Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T01:00:11.513Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Household food insecurity and early childhood development in Brazil: an analysis of children under 2 years of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2021

Klébya HD de Oliveira*
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, University of Brasilia, Darcy Ribeiro University Campus, BrasiliaDF 70910-900, Brazil
Gabriela Buccini
Affiliation:
Department of Environmental Health, School of Public Health, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, NV, USA
Daphne C Hernandez
Affiliation:
Department of Research, Cizik School of Nursing, University of Texas Health Science Center, Houston, TX, USA
Rafael Pérez-Escamilla
Affiliation:
Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Yale School Public Health, New Haven, CT, USA
Muriel B Gubert
Affiliation:
Department of Nutrition, University of Brasilia, Darcy Ribeiro University Campus, BrasiliaDF 70910-900, Brazil
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To determine if household food insecurity (HFI) is associated with the risk of developmental delays.

Design:

Cross-sectional study of a representative sample of children under 2 years old. Risk of developmental delays was assessed with the Denver Developmental Screening Test II. HFI was measured with the Brazilian Food Insecurity Measurement Scale. Multivariable logistic regression was used to test the association between HFI (food secure/insecure) and risk of developmental delays, adjusting for household, maternal and child variables.

Setting:

Community Health Centers in the Federal District, Brazil.

Participants:

1004 children under 2 years old.

Results:

Among participants, 15 % were at risk of developmental delays and about 40 % of children lived in food-insecure households. HFI was associated with the risk of developmental delays (adjusted OR 2·61; 95 % CI 1·42, 4·80) compared with food-secure households after adjusting for key confounders.

Conclusions:

HFI was strongly associated with the risk of developmental delays in children under 2 years. Investments that prevent or mitigate HFI are likely to be key for improved human and national development.

Type
Research paper
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Investing in early childhood development (ECD) is linked with better health, human capital and lifelong well-being(Reference McCoy, Peet and Ezzati1Reference Heckman, Pinto and Savelyev4). Despite advances in improving child survival, millions of children are at risk of not reaching their full development potential, especially in low- and middle-income countries(Reference Lu, Black and Richter5). According to UNICEF’s Early Childhood Development Index, 33 % of children in low- and middle-income countries have low cognitive and/or socioemotional development(Reference McCoy, Peet and Ezzati1). This may be a result of the continued exposure to multiple adversities such as poverty, violence and household food insecurity (HFI) that affect brain architecture by limiting child’s social, cognitive and emotional development, causing negative impacts throughout their life(Reference Johnson and Markowitz6Reference Huang, Potochnick and Heflin12).

HFI is the lack of regular access to enough safe and nutritious food for normal growth and development and an active and healthy life(13). Over one-quarter (25·9 %) of the world population has been found to experience moderate and severe HFI(14). In Brazil, more than a third of the population (36·7 %) have HFI and almost one in two young children (49·9 %) live in food-insecure households(15). HFI has been associated with poor health outcomes in children; e.g. children had a greater likelihood of having cough and being hospitalised for diarrhoea(Reference Gubert, Spaniol and Bortolini16,Reference Poblacion, Cook and Marin-Leon17) . Furthermore, HFI has been linked to food scarcity(Reference Bernal, Frongillo and Jaffe18,Reference Ke and Ford-Jones19) , poor diet quality(Reference Rodríguez, Mundo-Rosas and Méndez-Gómez-Humarán20,Reference Fram, Ritchie and Rosen21) , psycho-emotional stress(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7) and poor maternal mental health(Reference Wu, Harwood and Feng22Reference Cook, Black and Chilton24) – all of which are also risk factors for poor ECD. Literature reviews have consistently found that HFI, even at mild levels, is negatively associated with developmental outcomes(Reference Shankar, Chung and Frank8,Reference Cook and Frank25) compared with children living in food-secure households(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7). A comprehensive systematic review and meta-analysis found that HFI was associated with developmental risk and poor math skills in high-income countries and with poor vocabulary skills in low-, middle- and high-income countries(Reference de Oliveira, de Almeida and Gubert26).

Recent evidence from a pooled analysis of thirteen low- and middle-income countries found that low birth weight, preterm birth and anaemia in infancy were significant risk factors for poorer cognitive and motor development(Reference Sania, Sudfeld and Danaei27). In addition, parental factors such as low maternal education and short maternal stature have been positively associated with cognitive, motor and language development scores, but HFI was not investigated as a risk factor(Reference Sania, Sudfeld and Danaei27). There is a lack of evidence exploring the association between HFI and risk of developmental delays, specifically among young children under 2 years of age. Focusing on children under 2 years is important due to their brain plasticity. In the first years of life, the brain is more prone to changes in responses to environmental experiences and adapts to adversities experienced, setting either a positive or a negative developmental trajectory for life(Reference Ismail, Fatemi and Johnston28Reference Fernald, Prado and Kariger31). This study aimed to fill this gap by evaluating the association between HFI and risk of developmental delays among Brazilian children under 2 years of age.

Methods

Sampling and data collection

This is a cross-sectional study conducted in Community Health Centers (CHC) in the Federal District, Brazil. The sampling process included two stages. In the first stage, twenty out of 131 existing CHC that monitor child growth and development in the Federal District were randomly selected. In the second stage, the number of children to be included in each CHC was estimated based on self-weighted sampling stratified into two age groups (0–12 and 12–24 months). The study sample was designed to be a representative of children attending primary care visits in the Federal District. Assuming a confidence level of 95 %, an error of 5 % and considering a maximum sample loss of 10 %, the minimum sample size calculated was 856 mother–child dyads. Full-term children up to 2 years of age accompanied by their biological mothers were eligible for the study. Preterm, twins or children with congenital malformations or diagnosed pathologies that impact on physical or cognitive development were not included in the study. Children with previous medical diagnosis of developmental delays or who had undergone major surgery were excluded from the study.

In the selected CHC, on the days of data collection, a trained research assistant invited mothers and their children under 2 years of age to participate in the research. The data collection instrument included closed-ended questions related to the children and mother’s socio-economic, demographic and biomedical profiles as well as standard tools for assessing the ECD and HFI. Data were collected between March 2017 and March 2018. Quality control was carried out with a random subsample of 20 % of the sample, through the replication of three different questions by telephone within 4 weeks after participating in the research.

Measurements

Outcome variable

ECD was assessed using the Denver Developmental Screening Test II (DDSTII)(Reference Frankenburg, Dodds and Archer32). This tool had been previously translated and adapted in Brazil(Reference de Souza, Leone and Takano33). DDSTII assesses the child’s risk of developmental delays across the four domains: personal-social (child’s socialisation skills inside and outside the family environment); fine motor (hand eye coordination skills and small object manipulation); language (sound emission and ability to recognise, understand and use language) and gross motor (body motor control and ability to perform broad muscle movements)(Reference Frankenburg and Dodds34). According to the DDSTII, developmental skills were classified as normal (0 item performed as delay for age and ≤1 item performed as caution for age) or suspect (≥1 item performed as delay for age and/or ≥2 items performed as caution for age)(Reference Frankenburg, Dodds and Archer32,Reference Frankenburg and Dodds34) . The outcome considered in this study was the risk of developmental delays which included children who had suspect performance across one or more developmental domains.

ECD was evaluated in a private room in the selected CHC by previously trained researchers. To ensure accuracy when applying the test, the researchers answered a self-administration checklist of DDSTII(Reference Frankenburg and Dodds34) during the first ten evaluations. Concurrent examiner–observer reliability was determined in a random subsample of 5 % of the sample, and interobserver reliability analysis was performed by agreement on the classification of developmental skills (κ = 0·62, P < 0·0001).

After the assessment, mothers of children found to be at risk of developmental delays were offered information about early life actions to foster ECD, such as adequate stimulation, strengthening caregiver/child bonds and healthy eating practices. In addition, mothers were encouraged to discuss these results in the follow-up appointment with the child’s paediatrician.

Independent variable

HFI was measured with the experience-based Brazilian Food Insecurity Measurement Scale (Escala Brasileira de Insegurança Alimentar, EBIA), which contains fourteen questions about experiencing HFI in the previous 90 d(15). EBIA is a reliable and valid scale derived from the Household Food Security Survey Module and is the official household food security measure in Brazil(15,Reference Segall-Corrêa, Marin-León and Melgar-Quiñonez35) . In this study, the additive score of affirmative responses to EBIA’s items was used to classify households as food secure (0) and food insecure (1–14) (initially recoded as mild, moderate or severe HFI, and subsequently recoded as food secure v. HFI due to sample size limitations)(15).

Covariates

The covariates or potential confounders were selected based on theoretical grounds and empirical evidence supporting their associations with both HFI and ECD(Reference Sania, Sudfeld and Danaei27). The household variables included were head of household (mother, other (i.e. both parents, father, grandparents)), participation in any social government programme (yes, no), number of children under 5 years of age at home (1, ≥2) and number of rooms in the home (1, ≥2). The maternal variables included were educational level (≤8 years, ≥9 years), employment status (working outside home, not working outside home/on maternity leave), parity (nulliparous, multiparous), interpregnancy interval (<2 years, ≥2 years), type of delivery (vaginal, caesarean), early initiation of prenatal care (≤12 weeks of gestation) (yes, no) and the habit of drinking alcoholic beverages during pregnancy (yes, no). The child variables were age (<12 months, 12–24 months), gender (male, female), skin colour (white, other), low birth weight (≤2·500 g) (yes, no), hospitalisation for any health problem in the previous year (yes, no), breast-feeding (yes (still breastfed), no (never breastfed or stopped breast-feeding)), food allergy/intolerance (yes, no), bottle feeding in the previous 24 h (yes, no) and pacifier use in the previous 24 h (yes, no).

Statistical analysis

The analytical sample of this study was 1004 children under 2 years of age. Out of the 1285 mothers who answered the survey, 87 (6·7 %) refused to participate, 33 (2·6 %) were excluded because ≥10 % of data missing of the total number of variables, 95 (7·4 %) had missing information on ECD or HFI, 33 (2·6 %) had untestable results according to the DDSTII assessment (i.e. refusal ≥1 item performed as delay for age or >1 item performed as caution for age), 20 (1·5 %) had previous medical diagnosis of developmental delays and 13 (1·0 %) had a major surgery. Potential confounders were collected from all invited mothers. The characteristics of the participants with complete data compared with the participants who refused to participate or had incomplete data were similar for key potential confounders such as maternal age, maternal educational level and child’s age.

Analyses were conducted with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences software (IBM SPSS, version 21.0). Descriptive analyses of the outcome, independent variable and covariates were performed. Bivariate analyses were conducted to verify the association between risk of developmental delays, HFI and covariates using χ 2 test. Covariates were selected for inclusion in a multivariable model when the association had a P < 0·20 in the bivariate analyses. Multivariable logistic regression coefficients examining the association of HFI with the risk of developmental delays were expressed as unadjusted and adjusted OR and corresponding 95 % CI. In all analyses, HFI was modelled as a dichotomous variable (household food secure v. food insecure).

Results

Descriptive statistics of the sample are showed in Table 1. A total of 15·1 % of the children were at risk of developmental delays. Nearly 40 % of children lived in food-insecure households (Table 1).

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics and prevalence of risk of developmental delays of children under 2 years and their mothers by household–maternal–child characteristics (n 1004). Federal District, Brazil, 2018

* P < 0·05.

Bivariate analyses indicated that the prevalence of risk of developmental delays was higher among children living under conditions of HFI than among children in food-secure households (55·9 % v. 44·0 %, respectively). Mother-headed households, short interpregnancy interval, child’s age (more than 12 months) and low birth weight were associated with a higher prevalence of risk for developmental delays (Table 1).

Unadjusted analyses indicated that HFI was negatively associated with the risk of developmental delays (OR 2·17; 95 % CI 1·53, 3·08). Multivariable logistic regression confirmed a strong negative association between HFI with the risk of developmental delays after adjusting for confounders (adjusted OR 2·61; 95 % CI 1·42, 4·80) (Table 2).

Table 2 Unadjusted and adjusted OR and 95% CI for early childhood development outcomes according to household food insecurity status (n 1004). Federal District, Brazil, 2018

* P < 0·01.

** P < 0·001.

Logistic regression analysis was performed. Risk of developmental delays was adjusted for maternal educational level, employment status, head of household, type of delivery, interpregnancy interval, habit of drinking alcoholic beverages during pregnancy, child’s age, low birth weight, food allergy/intolerance and bottle feeding in the previous 24 h.

Discussion

This study found that HFI is an independent risk factor for poor ECD outcomes among Brazilian children under 2 years of age. Our findings fill an important knowledge gap given the scarcity of literature focusing on HFI and ECD during the first 2 years of life, which is a highly sensitive period for brain development(Reference Knickmeyer, Gouttard and Kang36,Reference Aboud, Yousafzai, Black, Laxminarayan and Walker37) . Furthermore, our results documenting the independent association between HFI and risk of developmental delays among Brazilian children are consistent with findings from previous studies conducted in high-income countries(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7,Reference Shankar, Chung and Frank8,Reference Cook and Frank25) , indicating that HFI is associated with increased risk of developmental delays across contrasting socio-economic and cultural contexts. Our results are also consistent with previous studies that found young children living in food-insecure households have an increased chance for risk of developmental delays(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7,Reference Shankar, Chung and Frank8,Reference Cook and Frank25,Reference de Oliveira, de Almeida and Gubert26) .

The prevalence of risk for developmental delays found in our study is similar to prior US-based research examining ECD outcomes among children under 3 years of age (range: 14·0 %(Reference Rose-Jacobs, Black and Casey38) to 15·2 %(Reference Black, Quigg and Cook39)) and 4 years of age (11·5 %)(Reference Drennen, Coleman and de Cuba40). Our findings are consistent with previous estimates of risk for poor development in Brazil (range: 11 % to 14 %) based on the prevalence of stunting among children younger than 5 years and poverty ratios in 2010(Reference Lu, Black and Richter5). Likewise, the prevalence of HFI found in our study was similar to the Brazilian nationally representative estimates for households with children <4 years of age. In 2017–2018, 49·9 % of Brazilian households with children <4 years old were found to be living in households with mild, moderate or severe food insecurity(15).

The association between HFI and risk of developmental delays among children under 2 years of age in Brazil, which is an upper-middle income country, is consistent with prior studies in high-income(Reference de Oliveira, de Almeida and Gubert26,Reference Rose-Jacobs, Black and Casey38Reference Drennen, Coleman and de Cuba40) and low-middle income settings(Reference de Oliveira, de Almeida and Gubert26,Reference Saha, Tofail and Frongillo41,Reference Milner, Fiorella and Mattah42) . HFI can negatively impact the development of children and the well-being of caregivers in different ways. First, childhood hunger and/or inadequate nutrition can lead to micronutrient deficiencies, as well as lack of energy or increased fatigue, distraction and irritability(Reference Johnson and Markowitz6,Reference Tanner and Finn-Stevenson11) . As a result, children exposed to food insecurity can become less active and reduce the level of nurturing interactions with their caregivers. In return, this limits their opportunities to explore the environment, compromising their gross motor as well as their social and language development(Reference Johnson and Markowitz6,Reference Tanner and Finn-Stevenson11) . Prior evidence has demonstrated that motor development is closely linked to language development, i.e. motor skills enable the child to interact with the environment and this interaction is required by the child to develop proper language skills(Reference Houwen, Visser and van der Putten43). Second, HFI can compromise parental well-being, including maternal mental health(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7,Reference Whitaker, Phillips and Orzol44Reference Reesor-Oyer, Cepni and Lee46) , and interferes with parent–child interactions and the emotional environment at home(Reference Gill, Koleilat and Whaley47,Reference Slopen, Fitzmaurice and Williams48) , which may lead to delays in ECD due to poor interaction between child and caregivers(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7,Reference Shankar, Chung and Frank8) . In summary, the nutrition- and psycho-emotional stress related to HFI may lead to a lack of responsive and stimulating care by caregivers(Reference Aboud, Yousafzai, Black, Laxminarayan and Walker37,Reference McClure, Cunningham and Bull49) , limiting early stimulation and learning opportunities needed for proper child development, including activities such as talking to children, telling stories, playing and well-supervised explorations of environments outside the home(Reference Britto and Ulkuer50).

Regarding the association between HFI with the risk of developmental delays, some limitations must be considered when interpreting our findings. First, we acknowledged that the limited sample size influenced the somewhat wide CI in the relationship between HFI and delayed ECD multivariable analysis. Du Prel et al. (2009)(Reference du Prel, Hommel and Röhrig51) emphasise three types of information provided by CI: (i) the direction of the effects; (ii) its strength and (iii) the presence of a statistically significant result. Through this lens, our findings are innovative and useful as they show the direction of the associations between HFI and risk of ECD delays in very young children in a middle-income country where data on ECD are scarce. Furthermore, our findings can be used for postulating clinical and policy hypotheses that can be tested through studies with larger sample sizes. Second, due to the cross-sectional nature of our data, no causal relationships between ECD outcomes and independent variable can be established. In our study, ECD outcomes were assessed at the individual level, while HFI was measured at the household level. Therefore, individuals in the same household may experience different levels of food insecurity. Previous literature has indicated that in homes with children, parents tend to protect them from food insecurity by ensuring they have food(Reference Chilton, Knowles and Bloom52Reference Hadley and Crooks54), yet young children may experience poor cognitive outcomes as a consequence of adults experiencing food insecurity(Reference Hernandez and Jacknowitz55). Our findings add to the literature that suggests that HFI is a stressor in children, even when HFI was reported by adults in the household. Further studies need to be conducted to better understand the direct and indirect effects of HFI through nutrition and psycho-emotional stress pathways on ECD outcomes(Reference Pérez-Escamilla and Vianna7).

When interpreting our findings, it is important to note that HFI is an important but not the only factor influencing the risk of developmental delays. One strength of our analyses is the inclusion of known confounders in the multivariable analysis. However, we acknowledge that additional confounders influencing the relationship between HFI and poor ECD were not assessed in our study, e.g. caregivers’ stress and mental health problems (maternal anxiety and depression), micronutrient deficiency and lower-quality home environment (lack of stimulating objects, books and play materials). Last, because we focused on children who received services through the Universal Health Care System (SUS) in Brazil, which targets families with low incomes, our study may have overestimated the prevalence of HFI and ECD risks. Nevertheless, our results add to the emerging evidence previously showing in other contexts that HFI is a risk factor for developmental delays among children under 2 years of age.

Supporting household food security during infancy and toddlerhood, a highly sensitive period for the development of synapses or neural networks(Reference Ismail, Fatemi and Johnston28), can help improve the chances that children will have the opportunity to reach their full development potential(Reference Pérez-Escamilla56). Hence, we call for food security interventions and policies targeting children and families to ensure that they routinely prioritise pregnancy and the early years of postnatal life. This recommendation is consistent with the UN’s Sustainable Development Goals, which include achieving food security and the full potential development of young children as an international priority for the twenty-first century(Reference Pérez-Escamilla56,57) . It is also consistent with recent evidence-based recommendations(Reference Black, Walker and Fernald29,Reference Britto, Lye and Proulx58,Reference Richter, Daelmans and Lombardi59) emphasising that effective interventions which integrate child’s health, development and well-being must be designed and implemented considering all the nurturing care dimensions. Promoting ECD under nurturing care includes supporting parents, caregivers and families that provide responsive and stimulating care to meet the needs for healthcare, nutrition/food security, education, social protection and child protection(Reference Black, Walker and Fernald29). Additionally, consistent with American Academy of Pediatrics recommendations, HFI and ECD surveillance and screening during visits to CHC may assist paediatricians and other healthcare professionals to identify children who are at risk of developmental delays and the lifelong implications they carry with them(Reference Barnidge, LaBarge and Krupsky6063).

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: We thank Ana Maria Spaniol and Amanda Souza Moura for assisting with coordinating data collection. Financial support: This study was funded by The National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq) (MCTI/CNPq/Universal 14/2014, n° 446269/2014-0). The Coordination of Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES) funded the doctoral research scholarship of Klébya Oliveira. CNPq and CAPES had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article. Conflict of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: K.H.D.O. and M.B.G. designed the study and prepared the first draft of the manuscript. K.H.D.O. coordinated data collection. K.H.D.O., G.B. and M.B.G. contributed substantially to the data analysis and K.H.D.O. conducted the analysis. M.B.G., G.B., R.P.E. and D.C.H. reviewed the manuscript critically for important intellectual content. All authors reviewed the draft versions and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki, and the study protocol was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Health Sciences of the University of Brasília and Health Sciences Teaching and Research Foundation (FEPECS) Ethics Committee (1.178.564). Participation in the study was voluntary, and written informed consent was obtained from all mothers for themselves and on behalf of their participating child.

References

McCoy, DC, Peet, ED, Ezzati, M et al. (2016) Early childhood developmental status in low- and middle-income countries: national, regional, and global prevalence estimates using predictive modeling. PLoS Med 13, e1002034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckman, JJ (2007) The economics, technology, and neuroscience of human capability formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 104, 1325013255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoddinott, J, Maluccio, J, Behrman, J et al. (2008) Effect of a nutrition intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults. Lancet 371, 411416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heckman, J, Pinto, R & Savelyev, P (2013) Understanding the mechanisms through which an influential early childhood program boosted adult outcomes. Am Econ Rev 103, 20522086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, C, Black, MM & Richter, LM (2016) Risk of poor development in young children in low-income and middle-income countries: an estimation and analysis at the global, regional, and country level. Lancet Glob Health 4, e916e922.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, AD & Markowitz, AJ (2018) Associations between household food insecurity in early childhood and children’s kindergarten skills. Child Dev 89, e1e17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Escamilla, R & Vianna, R (2012) Food insecurity and the behavioral and intellectual development of children: a review of the evidence. J Appl Res Child 3, 116.Google Scholar
Shankar, P, Chung, R & Frank, DA (2017) Association of food insecurity with children’s behavioral, emotional, and academic outcomes: a systematic review. J Dev Behav Pediatr 38, 135150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J & Garner, A (2011) The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 129, e232e246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, CA (2000) The neurobiological bases of early intervention. In Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention, 2nd ed., pp. 204227 [Shonkoff, JP & Marshall, PC, editors]. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanner, EM & Finn-Stevenson, M (2002) Nutrition and brain development: social policy implications. Am J Orthopsychiatry 72, 182193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y, Potochnick, S & Heflin, CM (2018) Household food insecurity and early childhood health and cognitive development among children of immigrants. J Fam Issues 39, 14651497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Food and Agriculture Organization (2013) The State of Food Insecurity in the World. Rome: Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations.Google Scholar
Food and Agriculture Organization et al. (2020) The State of Food Security and Nutrition in the World 2020. Transforming Food Systems for Affordable Healthy Diets. Rome, FAO: Food and Agriculture Organization.Google Scholar
Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística (2020) Consumer Expenditure Survey: 2017–2018: Analysis of Food Security in Brazil. Rio de Janeiro: Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística.Google Scholar
Gubert, MB, Spaniol, AM, Bortolini, GA et al. (2016) Household food insecurity, nutritional status and morbidity in Brazilian children. Public Health Nutr 19, 22402245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poblacion, A, Cook, JT, Marin-Leon, L et al. (2016) Food insecurity and the negative impact on Brazilian children’s health-why does food security matter for our future prosperity? Brazilian national survey (PNDS 2006/07). Food Nutr Bull 37, 585598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernal, J, Frongillo, EA & Jaffe, K (2016) Food insecurity of children and shame of others knowing they are without food. J Hunger Environ Nutr 11, 180194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ke, J & Ford-Jones, EL (2015) Food insecurity and hunger: a review of the effects on children’s health and behaviour. Paediatr Child Health 20, 8991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodríguez, LA, Mundo-Rosas, V, Méndez-Gómez-Humarán, I et al. (2017) Dietary quality and household food insecurity among Mexican children and adolescents. Matern Child Nutr 13, e12372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fram, MS, Ritchie, LD, Rosen, N et al. (2015) Child experience of food insecurity is associated with child diet and physical activity. J Nutr 145, 499504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Q, Harwood, RL & Feng, X (2018) Family socioeconomic status and maternal depressive symptoms: mediation through household food insecurity across 5 years. Soc Sci Med 215, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garg, A, Toy, S, Tripodis, Y et al. (2015) Influence of maternal depression on household food insecurity for low-income families. Acad Pediatr 15, 305310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, JT, Black, M, Chilton, M et al. (2013) Are food insecurity’s health impacts underestimated in the U.S. population? Marginal food security also predicts adverse health outcomes in young U.S. children and mothers. Adv Nutr 4, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, JT & Frank, DA (2008) Food security, poverty, and human development in the United States. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1136, 193209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Oliveira, KHD, de Almeida, GM, Gubert, MB et al. (2020) Household food insecurity and early childhood development: systematic review and meta-analysis. Matern Child Nutr 16, e12967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sania, A, Sudfeld, CR, Danaei, G et al. (2019) Early life risk factors of motor, cognitive and language development: a pooled analysis of studies from low/middle-income countries. BMJ open 9, e026449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ismail, FY, Fatemi, A & Johnston, MV (2017) Cerebral plasticity: windows of opportunity in the developing brain. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 21, 2348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, MM, Walker, SP, Fernald, LC et al. (2017) Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet 389, 7790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markham, JA & Greenough, WT (2004) Experience-driven brain plasticity: beyond the synapse. Neuron Glia Biology 1, 351363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, LCH, Prado, EL, Kariger, PK et al. (2017) A Toolkit for Measuring Early Childhood Development in Low and Middle Income Countries. Washington, D.C.: World Bank Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankenburg, WK, Dodds, J, Archer, P et al. (1992) The Denver II: a major revision and restandardization of the Denver developmental screening test. Pediatrics 89, 9197.Google Scholar
de Souza, SC, Leone, C, Takano, OA et al. (2008) Development of children enrolled in preschools in Cuiabá, Mato Grosso State, Brazil. Cad Saúde Pública 24, 19171926.Google Scholar
Frankenburg, WK & Dodds, JB (1990) Denver II Technical Manual. Denver: Denver Developmental Materials Inc.Google Scholar
Segall-Corrêa, AM, Marin-León, L, Melgar-Quiñonez, H et al. (2014) Refinement of the Brazilian household food insecurity measurement scale: recommendation for a 14-item EBIA. Rev Nutr 27, 241251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knickmeyer, RC, Gouttard, S, Kang, C et al. (2008) A structural MRI study of human brain development from birth to 2 years. J Neurosci 28, 1217612182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aboud, FE & Yousafzai, A (2016) Very early childhood development. In Disease Control Priorities. Reproductive, Maternal, Newborn, and Child Health, 3rd ed [Black, RE, Laxminarayan, R, Walker, N et al., editors]. Washington: World Bank.Google ScholarPubMed
Rose-Jacobs, R, Black, MM, Casey, PH et al. (2008) Household food insecurity: associations with at-risk infant and toddler development. Pediatrics 121, 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, MM, Quigg, AM, Cook, J et al. (2012) WIC participation and attenuation of stress-related child health risks of household food insecurity and caregiver depressive symptoms. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 166, 444451.Google ScholarPubMed
Drennen, CR, Coleman, SM, de Cuba, SE et al. (2019) Food insecurity, health, and development in children under age 4 years. Pediatrics 144, e20190824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saha, KK, Tofail, F, Frongillo, EA et al. (2010) Household food security is associated with early childhood language development: Results from a longitudinal study in rural Bangladesh. Child Care Health Dev 36, 309316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, EM, Fiorella, KJ, Mattah, BJ et al. (2018) Timing, intensity, and duration of house-hold food insecurity are associated with early childhood development in Kenya. Matern Child Nutr 14, e12543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houwen, S, Visser, L, van der Putten, A et al. (2016) The interrelationships between motor, cognitive, and language development in children with and without intellectual and developmental disabilities. Res Dev Disabil 53, 1931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitaker, RC, Phillips, SM & Orzol, SM (2006) Food insecurity and the risks of depression and anxiety in mothers and behavior problems in their preschool-aged children. Pediatrics 118, e859e868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, P, Goolsby, S, Berkowitz, C et al. (2004) Maternal depression, changing public assistance, food security, and child health status. Pediatrics 113, 298304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reesor-Oyer, LM, Cepni, AB, Lee, CY et al. (in press) Disentangling food insecurity and maternal depression: Which comes first? Public Health Nutr, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, M, Koleilat, M & Whaley, SE (2018) The impact of food insecurity on the home emotional environment among low-income mothers of young children. Matern Child Health J 22, 11461153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slopen, N, Fitzmaurice, G, Williams, DR et al. (2010) Poverty, food insecurity, and the behavior for childhood internalizing and externalizing disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 49, 444452.Google ScholarPubMed
McClure, C, Cunningham, M, Bull, S et al. (2018) Using mobile health to promote early language development: a narrative review. Acad Pediatr 18, 850854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Britto, PR & Ulkuer, N (2012) Child development in developing countries: child rights and policy implications. Child Dev 83, 92103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
du Prel, JB, Hommel, G, Röhrig, B et al. (2009) Confidence interval or P-value? Part 4 of a series on evaluation of scientific publications. Dtsch Arztebl Int 106, 335339.Google ScholarPubMed
Chilton, M, Knowles, M & Bloom, SL (2017) The intergenerational circumstances of household food insecurity and adversity. J Hunger Environ Nutr 12, 269297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Council on Community Pediatrics & Committee on Nutrition (2015) Promoting food security for all children. Pediatrics 136, e1431e1438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadley, C & Crooks, DL (2012) Coping and the biosocial consequences of food insecurity in the 21st century. Am J Phys Anthropol 149, 7294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez, DC & Jacknowitz, A (2009) Transient, but not persistent adult food insecurity influences toddler development. J Nutr 139, 15171524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Escamilla, R (2017) Food security and the 2015–2030 sustainable development goals: from human to planetary health: perspectives and opinions. Curr Dev Nutr 1, e000513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United Nations Children’s Fund (2018) Progress for Every Child in the SDG Era. New York: United Nations Children’s Fund.Google Scholar
Britto, PR, Lye, SJ, Proulx, K et al. (2017) Nurturing care: promoting early childhood development. Lancet 389, 91102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richter, LM, Daelmans, B, Lombardi, J et al. (2017) Investing in the foundation of sustainable development: pathways to scale up for early childhood development. Lancet 389, 103118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnidge, E, LaBarge, G, Krupsky, K et al. (2017) Screening for food insecurity in pediatric clinical settings: opportunities and barriers. J Community Health 42, 5157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, AF, Henize, AW, Kahn, RS et al. (2014) Forging a pediatric primary care-community partnership to support food-insecure families. Pediatrics 134, e564e571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Council on Children with Disabilities, Section on Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Steering Committee et al. (2006) Identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders in the medical home: an algorithm for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediatrics 118, 405420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Council on Community Pediatrics & Committee on Nutrition (2015) Promoting food security for all children. Pediatrics 136, e1431e1438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics and prevalence of risk of developmental delays of children under 2 years and their mothers by household–maternal–child characteristics (n 1004). Federal District, Brazil, 2018

Figure 1

Table 2 Unadjusted and adjusted OR and 95% CI for early childhood development outcomes according to household food insecurity status (n 1004). Federal District, Brazil, 2018†