breakfast behaviour did not show any school or class effects and no convincing differences in B-values were found compared to the fixed regression analysis.
Discussion
This study used a cluster-randomised design to investigate the effects on dietary behaviours of the Dutch healthful diet programme called Krachtvoer, intended for 12–14-year-old students of lower vocational schools. Fixed regression intention-to-treat analyses revealed beneficial effects on both outcome indicators related to fruit intake, and most behavioural indicators related to high-fat snack intake. With regard to breakfast habits, beneficial effects were only found for vitamin C and total saturated fat intake. Some positive behavioural effects occurred in the total target population, others mainly or only among students with the least desirable values at baseline. The mixed regression analysis found comparable B-values for the behavioural outcomes. However, the favourable effects on the two behavioural outcomes related to fruit intake were not significant in mixed regression analyses.
The behavioural effects regarding fruit are in line with the findings of previous research, which found programme effects of between 0.2 and 1 pieces of fruitReference Contento, Balch, Bronner, Lytle, Maloney and White28, Reference Ammerman, Lindquist, Lohr and Hersey29. No comparable effect studies on breakfast and snack consumption are available, since previous evidence-based programmes were mostly aimed at nutrients like total fat, energy and carbohydrates, rather than at specific breakfast or snacking behaviours. The fact that several programme effects were strongest or were only found among students with the least desirable values on dietary behaviour at baseline, can be considered to be additional favourable results, as they imply that it was especially the students who needed the programme the most who benefited the most from it.
Nevertheless, our findings are definitely not optimal. This might be due to intervention-related aspects as well as aspects of study methodology. We blame some implementation problems that were revealed by the process evaluation for the small or even the lack of beneficial effects on some outcome indicatorsReference Martens, vanAssema, Paulussen, Schaalma and Brug30. The process evaluation indicated that the teachers implemented the programme largely according to plan, that the teachers appreciated most parts of the programme and that the students who were exposed to the programme perceived their nutritional lessons as significantly more pleasant, more interesting, easier and newer than students who followed the usual curriculum. However, especially the implementation of the last two lessons that were designed to translate positive behavioural intentions into actions was suboptimal. Degree of implementation was found to be influenced by the students’ preferences. Some teachers did not implement the final lessons at all because they did not expect their students to appreciate the activities of those lessons. Developing alternatives for those activities that students do not appreciate can improve the programme’s implementation. For some other programme components, it was concluded that a more intensive training programme for the teachers might result in greater intervention fidelity (degree to which a programme is implemented with its methods and strategies intact)Reference Bartholomew, Parcel, Kok and Gottlieb19.
We even found negative effects on the two behavioural measures related to fruit juice consumption in one subgroup. However, these might be due to the fact that students have difficulties distinguishing fruit juices from beverages that are not real fruit juicesReference Martens, Wind, Van Assema and Brug20. It is possible that the intervention made students better able to distinguish fruit juices from other beverages, as a result of which the reported fruit juice consumption at post-test was lower among those who reported initial high fruit juice consumption in the experimental group. Also a negative effect related to snack intake occurred among students with the most desirable values at baseline. The programme might have made these students aware of their relatively good eating habits and therefore the students might have allowed themselves to make some less-healthful food choices. This negative effect was smaller than the positive effect for the students with the least desirable values at baseline. Nevertheless, the programme needs to be revised in order to prevent such effects among this subgroup of students.
The lack of effect on breakfast frequency may be related to the fact that 79% of the students already had breakfast on at least 5 days a week. Earlier studies on breakfast prevalence showed inconsistent results1, Reference De Vries and Da Costa Senior16, Reference Currie, Roberts, Morgan, Smith, Settertobulte and Samdal31, but in our study population, skipping breakfast was definitely not yet a major health behaviour problem. However, as eating breakfast has been found to decline rapidly during adolescenceReference Currie, Roberts, Morgan, Smith, Settertobulte and Samdal31, the Krachtvoer intervention might be effective in the longer run in that fewer students quit eating breakfast.
Important methodological considerations include our data analyses. At post-test, two control schools dropped out altogether and several students dropped out from the remaining 16 schools. We dealt with this problem by conducting intention-to-treat analyses as the primary analyses. We further took the clustering of students in classes and schools into account by checking all significant behavioural effects from the fixed regression analysis with the help of mixed regression analyses. Not all our positive findings were confirmed in these further analyses.
Another possible limitation is that all of the data were self-reported by students and may thus be subject to memory and recording errors. However, we included several self-reported measures, i.e. food frequency questions as well as recording of daily consumption, and results for these different self-reports were largely consistent. A final study limitation worth mentioning is that schools self-selected the classes that participated in the study. Schools might have chosen those classes in which they expected the programme implementation to be the easiest.
The potential effectiveness of the programme was supported by the results of secondary analyses that revealed beneficial effects on targeted behavioural determinants that were supposed to precede behavioural change, i.e. intention to change, attitudes, subjective norms, self-efficacy, awareness of risk behaviour, interest in the topics, perceived food rules and the availability and accessibility of food items at home. Similar to the behavioural effects, some of these favourable effects occurred in the entire target population, and others occurred mainly or only among those students who had the least desirable values at baselineReference Martens32.
Although we did not find effects on all outcome indicators, the total pattern of results made us conclude that Krachtvoer had a certain surplus value over existing curricula. However, some parts of the programme need to be revised to improve its effectiveness and prevent negative effects among students with favourable intakes at baseline, and more information is needed about the long-term effects of the programme.
Acknowledgements
Sources of funding: This study was financially supported by the Netherlands Organisation for Health and Development (ZonMW) and the Netherlands Heart Foundation.
Conflict of interest declaration: None.
Authorship responsibilities: All the authors listed on this paper meet the criteria set down by the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. No one who might consider that he or she has a right to be an author had been excluded.