Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T05:39:58.620Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Improvement of meal composition by vegetable variety

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 May 2011

Tamara Bucher*
Affiliation:
ETH Zürich, Institute for Environmental Decisions, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CHN J 78, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Klazine van der Horst
Affiliation:
ETH Zürich, Institute for Environmental Decisions, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CHN J 78, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
Michael Siegrist
Affiliation:
ETH Zürich, Institute for Environmental Decisions, Consumer Behavior, Universitaetstrasse 22, CHN J 78, CH-8092 Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective

Our physical environment influences our daily food choices unconsciously. Strategic changes in the food environment might therefore be potential measures to influence consumers’ food selection towards better nutrition, without affecting the consumers’ freedom of choice. The present study aimed to examine whether increased vegetable variety enhances healthy food choices and improves meal composition.

Design

A randomised experiment.

Setting

Participants were instructed to serve themselves a lunch from a buffet of food replicas. Individuals were randomly assigned to one of three food combinations: condition A – cooked carrots, pasta and chicken; condition B – cooked green garden beans, pasta and chicken; condition AB – carrot sticks, green garden beans, pasta and chicken. Two one-vegetable conditions were compared with one two-vegetable condition. Data from Zurich, Switzerland, were analysed using one-way ANOVA.

Subjects

Ninety-eight students (fifty-three men; mean age 22·8 (sd 2·2) years, minimum = 19 years, maximum = 29 years).

Results

Participants who could choose from two vegetables derived significantly more energy (141 kJ) from vegetables compared with participants in the one-vegetable condition (104 and 84 kJ, respectively). Furthermore, in the two-vegetable condition, the relative energy of the meal derived from vegetables (10·9 %) increased significantly compared with the one-vegetable condition (8 % and 6·1 %, respectively). The total energy content of the meal (mean 1472 (sd 468) kJ) was not affected by the experimental manipulation.

Conclusions

Having a choice of vegetables increases a person's tendency to choose vegetables and leads to the selection of a more balanced meal. Serving an assortment of vegetables as side dishes might be a simple and effective strategy to improve food selection.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2011

A balanced diet means eating a variety of foods in healthy proportions. A healthy balanced diet should be based on fruit, vegetables and whole foods high in nutrients(13). Consuming an unbalanced diet can cause weight gain and poor health(Reference Melhorn, Krause and Scott4, Reference Grundy5), whereas consumption of high amounts of fruit and vegetables has been suggested as a potential dietary strategy for optimizing health(Reference Rolls, Ello-Martin and Tohill6). A diet rich in fruit and vegetables has been shown to reduce the risk of cancer(7) and CVD(Reference Bazzano, He and Ogden8), improve bone health(Reference Prynne, Mishra and O'Connell9, Reference Tucker, Hannan and Chen10) and reduce age-related cognitive decline(Reference Morris, Evans and Tangney11). Such a diet may even result in weight loss(Reference Epstein, Gordy and Raynor12, Reference Lanza, Schatzkin and Daston13). To increase people's fruit and vegetable intakes, it is important to understand the factors influencing human food choice and meal composition. Previous research has shown that, in addition to psychological factors such as cognition and social influences(Reference Shepherd14), environmental factors affect food choice(Reference Hill and Peters15Reference Kremers, de Bruijn and Visscher17). Therefore, changes in the food environment, such as increasing the availability of healthy foods, are potential measures to influence consumers’ food selection towards better nutrition without affecting consumers’ freedom of choice. Environmental measures within public eating areas are of importance, as modern living has led to an increase in the frequency and variety of meals and snacks consumed away from home(Reference Orfanos, Naska and Trichopoulos18, Reference Orfanos, Naska and Trichopoulou19).

To classify environmental influences on food choice, Herman and Polivy(Reference Herman and Polivy20) introduced the distinction between normative and external (sensory) cues. Although sensory cues refer to the hedonic appeal of the food (e.g. palatability and odour), normative cues refer to indicators of appropriate intake, such as portion sizes. The authors argue that sensory cues are especially powerful for certain groups, such as obese or dieting/restrainted individuals, whereas normative cues exert control over food selection for all. It was shown that the variety of foods presented(Reference Remick, Polivy and Pliner21), portion sizes(Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe22), the plate size, or even the number of peers at the table can influence consumption(Reference Wansink16). Although several studies have shown how these cues can cause overconsumption and poor nutrition(Reference Rolls, Morris and Roe22Reference Wansink24), little is known about implementation of this knowledge to actually improve consumers’ food choice(Reference Wansink25, Reference Rolls, Roe and Meengs26).

In the present study, the focus lies in how variety might be strategically used to improve meal composition. In general, variety in a meal is thought to enhance food intake(Reference Remick, Polivy and Pliner21). Consumers offered an assortment of three different flavours of yoghurt consumed on average 23 % more than when offered only one flavour(Reference Rolls, Rowe and Rolls27). Kahn and Wansink(Reference Kahn and Wansink23) have shown that people eat more candies if they come in more colours. Therefore, it does not matter whether the variety is real (e.g. food tastes very different) or just perceived (e.g. food structures differ, but tastes are identical). Both increase the quantities consumed. The effect of variety on food consumption is commonly explained by a reduction of sensory-specific satiety. Sensory-specific satiety refers to the finding that as a food is eaten its palatability gradually declines. Greater variety is assumed to overcome sensory-specific satiety and therefore lead to increased food intake. The variety effect might have been advantageous during human evolution, as a varied diet is more likely to cover all essential nutrients. In today's affluent society, though, the effect has rather become a threat to human health.

Past studies have mainly focused on single foods instead of examining the influence of a variety in healthy foods on consumers’ meal composition. The focus of the present study lies in variety as a modifiable normative cue to improve consumers’ food choice. The hypothesis is that by increasing the variety of healthy foods, namely vegetables, consumers can be manipulated towards a healthier and more balanced diet. This might be explained by the so-called 1/N equality heuristic, suggested by DeMiguel et al.(Reference DeMiguel, Garlappi and Uppal28). The heuristic originates in economics and addresses the question of how money is invested in N assets. It states the tendency of people to allocate money equally to each of N funds. Similarly, people might allocate the limited space on a serving plate equally to different foods offered. If this hypothesis is true for food choice, people offered more vegetables should not only just choose more vegetables but should also improve the composition of the overall meal. In summary, we expect participants with a wider choice of vegetables to choose a higher proportion of vegetables and reduce the selection of other foods at the same time.

Methods

To accurately investigate the influence of vegetable availability on serving amounts and meal composition, a fake food buffet (FFB) was used. A buffet of food replicas cast from polyvinylchloride (Döring GmbH, Munich, Germany) was prepared. Four foods commonly eaten for a warm meal in Switzerland were selected: carrots (vegetable A), green beans (vegetable B), pasta and fried skinless chicken breast. All items were placed in aluminium serving dishes to remind students of the local university cafeteria.

Subjects

An invitation to participate in the experiment was sent to the subject pool of the University of Zürich. Students taking classes in Nutrition Science, following a medically prescribed diet or following a vegetarian diet were excluded from study participation. Individuals aged >30 years were also excluded from the experiment, as age is a potential modifier of the effect of variety(Reference Remick, Polivy and Pliner21). Ninety-eight individuals (fifty-three men and forty-five women) completed the experiment.

Experimental procedure

Each participant was randomly assigned to one of three replica food selections. The buffet for group 1 (n 29, seventeen men) consisted of cooked carrots, pasta and chicken (condition A). Group 2 (n 35, nineteen men) could serve themselves beans, pasta and chicken (condition B) and group 3 (n 34, seventeen men) had both vegetables in addition to pasta and chicken (condition AB; Fig. 1). Participants were provided with a standard serving plate (27 cm diameter) and asked to serve themselves a meal such as they would normally eat for lunch from the presented selection. After serving, the participants filled out a food questionnaire. Meanwhile, the investigator quantified each food component of the meal by weighing it (Shimadzu UW6200H; Swiss Waagen DC GmbH, Uster, Switzerland). Students were compensated with CHF 15 ($US 15) for participation. Subsequent to study completion, participants were informed about the study aim. Data were collected and analysed in Zürich in 2010.

Fig. 1 Experimental conditions: condition A – carrots were offered along with pasta and chicken; condition B – beans were offered along with pasta and chicken; condition AB – both vegetables were offered in addition to chicken and pasta

Calculation of the theoretical energy content of replica food items

To estimate the theoretical energy content of the replica foods, corresponding real foods were prepared and weight was measured (for comparison of real and replica food, see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Food replicas and real foods in comparison: (a) raw beans (lower panel) and replica beans (upper panel); (b) raw carrots (lower panel) and replica carrot sticks (upper panel); (c) cooked pasta (right) and replica pasta (left); and (d) fried chicken breast (upper panel) and replica chicken (lower panel)

Carrots

Carrots (vegetable A) were chopped into pieces comparable to the replica carrots. Equal volumes of replica carrots and real carrots were selected. The weight of real and replica carrots was measured and a factor was calculated. This procedure was repeated three times. The average conversion factor was used for energy estimation of 100 g of replica carrots. The conversion factor from carrots to replica carrots was 1·10 (sd 0·06); therefore, 100 g of replica carrots had a theoretical energy content of 149 kJ (fresh carrots (100 g) = 135 kJ(29)).

Beans

Green garden beans (vegetable B) were chopped into pieces comparable to the replica beans. Forty replica beans and forty real beans were selected randomly. The weight of real and replica beans was measured and a factor was calculated. This procedure was repeated three times. The average conversion factor from beans to replica beans was 0·73 (sd 0·01); therefore, 100 g of replica beans had a theoretical energy content of 75 kJ (fresh beans (100 g) = 103 kJ(29)).

Pasta

A type of pasta was selected that looked similar in size and shape to the replica pasta after cooking. Forty pieces of real pasta and forty pieces of replica pasta were selected randomly. The weight of the real and replica pasta was measured and the conversion factor was calculated. The procedure was repeated three times. The average conversion factor from real pasta to replica pasta was 0·30 (sd 0·01); therefore, 100 g of replica pasta corresponded to 455 kJ (egg pasta (100 g) = 1498 kJ(29)).

Chicken

Chicken breast was flavoured and subsequently fried in a pan. The weight of the raw and fried meat was measured, as well as the frying oil remaining on the chicken. The chicken breasts were then chopped into pieces similar to the replica food. Six similarly shaped pieces of real and replica meat were selected and measured to calculate a conversion factor. The procedure was repeated three times. The average conversion factor from fried chicken breast to replica chicken breast was 0·98 (sd 0·03); therefore, 100 g of replica chicken corresponded to 636 kJ (fried chicken breast (100 g) = 456 kJ(29)). Note that, for chicken, the oil remaining on the meat after frying was taken into account, whereas for all other food items no addition of fat was assumed.

Measures

The first outcome variable ‘total energy from vegetable’ was measured by weighing the replica vegetables that were served. The weights of the replica vegetables were multiplied by the conversion factors and summed up to compute the theoretical energy content from the vegetables served. Similarly, the second outcome variable ‘total energy meal’ was established by weighing all food items. The weight of the individual food items was multiplied by the corresponding conversion factor and added up to compute the theoretical energy content of the meal. The third outcome variable ‘percentage energy from vegetables’ was determined by computing the relative energy served from vegetables compared with the total energy served. The inverse variable, percentage energy from non-vegetables, was calculated in the same way.

After the participants served themselves a meal, they provided self-reported height, weight and age measures. These measures were used to calculate their BMI (weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in metres (kg/m2)) and daily energy requirement (weight multiplied by energy per kilogram body weight factor, which depends on age and gender(30)). Note that calculated energy requirements might be lower than true energy requirements, as there is a general tendency to under-report weight and over-report height(Reference Perez-Cueto and Verbeke31). The students completed questions about their levels of hunger, food-frequency questions on offered food items and questions on food preferences and the authenticity of the replica foods. Hunger was assessed with the question ‘How hungry are you right now?’ on a 6-point scale (1 = not hungry at all; 6 = very hungry). Food preferences were assessed with the question ‘How much do you like the following food items?’ on a 6-point scale (1 = I do not like it at all; 6 = I like it very much). The authenticity of the replica foods was measured with the question ‘How realistic were the following replica foods?’ on another 6-point scale (1 = not realistic at all; 6 = very realistic).

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed using the Predictive Analytics Software (PASW; formerly known as SPSS) statistical software package version 18·0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). To assess whether the three study conditions were successfully randomized by age, BMI, hunger and food preference, one-way ANOVA was performed. Three assessments using univariate ANOVA were conducted to determine whether the buffet conditions were associated with the three outcome variables, and the effect sizes (η 2) are reported. Gender × condition interactions were analysed using two-way ANOVA. In case of a significant omnibus F test, planned group comparisons were made using the Bonferroni correction. Mean differences (D) and se are reported. All tests are based on a 0·05 significance level.

Results

A total of ninety-eight individuals participated in the present study, of whom 45·9 % were female (n 45) and 54·1 % were male (n 53). One-way ANOVA did not detect any significant differences between the experimental groups in terms of age (22·8 (sd 2·25) years), BMI (22·0 (sd 2·5) kg/m2) or hunger levels (2·9 (sd 1·3); Table 1). There was a slight difference in food preference for carrots (vegetable A) between the groups. Participants who were not presented any carrots stated liking them slightly more than participants who could actually serve themselves carrots. The effect, however, was negligible (F (2, 95) = 3·14, P = 0·048, η 2 = 0·062). High mean ratings for the authenticity of replica food items give an indication of the usefulness of replica foods (carrots: 4·43 (sd 1·06); beans: 4·34 (sd 1·18); chicken: 3·33 (sd 1·54); pasta: 4·61 (sd 1·18)).

Table 1 Differences in background and demographic variables of participants in the three experimental conditions (one-way ANOVA)

Vegetable A, cooked carrots; vegetable B, cooked beans.

*One individual refused to state her weight. Total n for BMI is therefore 97.

†Hunger levels were measured on a 6-point scale (1 = not hungry at all; 6 = very hungry).

‡Food preference was measured on a 6-point scale (1 = I do not like it at all; 6 = I like it very much).

To test whether vegetable variety affects the total energy from vegetables (kJ), the means of the energy derived from vegetables were compared between experimental groups (Table 2). ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of vegetable variety (F (2, 95) = 23·776, P = 0·000, η 2 = 0·334) on the total energy derived from vegetables. The energy derived from vegetables increased significantly when two vegetables were offered instead of only one. Planned contrasts with Bonferroni's correction showed statistically significant differences between condition AB (carrots and beans) and conditions A (carrots only; D = 8·87, se 2·11, P = 0·000) and B (beans only; D = 13·71, se 2·11, P = 0·000). To exclude interactions of gender with experimental conditions, a two-way ANOVA was performed. No significant interaction was found (F (2, 92) = 0·047, P = 0·954, η 2 = 0·001).

Table 2 Differences in meal composition depending on the experimental conditions (one-way ANOVA)

a,bMean values within rows with unlike superscript letters were significantly different using the Bonferroni test (α = 0·05).

A significant main effect of vegetable variety on the percentage energy from vegetables was found (F (2, 95) = 12·229, P = 0·000, η 2 = 0·205). The percentage energy from vegetables was significantly higher in condition AB compared with conditions A (D = 2·83 %, se 1·01, P = 0·018) and B (D = 4·74 %, se 0·96, P = 0·000). No significant interaction of the experimental condition with gender was found (F (2, 92) = 0·335, P = 0·716, η 2 = 0·007). Few people in the AB condition chose only one vegetable (n 2, 5·9 %). We can therefore rule out that people offered two vegetables served themselves more vegetables because they had a better chance of finding their favourite vegetable.

Further, the variety of vegetables presented did not influence the total energy of the meal (F (2, 95) = 1·071, P = 0·347, η 2 = 0·022). There are, however, significant differences in total energy (kJ) served between genders. Men (mean = 1699 (se 66) kJ) served themselves on average 494 kJ more compared with women (mean = 1205 (se 40) kJ; t (96) = 6·11, P = 0·000, r = 0·42). The total amount of energy served from the buffet correlated with the individual daily energy requirement (r = 0·35, P = 0·000).

Discussion

The present study shows that, by increasing the vegetable variety, consumers’ food selection can successfully be influenced towards a healthier food choice. An increase in vegetable variety results in a change in meal composition. Giving participants one additional vegetable to choose from increases the total as well as the relative energy served from vegetables. In contrast, the relative energy derived from non-vegetables (e.g. pasta and chicken) is significantly decreased. This indicates that participants chose a more balanced meal containing a higher proportion of vegetables.

Interestingly, the increase in the variety of vegetables did not affect the total energy served, even though the variety effect is in general known to increase consumption(Reference Remick, Polivy and Pliner21). This finding might be explained by limiting plate space. The plate restricts the maximal amount of food that can be served. Even though more food items are served once variety increases, plate space is limited at one point. Here, relative energy differences between food items are suspected to play a role. Foods relatively low in energy were chosen for the experiment. It is possible that, by substituting the relatively low-energy-dense chicken and pasta with foods higher in energy density, such as pork and fries, the total energy of the meal might even be reduced when two vegetables are offered instead of one. For the variety effect within a meal, however, it seems important to identify in which food items variety lies. If there is a wider variety of low-energy-dense foods, such as vegetables, the total energy of the meal does not necessarily increase. On the contrary, it might even be strategically used to reduce energy intake.

The limited plate space forced participants to decide which foods to serve and in what quantity. We assume that participants allocate the available space of the plate equally to chosen food items, which might be explained by the 1/N equality heuristic. People may also be likely to allocate space on the plate equally to N food items. We name this observed tendency the ‘plate space equality heuristic’. Future studies should quantify the relationship between the number of foods offered and the plate space available. It would also be of interest to know how preferences and aversions influence plate space allocation.

Even though the food replicas were of outstanding quality, the foods were fake. However, we found high correlations between the served amounts of replica foods from an FFB and those from a buffet of corresponding real foods (T Bucher, K van der Horst and M Siegrist, unpublished results). Using replica food has numerous advantages over real food. Most important, the replicas allow studying food choice under well-controlled laboratory conditions and each individually tested participant finds an identical food buffet. A limitation of using replica foods might be that the served theoretical energy may not be identical to the consumed energy from an identical buffet of real foods. In a real-life situation, parts of meals might be left over or a second serving eaten(Reference Rolls, Roe and Halverson32). It is known from previous research, however, that people tend to empty their plates(Reference Wansink16) and take single portions as units for consumption, rather than compensating by eating more portions(Reference Geier, Rozin and Doros33). No sauces were offered along with the replica food items, and minimal fat was assumed for meal preparation and calculation of weight factors. A meal from replica foods might therefore seem lower in energy when compared with the same meal composed from real foods. The study population included students only and hence may not represent the general population.

To verify whether participants behaved naturally in the experiment, gender differences in total energy served were quantified. It was found that women served themselves less energy-dense food compared with men. In addition, the energy served correlated with the actual personal energy requirement. In addition, students rated the appearance of the replica food items as very realistic. Altogether, these findings indicate that participants behaved naturally when serving themselves the replica food.

Eating a balanced diet high in fruit and vegetables is common advice for maintaining good health. However, human food selection is not driven only by individual decisions. Food and eating environments are likely to influence food choice over and even above individual factors such as knowledge, skills or even motivation(Reference Story, Kaphingst and Robinson-O'Brien34). The present findings show that a simple and inexpensive environmental measure, i.e. increasing the selection of healthy foods, can improve consumers’ food choices. This finding might be of interest for public health authorities to promote adapting out-of-home eating environments. Consumers themselves could profit from this measure at home. We suggest that serving an assortment of vegetables as side dishes might be a simple and effective strategy to improve meal composition.

Acknowledgements

The present study received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. The authors have no conflict of interest to declare. T.B. contributed towards data acquisition, data analysis and led the writing. K.v.d.H. and M.S. helped interpret the results and provided critical feedback on drafts of the article. All authors contributed to the design of the study.

References

1.US Department of Agriculture (1992) Food Guide Pyramid. A Guide to Daily Food Choices [US Department of Health and Human Services, editor]. Hyattsville, MD: USDA.Google Scholar
2.Derrickson, JP, Sakai, M & Anderson, J (2001) Interpretations of the ‘balanced meal’ household food security indicator. J Nutr Educ 33, 155160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Bundeasmt für Gesundheit BAG (2007) Empfehlungen für eine ausgewogene Ernährung [Bundesamt für Gesundheit DV, editor]. Bern: Bundesamt für Gesundheit.Google Scholar
4.Melhorn, SJ, Krause, EG, Scott, KA et al. (2010) Acute exposure to a high-fat diet alters meal patterns and body composition. Physiol Behav 99, 3339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Grundy, SM (2000) Metabolic complications of obesity. Endocrine 13, 155165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Rolls, BJ, Ello-Martin, JA & Tohill, BC (2004) What can intervention studies tell us about the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and weight management? Nutr Rev 62, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.World Cancer Research Fund & American Institute for Cancer Research (2007) Food, Nutrition, Physical Activity, and the Prevention of Cancer: A Global Perspective. Washington, DC: WCRF/AICR.Google Scholar
8.Bazzano, LA, He, J, Ogden, LG et al. (2002) Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of cardiovascular disease in US adults: the first National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Epidemiologic Follow-up Study. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 9399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Prynne, CJ, Mishra, GD, O'Connell, MA et al. (2006) Fruit and vegetable intakes and bone mineral status: a cross sectional study in 5 age and sex cohorts. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 14201428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Tucker, KL, Hannan, MT, Chen, H et al. (1999) Potassium, magnesium, and fruit and vegetable intakes are associated with greater bone mineral density in elderly men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 69, 727736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11.Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Tangney, CC et al. (2006) Associations of vegetable and fruit consumption with age-related cognitive change. Neurology 67, 13701376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12.Epstein, LH, Gordy, CC, Raynor, HA et al. (2001) Increasing fruit and vegetable intake and decreasing fat and sugar intake in families at risk for childhood obesity. Obes Res 9, 171178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13.Lanza, E, Schatzkin, A, Daston, C et al. (2001) Implementation of a 4-y, high-fiber, high-fruit-and-vegetable, low-fat dietary intervention: results of dietary changes in the Polyp Prevention Trial. Am J Clin Nutr 74, 387401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14.Shepherd, R (2006) The Psychology of Food Choice. Wallingford: CABI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15.Hill, JO & Peters, JC (1998) Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science 280, 13711374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16.Wansink, B (2004) Environmental factors that increase the food intake and consumption volume of unknowing consumers. Annu Rev Nutr 24, 455479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Kremers, SP, de Bruijn, GJ, Visscher, TL et al. (2006) Environmental influences on energy balance-related behaviors: a dual-process view. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Orfanos, P, Naska, A, Trichopoulos, D et al. (2007) Eating out of home and its correlates in 10 European countries. The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC) study. Public Health Nutr 10, 15151525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Orfanos, P, Naska, A, Trichopoulou, A et al. (2009) Eating out of home: energy, macro- and micronutrient intakes in 10 European countries. The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition. Eur J Clin Nutr 63, Suppl. 4, S239S262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.Herman, CP & Polivy, J (2008) External cues in the control of food intake in humans: the sensory–normative distinction. Physiol Behav 94, 722728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Remick, AK, Polivy, J & Pliner, P (2009) Internal and external moderators of the effect of variety on food intake. Psychol Bull 135, 434451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Rolls, BJ, Morris, EL & Roe, LS (2002) Portion size of food affects energy intake in normal-weight and overweight men and women. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 12071213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Kahn, BE & Wansink, B (2004) The influence of assortment structure on perceived variety and consumption quantities. J Consum Res 30, 519533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
24.Wansink, B (2006) Mindless Eating: Why We Eat More Than We Think. New York, NY: Bantam.Google Scholar
25.Wansink, B (2010) From mindless eating to mindlessly eating better. Physiol Behav 100, 454463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS & Meengs, JS (2010) Portion size can be used strategically to increase vegetable consumption in adults. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 913922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.Rolls, BJ, Rowe, EA, Rolls, ET et al. (1981) Variety in a meal enhances food intake in man. Physiol Behav 26, 215221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
28.DeMiguel, V, Garlappi, L & Uppal, R (2009) Optimal versus naive diversification: how inefficient is the 1/N portfolio strategy? Rev Financ Stud 22, 19151953.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
29.ETH Zürich (2010) SwissFIR. http://www.swissfir.ethz.ch/(accessed December 2010).Google Scholar
30.Deutsche Gesellschaft für Ernährung (2001) Referenzwerte für die Nährstoffzufuhr. Frankfurt am Main: Umschau/Braus.Google Scholar
31.Perez-Cueto, FJA & Verbeke, W (2009) Reliability and validity of self-reported weight and height in Belgium. Nutr Hosp 24, 366367.Google ScholarPubMed
32.Rolls, BJ, Roe, LS, Halverson, KH et al. (2007) Using a smaller plate did not reduce energy intake at meals. Appetite 49, 652660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
33.Geier, AB, Rozin, P & Doros, G (2006) Unit bias: a new heuristic that helps explain the effect of portion size on food intake. Psychol Sci 17, 521525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34.Story, M, Kaphingst, KM, Robinson-O'Brien, R et al. (2008) Creating healthy food and eating environments: policy and environmental approaches. Annu Rev Public Health 29, 253272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Experimental conditions: condition A – carrots were offered along with pasta and chicken; condition B – beans were offered along with pasta and chicken; condition AB – both vegetables were offered in addition to chicken and pasta

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Food replicas and real foods in comparison: (a) raw beans (lower panel) and replica beans (upper panel); (b) raw carrots (lower panel) and replica carrot sticks (upper panel); (c) cooked pasta (right) and replica pasta (left); and (d) fried chicken breast (upper panel) and replica chicken (lower panel)

Figure 2

Table 1 Differences in background and demographic variables of participants in the three experimental conditions (one-way ANOVA)

Figure 3

Table 2 Differences in meal composition depending on the experimental conditions (one-way ANOVA)