Dairy foods are a major source of nutrients in children's diets and inadequate consumption during childhood has potential health implications later in life. Dairy foods provide a significant amount of total Ca and are an important source of protein, Mg and other essential nutrients(Reference Baird, Syrette and Hendrie1). Dairy products are unique in that they offer high Ca availability and high Ca content, and Ca provided through dairy has been shown to stimulate bone development and maximise peak bone mass(Reference Heaney2, Reference Nicklas3). Sixty per cent of peak bone mass is acquired throughout the pubertal years(Reference Loud and Gordon4), thus a constant intake of readily available dietary Ca during this intense growth period, among other factors, is essential in strengthening bone and reducing the risk of osteoporosis later in life(Reference Heaney2, Reference Huth, DiRienzo and Miller5). Therefore prevention strategies that start with children, to maximise their bone density, are critical to reducing the health-care burden in the future.
Dietary guidelines recommend two to three servings of dairy foods (or about 1000 mg of Ca) daily for children(6–8), but shortfalls in intake have been reported in Australian and American children. In Australia, on average, children aged 4–13 years consume fewer than two dairy servings daily(Reference Baird, Syrette and Hendrie1). The percentage of children meeting their Ca requirement decreases with age(9, Reference Nicklas, O'Neil and Fulgoni10). A large cross-sectional data set of American children suggests that as dairy consumption decreases, so does the proportion of children who meet the recommendations for Ca(Reference Nicklas, O'Neil and Fulgoni10). It is evident that without dairy foods, children would find it difficult to achieve adequate Ca intakes.
The majority of research to understand the influences of children's dairy intake or efforts to increase children's dairy have been conducted in the USA. Cross-sectional studies suggest that knowledge of Ca-rich foods and self-efficacy to consume dairy foods are positively associated with milk and Ca intake(Reference Sharma, Hoelscher and Kelder11, Reference Thompson, Bachman and Watson12). Dairy food availability at home(Reference Downs, Arnold and Marshall13–Reference Vue and Reicks15), role modelling of eating behaviour, parents’ expectation(Reference Vue and Reicks15) and provision of milk with meals(Reference Fisher, Mitchell and Smiciklas-Wright16) have also been positively associated with children's dairy consumption. There is also strong evidence that the consumption of both plain and flavoured milk is positively associated with Ca intake(Reference Fisher and Mitchell D Smiciklas-Wright17–Reference Striegel-Moore, Thompson and Affenito20). Large population surveys from the USA show that eating a cereal-based breakfast facilitates milk consumption(Reference Albertson, Thompson and Franko21, Reference van den Boom, Serra-Majem and Ribas22) and has a positive impact on Ca intake over the whole day(Reference Affenito, Thompson and Barton23, Reference Barton, Eldridge and Thompson24).
While cross-sectional research is valuable for its insights into associations, the direction of influence is unknown and causality is difficult to determine. Therefore, we aimed to review intervention studies that targeted an increase in children's dairy food intake, to determine which strategies assist children meet their Ca requirements. The review is limited to studies targeting children of primary school age (5–12 years) as intakes are low during this critical window for attaining peak bone mass and it is considered an important stage in the development of food preferences and dietary habits that will continue into adulthood. It is also important to limit the focus to a particular target group as interventions may have different effectiveness in children of different ages. The objectives of the present review are to: (i) identify interventions that focus on children's dairy food or Ca intakes; and (ii) determine the characteristics associated with successful interventions. Using the findings of the review and incorporating current literature on other dietary change interventions including fruits and vegetables, we identify a set of promising strategies that are likely to be the most effective for improving the dairy and Ca intake of children.
Method
Search method for identification of studies
A list of search terms and keywords was developed based on recent and relevant reviews(Reference Hendrie, Brindal and Corsini25, Reference Golley, Hendrie and Slater26) and refined to reflect a focus on dairy food intake in young children. Search term lists were comprehensive and inclusive (see Supplementary Materials). Terms were combined under the following headings.
1. Study design: e.g. interventions, clinical trials, prospective and comparative studies.
2. Nutrition: e.g. intake, diet, food and beverages.
3. Children: e.g. child, young people and schoolchildren.
4. Dairy intake: e.g. calcium, dairy, milk and cheese.
The main search was conducted by an information specialist in early February 2011 and included the following databases: PubMed, ProQuest, Web of Knowledge, Cochrane databases, PsychINFO and FS Tech Abstracts. Finally, reference lists of identified reviews and articles were searched for additional studies. Unpublished work was not included in the search. However, authors of published protocols that were not accompanied by any published results were contacted to assess whether any results were available. In all cases no further results were available.
Inclusion criteria
Types of studies
Prospective studies of any duration evaluating the effectiveness of an intervention, with or without a comparison group. Interventions that stated an aim to increase or promote dairy food or Ca intake in children (interventions that targeted a switch from regular- to low-fat dairy only, without promoting an increase in dairy intake, were excluded). Interventions delivered directly to young children and/or their parents, through schools or any other settings were included.
Types of participants
Children of primary school age. Primary school in Australia was considered to be children aged 5 to 12 years. If the age range crossed outside this range, then studies were included if the mean age was ≥5 years or ≤12 years. If children's age was described in terms of school years then studies involving ‘primary school’ or ‘elementary school’ were included, thereby excluding studies in pre-school, kindergarten and high schools.
Types of outcomes
Dietary outcomes describing Ca intake, individual dairy food or total dairy intake, reported at an individual or group level.
Exclusion criteria
To limit the focus to interventions relevant to the general population, studies that targeted specific subgroups of the population such as clinical populations, individual case studies and children with diabetes or lactose intolerance were excluded. Studies about Ca supplementation, pregnancy, breast-feeding, infants, allergies or complementary medicine were excluded by title search. All animal studies, food science, sensory, metabolic and food processing research was also excluded by title and abstract search (these exclusion search terms are referred to as the ‘exclusion criteria’ in Fig. 1). Studies were limited to those published in English in the last 20 years, to ensure that the findings of the review are relevant to children's current context. Review authors assessed abstracts and full text where necessary against review criteria (Fig. 1). Fourteen intervention studies (described within sixteen papers) were included.
Data extraction and synthesis
Reviewers independently extracted data from papers including details of the study population, setting and mode of delivery, dietary targets and outcome measures, effectiveness, measures of intervention intensity and intervention description (see Supplementary Materials for comprehensive data extraction table). Two reviewers were used to ensure the quality of data entry. Any inconsistencies were resolved by a third reviewer.
Intervention effectiveness
Dietary intake assessment methodology varied from self- or parent-reported intake behaviour to multiple 24 h recalls. Because of heterogeneity in assessment method and outcomes (e.g. Ca, milk consumed, total dairy food intake), meta-analysis was not conducted.
We applied our own criteria whereby a study was considered to be effective when a statistically significant increase (P < 0·05 or better) in a dairy outcome was reported by the authors AND this outcome was measured using an objective method (such as sales data, observed intake) or a robust dietary assessment method (such as FFQ, 24 h recall, weighed records).
The magnitude of increase in Ca or dairy food intake was described, and comparisons made between studies where possible. Intervention effectiveness by study characteristics, intervention intensity and behaviour change techniques were assessed in all interventions.
Intervention intensity
Because of the varied study design and range of intervention settings included in the review, an assessment of intervention requirements or ‘intensity’ was developed to allow for easier comparison between all studies. Two reviewers independently scored the characteristics and resolved discrepancies by discussion. Assessment of intensity was based on four characteristics, guided by a recent review on community-wide interventions for increasing physical activity(Reference Francis, Baker and Doyle27, Reference Baker, Francis and Soares28). A 5-point scale of intensity (1 = low, 2 = low–medium, 3 = medium, 4 = medium–high, 5 = high) was developed to assess each of the following four characteristics, with the exception of ‘reach of the strategies’ (see scale details below and in Table 2).
1. Duration of intervention: described the length of the intervention (scaled relative to interventions included in the review). The longer the intervention, the higher the intensity.
2. Contact with intervention: described the frequency of contact. Where interventions used a multiple frequency of contact points (e.g. weekly for 6 weeks, then monthly for 6 months and then quarterly for 1 year), a sum of the total number of points of contact divided by the duration of the intervention was used to give an average rate of contact.
3. Type of contact or level of contact with the intervention: described the level of personalisation of the intervention. The more personalised the contact, the higher the intensity; from intervening at the environmental level (low: physical, policy or legislative) through to higher intensity individual, one-on-one personalised contact.
4. Reach of the strategies: described the number of settings used to reach the target audience.
Including a description of the cost and a statement of intensity by the authors were also considered for the overall intensity rating. However, due to a lack of consistent reporting on these measures, they were not included.
Studies were scored from 1 to 5 on each of the four intensity criteria and an overall intensity score calculated based on the sum of the four components of intensity (out of a possible 20). Relative to the studies included in the present review, scores greater than 13 were considered as ‘higher’ intensity, 11–12 as ‘medium’ intensity and 10 or less as ‘lower’ intensity.
Behaviour change techniques
Standardised definitions from Abraham and Michie's taxonomy of behaviour change techniques (BCT)(Reference Abraham and Michie29) were used to define the techniques applied in the intervention (Table 1). A user guide provides definitions to code twenty-six different BCT. A health psychologist coded all interventions using the taxonomy. Techniques were also coded according to whom the BCT was aimed at (child, parent or family). After preliminary coding, an additional four categories were added to the original taxonomy to account for commonly recurring techniques unique to the interventions analysed (see techniques 30–33 in Table 1). ‘Taste exposure’ was used to describe a technique where participants were exposed (once or multiple times) to the foods targeted in the intervention. ‘Provide general nutrition education’ and ‘provide prescriptive diet’ were added to describe BCT originating from nutrition approaches to behaviour change. ‘Provide general nutrition education’ was coded as present when the intervention described a general nutrition education programme. The use of this technique may have occurred independently or in combination with technique 1 or 2. For ‘provide prescriptive diet’, interventions needed to provide participants with clear dietary parameters (e.g. percentage of macronutrients). Finally, a technique described as ‘environmental restructuring’ was added. The authors have used this technique previously(Reference Hendrie, Brindal and Corsini25) to describe intervention attempts to change a child's direct environment to promote behaviour change. For example, this may involve changing a child's daily environment across a variety of settings (e.g. home or school) and include changes in availability of certain foods.
Bold: techniques that differentiate effective and ineffective studies.
Italics: most commonly used techniques.
* Intervention effectiveness is defined as a statistically significant increase (P < 0·05) in a dairy outcome AND outcome measured using an objective method or robust dietary assessment method.
† Behaviour change techniques from taxonomy by Abraham and Michie(Reference Abraham and Michie29).
Results
Intervention studies
Study description
The present review summarises sixteen papers describing fourteen interventions published between years 1990 and 2010, conducted mainly in the USA(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30–Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41), one in South America(Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42) and three in Europe(Reference Evans, Greenwood and Thomas43–Reference Alexy, Reinehr and Sichert-Hellert45). Children's age spanned from 5 to 12 years, and with the exception of one study that recruited girl scouts only(Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35), all studies targeted both girls and boys. One intervention targeted overweight and obese children(Reference Alexy, Reinehr and Sichert-Hellert45), while the others targeted the general or at-risk populations.
Sample size ranged from a small group of children to federally funded school programmes in the USA. Outcome evaluation focused primarily on short-term effects (end of intervention) with only five articles reporting follow-up effects post-intervention(Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference Friedman, Snetselaar and Stumbo36, Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37, Reference Evans, Greenwood and Thomas43, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44).
The intervention techniques were delivered in schools (n 4), the community (n 3), the home (n 2) and within research organisations (n 2). Three studies delivered the intervention across multiple settings. Study designs included randomised controlled trials (n 6), clinical controlled trials (n 4) and uncontrolled trials (n 4; see Supplementary Materials).
Six interventions targeted both dietary and physical activity behaviours(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Alexy, Reinehr and Sichert-Hellert45), the other eight focused solely on dietary behaviour. It was common for interventions to target an increase in dairy foods within the context of a healthy diet (n 9). Two studies targeted milk intake only and three targeted an increase in Ca specifically, through an increase in dairy food intake (Table 1). Most interventions targeted children's behaviour (n 11) but three included parent or family behaviours as well. One study focused on parents’ provision of children's lunch(Reference Evans, Greenwood and Thomas43) and two focused on the family diet as a whole(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33, Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41).
Resources provided to the participants as part of the intervention were varied. Some studies provided government-published pamphlets about healthy eating and/or access to specialised equipment for physical activity components of the intervention(Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38), others provided children with milk to be consumed as part of the intervention(Reference Condon, Crepinsek and Fox31, Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44) or with Ca-rich meals or snacks to be consumed during the intervention sessions(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37) (see Supplementary Materials).
Dietary assessment methods and outcomes
There was heterogeneity in both measurement and reporting of intake across the studies. Increase in Ca intake (reported in mg, n 4), over the intervention period (10–52 weeks), ranged from 202 to 671 mg. An increase of 200–250 mg (0·7–0·8 servings) was most common; however one intervention reported a 671 mg increase (2·2 servings) over a 16-week period(Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42). That study was unique in that it promoted an increase in dairy through a swap message. Children were advised to replace sugar-sweetened beverages with low-fat flavoured milk, which was delivered to their homes(Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42). Two studies that targeted dairy within the context of a healthy diet reported an increase in low-fat dairy foods of about 0·3 servings(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33, Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41). Due to the study design and variation in intake, the increase in servings of dairy was significant and sustained over a 3-year period in one study(Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41), while the other result was not significant(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33).
Intervention intensity
Five studies were scored as higher intensity and three as lower intensity, with only five points separating the lowest- from the highest-intensity study (Table 2). Studies which achieved a higher intensity rating scored consistently well across all four measures, with the exception of reach or number of settings targeted. That is, higher intensity interventions were longer in duration, had frequent points of contact, and the contact was of a personalised nature.
Italics: highlights ineffective studies.
*1 = <6 weeks, 2 = 6 to 11 weeks, 3 = 12 weeks to 5 months, 4 = 6 to 12 months, 5 = >12 months.
†1 = annually, 2 = bimonthly to quarterly, 3 = monthly, 4 = weekly, 5 = daily.
‡1 = environmental (intervening at the physical, policy or legislative level), 2 = environmental with a small group/education component, 3 = group, 4 = group with an individual component (goal setting, homework task), 5 = individual (one-on-one personalised contact).
§1 = one setting, 3 = two settings, 5 = three or more settings. No values for 2 and 4 because three was the maximum number of settings used.
IILower = score ≤10, medium = score 11–12, higher = score ≥13.
¶Intervention effectiveness is defined as a statistically significant increase (P < 0·05) in a dairy outcome AND outcome measured using an objective method or robust dietary assessment method.
**Results published in Dwyer et al. and Osganian et al.(Reference Dwyer, Hewes and Mitchell32, Reference Osganian, Ebzery and Montgomery39).
††Results published in Van Horn et al. and Friedman et al.(Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41, Reference Friedman, Snetselaar and Stumbo36).
Overall, the majority of studies (n 11) targeted only one setting (including all four lower intensity studies). Only two (of five) higher intensity studies(Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38) targeted more than one setting, subsequently receiving the highest overall intensity ratings. French and colleagues targeted girl scouts at troop meetings and summer camps, as well as the family through activities at home(Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35). Muth et al. targeted primarily the school setting through nutrition education sessions, combining this with homework tasks requiring parental involvement(Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38).
All five higher intensity studies provided a personalised point of contact with intervention participants. This was in the form of either one-on-one individual contact (e.g. nutritionist visits(Reference Friedman, Snetselaar and Stumbo36, Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42)) or group contact with an individual component (e.g. group education with individualised goal setting(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33, Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37, Reference Evans, Greenwood and Thomas43)). Intervention duration of the higher intensity studies was at minimum 12–16 weeks(Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42), with most intervening for at least 12 months, but some up to 36 months.
None of the lower intensity studies were individualised, instead utilising either group education(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Powers, Struempler and Guarino40) or environmental strategies (e.g. provision of milk as part of school lunch programme(Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44)). However, frequency of contact was high in all lower intensity studies, directly contacting participants on either a weekly or a daily basis.
Intervention characteristics and behaviour change techniques
Interventions were delivered by a range of facilitators (from high-school students to paediatricians) in a variety of settings. Four studies used trained intervention facilitators who had no previous nutrition qualification (e.g. librarians)(Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44).
In six studies, general nutrition education or behavioural nutrition approaches guided intervention design(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41). Only five studies (of fourteen) made reference to a theoretical approach underlying their intervention; they all referred to Social Cognitive (or Learning) Theory(Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Powers, Struempler and Guarino40, Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41). However, only one study(Reference Freedman and Nickell34) explicitly described how theory was applied in the intervention.
Studies used between one and twenty-one BCT (Table 1). General nutrition education was the most commonly applied BCT (n 10). Other commonly used BCT included prompting intention formation, general encouragement, providing instruction and prompting specific goal setting.
Table 3 further describes the BCT used according to who were the primary targets. Seven studies used BCT targeted at children only(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30–Reference Dwyer, Hewes and Mitchell32, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Powers, Struempler and Guarino40, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44), and two studies focused on children but had a parent or family component(Reference Freedman and Nickell34, Reference Alexy, Reinehr and Sichert-Hellert45). Three studies used BCT predominantly targeted at the family (parent and child together)(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33, Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41, Reference Evans, Greenwood and Thomas43), one primarily targeted parents(Reference Hovell, Nichols and Irvin37) and one used BCT aimed at parents and children equally(Reference Albala, Ebbeling and Cifuentes42).
Bold: highlights points of difference between targets.
*Behaviour change techniques from taxonomy by Abraham and Michie(Reference Abraham and Michie29).
BCT including providing information on the health–behaviour link, modelling the target behaviour, teaching the use of prompts/cues to perform behaviour, promoting practice, providing opportunities for social comparison (i.e. doing activities in groups) and taste exposure were targeted at children more so than parents or the family. Planning for social support was directed more at the family level, while identifying position as a role model for others’ behaviour was aimed at parents. Neither of these two techniques was applied in relation to children's behaviour in any intervention.
Characteristics of effective interventions
Eleven interventions significantly increased children's dairy or Ca intake. One did not use a robust method of intake; therefore according to the definition described, that study was considered ineffective(Reference Powers, Struempler and Guarino40). Therefore, 71 % of interventions (n 10) were considered to be effective in increasing children's dairy intake. Studies focused on encouraging dairy foods or Ca intakes alone were all effective, compared with 55 % of studies promoting low-fat dairy foods within the context of a healthy diet (Table 1). The greatest increase in Ca intake was reported in one of the effective interventions (671 mg). Apart from this intervention, the overall ranges in the magnitude of change reported in the effective and ineffective interventions were similar (30–248 mg and 90–210 mg, respectively).
Effectiveness by intervention description
Table 2 shows the intervention intensity by intervention effectiveness. A majority (80 %) of the higher intensity interventions were effective. In comparison, 67 % of lower–medium intensity interventions were effective. Interestingly, the effective lower intensity interventions(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44) all provided dairy foods as part of the programme (see Table 1). One provided dairy as a snack with nutrition education sessions(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30), the other focused on an environmental strategy with the provision of milk at school lunch(Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44). Provision of dairy was common to effective intervention – 60 % of effective studies provided Ca-rich foods, while no ineffective studies provided dairy food to their participants.
Three of the four ineffective studies shared similar intervention characteristics(Reference Dwyer, Hewes and Mitchell32, Reference Muth, Chatterjee and Williams38, Reference Powers, Struempler and Guarino40). These interventions tended to be delivered in schools and all focused on children's dairy food intake within the context of general health behaviours. The ineffective studies directed BCT at children only. In contrast, only one effective intervention was delivered through a school(Reference Condon, Crepinsek and Fox31) and only three (of ten) effective studies used BCT directed at children only(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Condon, Crepinsek and Fox31, Reference French and Story M Fulkerson35). The fourth ineffective study(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33) was focused primarily on the family and delivered in the home. Interestingly, it was the only intervention to include an active control group (which may have influenced its effectiveness according to the definition used in the present review).
In the review, there were a small number of ineffective studies (four of fourteen) which made it difficult to draw distinct inferences about the potential differences in BCT used between effective and ineffective studies. The number of techniques did not differentiate effective from ineffective studies; ranging from one to twenty-one and from two to twenty for effective and ineffective studies, respectively. Only two BCT seemed to discriminate effective and ineffective studies. These were the use of taste exposure and prompting practice, which were used by 60 % the effective studies but by none of the ineffective studies. Interestingly, none of the effective studies described the use of setting graded tasks for participants or providing feedback on performance, whereas some of the ineffective studies (n 2 for each BCT) detailed the use of these BCT.
There was no clear link between the reference to a theoretical basis and effectiveness. The limited description of how theory was incorporated in those papers mentioning a theoretical underpinning limited the ability to make detailed comparisons according to theory.
The five interventions that aimed to increase dairy alone, i.e. not within the context of a healthy diet, were all effective (Table 1). The intervention intensity of these studies varied (two lower, one medium and two higher). These interventions used a wide range of techniques, delivered across a variety of settings, to a range of primary targets (children, parents and/or the family as a whole). All the dairy-specific interventions used the technique of taste exposure through the provision of dairy foods to participants. Four (of five) used general encouragement, specific goal setting or opportunities for social comparison.
Discussion
The present review identified fourteen interventions that focused on increasing young children's dairy food or Ca intakes. Over 70 % of dairy interventions were classified as effective, supporting the proportion of effective interventions reported in other reviews focusing on changing children's dietary behaviours, such as fruit and vegetable intake(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittales46, Reference de Sa and Lock47). Our review has highlighted an absence of intervention research being published outside the USA. A clear majority of papers included here examined dairy intakes of children in the USA. While there are similarities between the USA and other Westernised cultures, there are unique aspects of American society that require consideration, so it is important to determine the effectiveness of similar interventions published from other countries where children's Ca intake is below recommendations.
All of the interventions identified that aimed to increase dairy or Ca intake alone were effective. In contrast, only half of the interventions that promoted dairy foods as part of a healthy diet were effective. Interventions that focus on one discrete dietary behaviour can use more specialised techniques, like specific goal setting. These interventions may also be more likely to report positive results because all their resources are targeted toward one behaviour and not diluted across several targets. Increasing dairy as a single intervention strategy may have the potential to increase overall energy intake, which should be considered in future interventions. It is also important to recognise that interventions targeting dairy within the context of the whole diet may have other positive dietary impacts (beyond Ca intake), which were not evaluated in the current review.
Despite the fact that interventions varied in their approach, content, intensity and subsequently the magnitude of behaviour change they were able to achieve, our results suggest that an increase of two-thirds to one serving of dairy (20–30 % of recommendations) daily is a possible target for future intervention. This is an important increase in the context of children's total intake. For example, in Australia, children's dairy food intake is about one serving, or about 30 %, short of recommendations(Reference Baird, Syrette and Hendrie1). Without meta-analysis direct comparisons between the change reported here and other studies targeting different food groups is limited. When considered in the context of dietary recommendations, the magnitude of change in dairy intake reported here is more than that described in interventions targeting children's fruit and vegetable intake, which report increases of 2–14 % of daily recommendations(Reference de Sa and Lock47). Similarly, an Australian population-level campaign reported an increase of about 11 % of daily recommendations(Reference Pollard, Miller and Daly48). If interventions were to achieve increase in dairy of one serving daily, the potential health impacts for children would be significant. Weaver and colleagues(Reference Weaver49) have estimated that increasing children's Ca intake by 500 mg/d (or 38 % of recommendations) would increase peak bone mass by 10 % and delay onset of fractures by 13 years(Reference Huth, DiRienzo and Miller5).
Providing dairy foods to children in this age group may be a promising way to strengthen the effect size of dairy interventions. This supports previous research that has explored other eating behaviours, such as fruit and vegetable consumption(Reference de Sa and Lock47, Reference Geller and Dzewaltowski50). The provision of food appears to directly influence children's access and availability to these foods, and accessibility and availability are consistent predictors of intake(Reference Geller and Dzewaltowski50). At a population level, this strategy may have policy and infrastructure implications. In the USA, milk is provided to some children in schools as part of national breakfast or lunch programmes(51) and participation in these programmes has been associated with significantly higher intake of Ca(Reference Gordon and McKinney52). It is unclear whether population-level programmes would be as effective in other countries.
When we examined how intensity may be associated with effectiveness, there was no clear association, with some lower, medium and higher intensity interventions considered effective. However, within different levels of intensity different characteristics could be important. For example, effective lower intensity interventions had contact with participants either through group or environmental strategies, targeted only one setting, had high frequency of contact and provided dairy foods to children(Reference Caine-Bish and Lowry Gordon30, Reference Koivisto, Edlund and Sjoden44). When resources are available to invest in intervention, higher intensity interventions can be equally effective in increasing children's dairy food intake.
Effective studies targeted parents or the family as a whole, whereas ineffective studies tended to use BCT directed only at children. The key techniques that appeared to discriminate effective from ineffective studies were the use of taste exposure to dairy foods and prompting practice of the behaviour. Taste is an important factor influencing children's food choices. Taste and taste preferences develop early in life. Milk flavour positively affects children's milk drinking behaviour, although this may be different for boys and girls(Reference Murphy, Douglass and Johnson19). Taste preferences are amendable to change and repeated exposure has been shown to increase intake of particular foods in children(Reference Birch, Gunder and Grimm-Thomas53). Increasing children's preference is one of the most consistent influences of fruit and vegetable intake(Reference Geller and Dzewaltowski50).
Limited detail in the interventions precluded the evaluation of what taste properties or sensory dimensions children were exposed to. However, one of the most successful interventions reviewed provided sweetened milk to participants. It is unclear how sweet these beverages were and any potential effects that this may have had on taste exposure specific to the flavour of dairy. If taste exposure is to be applied in order to encourage the development of a preference for dairy products, care needs to be taken as to which sensory properties children are exposed to. It is unclear how exposure to plain milk v. flavoured milk may operate to influence preference. The promotion of flavoured milk is likely to result in controversy within the discipline of public health given its potential sugar content. Nevertheless, cross-sectional and focus group research suggests that children who consume plain or flavoured milk have higher dairy intakes(Reference Fisher and Mitchell D Smiciklas-Wright17–Reference Striegel-Moore, Thompson and Affenito20, Reference Johnson, Frary and Wang54). Furthermore, children like flavoured milk(Reference Vue and Reicks15, Reference Auld, Boushey and Bock55–Reference Novotny, Ji-Sook and Biernacke57) and parents think flavoured milk is a good way to increase children's dairy intake(Reference Winzenberg, Hansen and Jones58). Therefore, the potential consequences of exposing children to flavoured milk requires more in-depth evaluation of its relative cost and benefits; as does exploration of dairy foods other than milk where taste exposure may also be relevant. Notwithstanding, our results suggest that increasing access to dairy products, such as milk, in combination with use of the taste exposure technique may be beneficial in increasing intake.
As well as taste exposure, we found effective interventions used prompting practice which is related to taste exposure as it involves rehearsing or repeating behaviour. However, upon closer examination of the interventions using this technique, we found it was not applied specifically to the consumption of dairy as in most cases it was targeted at food preparation or exercise behaviour. Therefore, its association with dairy consumption, per se, is not clear. Previous reviews of general dietary and obesity prevention interventions have also shown prompting practice to be associated with effectiveness(Reference Hendrie, Brindal and Corsini25), so it is possible that this technique has a benefit for dietary behaviour in general, encompassing dairy intake. Coding BCT specific to the behavioural target would be a useful addition to future reviews, but a greater number of studies would be required for this level of comparison. Nevertheless, prompting practice is worth consideration in the development of health intervention, particularly in children, because they are constantly refining and practising life skills as they mature into adulthood.
General nutrition education and providing instruction about how to perform the behaviour were not clearly linked to effectiveness. Cross-sectional literature and other reviews(Reference Nicklas3) suggest that knowledge of Ca-rich foods is related to intake of dairy foods(Reference Sharma, Hoelscher and Kelder11). Therefore, it is interesting that nutrition education was not associated with intervention effectiveness. This may be partly because it was used in most interventions, or it may reflect the quality of the nutrition education provided. Other dietary reviews also suggest there is limited evidence that education alone can positively alter behaviour in children(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittales46), but the additive role of providing knowledge in combination with other effective techniques cannot be determined from the present review. Improving knowledge is an attractive target of intervention because it is a relatively malleable characteristic, and at a population level the knowledge of individuals is most amendable to policy intervention(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittales46). However, the provision of information in an intervention does not mean the acquisition of knowledge. Furthermore, knowledge is ‘required but not sufficient’ for changes in consumers’ food behaviour(Reference Worsley59). For other dietary behaviours, such as the intake of fruit and vegetables and potentially for dairy intake as well, there is strong evidence that interventions combining nutrition education with improved availability and some parent involvement are more effective(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittales46).
Our review provided limited direction regarding the best setting to deliver interventions targeting children's dairy consumption. Schools are a logical setting for implementing interventions as they offer frequent contact(Reference Brown and Summerbell60) and are in a unique position to reach large numbers of children(Reference Peterson and Fox61). However, interventions delivered only within schools tended to be ineffective here, which questions the effectiveness of this setting for dairy interventions. Community- and home-based interventions alone or in combination were largely effective, and these possibly show the most promise as settings for the future.
The home, as an intervention setting, is likely to play an important role in the development of habits in children. Parents, as the nutritional gatekeepers(Reference Wansink62), can influence dairy food availability(Reference Downs, Arnold and Marshall13–Reference Vue and Reicks15, Reference Novotny, Ji-Sook and Biernacke57, Reference Edlefsen, Reicks and Goldberg63, Reference Olson, Chung and Reckase64), support and role model favourable behaviours(Reference Sharma, Hoelscher and Kelder11, Reference Downs, Arnold and Marshall13, Reference Sharma, Hoelscher and Kelder14, Reference Fisher, Mitchell and Smiciklas-Wright16, Reference Fisher and Mitchell D Smiciklas-Wright17, Reference Gillman, Rifas-Shiman and Frazier65), which are important to promote dairy intake, particularly in young children. Creating an environment for children where the consumption of dairy foods with meals or as a snack is regular practice appears to be an effective method to increase intake(Reference Thompson, Bachman and Watson12, Reference Vue and Reicks15, Reference Fisher, Mitchell and Smiciklas-Wright16) – possibly through links with taste exposure but also through habit formation. For example, eating breakfast, as a single dietary behaviour, has a positive impact on dairy intake(Reference Albertson, Thompson and Franko21–Reference Barton, Eldridge and Thompson24, Reference Song, Chun and Kerver66). We found that intervention techniques targeted at parents or the family as a whole were related to increases in dairy intake. A recent review of interventions to improve children's diets, in general, also found studies that used direct strategies to engage parents, such as requesting their attendance or participation in sessions, were more likely to report positive outcomes in children than those that relied on parents getting involved indirectly(Reference Hingle, O'Connor and Dave67). It appears engaging parents in interventions directly through the use of targeted behavioural techniques, or through strategies that require their involvement, shows promise and warrants inclusion into future research and education campaigns.
The present review has identified relevant literature to inform the development of future intervention aiming to increase children's dairy intakes, and in turn Ca intakes. To ensure findings were relevant to children's current context, studies were limited to those published in the last 20 years. A variety of intervention designs from clinical studies to population-level programmes were reviewed. Different measurement methods yield different results(Reference Stevens, Taber and Murray68); hence our results need to be considered within the context of the review. For example, two studies analysed in the review reported an absolute increase in dairy of 0·3 servings and one was significant(Reference Van Horn, Obarzanek and Friedman41) and the other not(Reference Epstein, Paluch and Beecher33). In the absence of uniformed reporting or effect size estimates, our definition of effectiveness was based on the significance of results as reported by the authors. While we acknowledge statistical significance is related to the assessment method and sample size, our effectiveness (or not) approach is applied elsewhere(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittales46, Reference Hingle, O'Connor and Dave67). Although a majority of academic communication is published in English, limiting the review to English-language papers may have created a small bias toward American papers. Only published literature was reviewed, which may have also biased the selection to more favourable outcomes and could partially explain the high proportion of successful interventions. This phenomenon is difficult to overcome. Inclusion of grey literature may identify more unsuccessful attempts to increase children's dairy intake not captured in this search, reducing our publication bias and providing more information about characteristics of ineffective intervention.
Only one-third of studies reported follow-up measures; therefore it is unclear what happens to dairy intake after the intervention period, particularly if provision of dairy foods was an integral component of the intervention. Studies of greater length are needed to make it possible to draw conclusions about the longer-term effectiveness of behaviour change and the associated health benefits for children in terms of bone density. For example, to achieve longer-term behaviour change according to social cognitive theory, barriers to dairy intake may need to be overcome. Although identification of barriers has featured in cross-sectional and focus group research, none of the interventions specifically targeting dairy, that we reviewed, utilised this technique. While the provision of dairy overcomes an immediate barrier to intake (availability), longer-term barriers, such as taste and perceived affordability, are also likely to be important. For long-term intervention to succeed intensity cannot remain high without available resources, including dollars. Few studies reported on costs but economic evidence must become part of health behaviour research as this influences the sustainability of the behaviour change.
Conclusions
Interventions that target an increase in children's dairy food or Ca intake could potentially increase children dairy food intake by about one serving daily. The current review has identified some promising strategies likely to be part of effective intervention for improving dairy and Ca intakes in countries where children's intake is insufficient (summarised in Table 4). Research to examine the effectiveness of interventions, conducted outside the USA, is required. The findings from our review can inform future interventions which aim to address the shortfall in children's Ca intake.
Acknowledgements
This research was co-funded by Dairy Australia and the Commonwealth Scientific Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO). CSIRO designed and conducted the systematic review, and prepared the manuscript. Dairy Australia reviewed the manuscript and approved it for publication. This review did not require ethics approval. The authors declare no conflicts of interest. All authors were involved in at least two of the following three processes: the development of the selection criteria, data extraction process and writing of results. G.A.H. drafted the manuscript. All authors contributed to the revisions of the manuscript and approved the final manuscript for publication.
Supplementary Materials
For Supplementary Materials for this article, please visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980012001322