Food choice is a complex human behaviour that is affected by many interdependent factors. It is neither fully determined by the physiological and nutritional needs of a person nor from the perceptions and interest of a person in the health effects of certain foods and diets. Food choice depends on a group of factors ranging from biological mechanisms and genetic predisposition at one end of the scale to social and cultural factors at the otherReference Shepherd1.
The organoleptic characteristics of a food such as smell, taste, appearance and texture can play an important role in food choice. Nevertheless, the existence of such characteristics does not necessarily drive the consumption of a food. The way a person perceives these characteristics is the final path that will lead to the food’s intakeReference Birch and Fisher2. In addition, there are psychological parameters, such as personality, that may intervene in this path. Besides internal or food-related factors, there are many more, external, factors that affect food choice. These factors include marketing and promotion techniques and price, as well as sociodemographic and cultural impacts on dietary habitsReference Shepherd1.
Food choice in Greek students has been reported in the past as following a Westernised model far from the Mediterranean diet. CruzReference Cruz3 reports that children in South Europe, including Greece, consume three or four meals per day, while a small percentage skips breakfast (not more than 15%). However, they consume large amounts of foods rich in sugar and fat, and added sugar provides more than 10% of daily energy intake. It was also observedReference Roma-Giannikou, Adamidis, Gianniou, Nikolara and Matsaniotis4 in Spain and Greece that dietary fat accounts for 40% of total energy intake, while saturated and monounsaturated fatty acids account for 13% and 17–19%, respectively, mainly due to the high consumption of olive oil. According to a more recent studyReference Yannakoulia, Karayiannis, Terzidou, Kokkevi and Sidossis5 in an adolescent population aged 11.5 years, 50% and 30% consume soft drinks and sweets/confectionery, respectively, on a daily basis. Girls have healthier food choices than boys, since they consume fruits and vegetables more often and foods rich in fat and sugar less frequently than boys. Consecutively, a 202% rise in the prevalence of obesity has been observed in 12-year-old Cretan boys from 1982 to 2002. During this 20-year period there has also been a significant increase in total cholesterol and low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, which are risk factors for cardiovascular diseaseReference Magkos, Manios, Christakis and Kafatos6.
Since food choice plays an important role in health and growth and because there are no published data regarding factors affecting food choice in Greece, the objective of the present study was to measure these factors in a large sample of Greek schoolchildren attending 5th and 6th grade.
Methods
Sampling procedure
The school was considered as the sampling unit. For the ELPYDES study, we randomly selected 100 primary schools and 4752 students from Attica and Thessaloniki regions, which are the two bigger regions of the country, stratified according to the geographical distribution of their population. In particular, schools and students included in the study sample were stratified as follows:
• 66 schools with 2982 students from Attica region;
• 34 schools with 1770 students from Thessaloniki region.
The students included in the sample lived mainly in urban areas (89.2% vs. 10.8% from rural areas) of the two regions. More specifically, 1808 (42.7%) students attended schools situated in the centre of Athens or Thessaloniki, 1520 (35.9%) students attended schools situated in the lower-class west suburbs of the two cities, and 909 (21.4%) lived in the middle- and upper-class east and south suburbs. Moreover, the sample consisted of 2313 students attending kindergarten through 4th grade, while 2439 were 5th and 6th grade students.
These schools were selected from a pool of school units where health education programmes were supported by the National Youth Institute of Greece, separated into two groups:
• 53 primary schools where programmes concerning ‘nutrition and dietary habits’ were underway or had been implemented in the past two years;
• 47 primary schools where programmes dealing with other issues were underway or had been implemented in the past two years.
Questionnaires
The tools of the ELPYDES study that were distributed to 5th and 6th grade students (11–13 years old) were self-administered questionnaires that evaluated: (1) nutritional knowledge; (2) self-efficacy and social support for dietary change; and (3) the frequency of intake of 15 food groups.
The time given to the subjects to respond was 40 minutes in total and advice from specialised nutritionists was given to either the teacher or the students, when required, prior to distribution of the questionnaires.
The Nutrition Knowledge Questionnaire included some demographic questions at the beginning regarding age, school and grade, sex, and nationality of parents and adults living with the student or caregivers. After that, there were 14 multiple-choice questions with two, three or four possible answers, of which only one was correct. The Nutrition Knowledge Questionnaire was based on a questionnaire used in the CATCH study (Child and Adolescent Trial for Cardiovascular Health) in the USAReference Perry, Baranowski, Parcel, Glansk, Lewis and Rimer7; it was translated into Greek and back-translated into English by two different groups of experts, as was the case with all of the questionnaires used in the ELPYDES study. All questionnaires were modified for regional nutritional particularities by another group of experts in our institution.
The Self-efficacy and Social Support for Dietary Change Questionnaire was based on a questionnaire used by Parcel et al.Reference Parcel, Edmundson, Perry, Feldman, O’Hara-Tompkins and Nader8, which consisted of a scale developed to evaluate self-efficacy of elementary-school students regarding nutrition issues (Child Dietary Self-Efficacy Scale; CDSS).
Using a similar methodology, we tried to evaluate our subjects’ intention to change. Our questionnaire included 20 questions that evaluated the perceived ability of students to choose certain products low in fat, sugar and salt instead of foods high in these ingredients and 10 questions that evaluated their perception on the support they would have when trying to make these choices. All of these questions started with the phrase ‘How sure are you that…’ and continued with the behaviour of the student or the perceived behaviour of a ‘significant other’ we were examining. The answers were given in a Likert-type scale that was numbered from 1 to 5, where 1 = not at all sure, 2 = a little sure, 3 = sure enough, 4 = very sure and 5 = extremely sure that the subject can do what the question asks or that the subject’s parents/friends will do what the question asks. Subjects were expected to circle the number that stated their level of confidence.
Questions focused mainly on consumption of fruits, vegetables, sweets/confectionery and carbonated soft drinks; factors that affect students’ dietary behaviour such as family, peers, television and marketing; organoleptic characteristics (smell, taste) of foods; and cost. As mentioned above, we took under consideration the regional nutritional particularities of the Greek sample and we performed repeatability tests using a test–retest procedure. In particular, we administered the questionnaires twice to a subgroup of our sample with an interval of 3 weeks. We then performed a Wilcoxon signed ranks test and observed that the P-value was larger than 0.5, concluding that the questionnaire was repeatable. The reliability of the questionnaire was also measured using Cronbach’s alpha statistic and found to be very satisfactory (α = 0.81).
We also used a simple food-frequency questionnaire (FFQ), which included 15 food groups corresponding to the food groups from the Mediterranean Diet Pyramid plus carbonated soft drinks. Thus, our FFQ included fresh fruits, vegetables, red meat, chicken, fish, milk/cheese/yoghurt, cereals/bread/pasta, potatoes, olive oil, other oils and fats, legumes, nuts, eggs, sweets and carbonated soft drinks; and seven frequency categories for responses, namely 4–6 times/day, 1–3 times/day, 5–6 times/week, 2–4 times/week, 1 time/week, 1–3 times/month and rarely or never. The reliability of the questionnaire was measured using Cronbach’s alpha statistic and found to be satisfactory (α = 0.64), while its repeatability was tested and verified at an earlier stage in a sub-sample of 50 elementary-school students from 5th and 6th grade. Although there is consensus among nutrition researchers that the FFQ is the least preferred method to assess diet, unless it is used in combination with another more accurate method like multiple 24-hour dietary recalls, this is true only as far as nutrient intake assessment is concerned. Use of the FFQ to evaluate food group preferences is considered a very adequate methodReference Willett9 and the FFQ is also used for the development of diet indices for the assessment of diet qualityReference Kant10. This was our main goal of this project and not evaluation of the nutrient intake of participating students.
Statistical analyses
For description of the results we used frequencies (and percentages) for categorical variables, and mean ± standard deviation or median (range) for continuous or ordinal variables, respectively. Normality tests were performed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov criterion. Tests for differences in mean values of continuous variables between two groups were performed with Student’s t-test (for normally distributed variables) or the Mann–Whitney U-test (for discrete or skewed variables). Tests for mean differences in continuous or ordinal variables between more than two groups were performed with analysis of variance or the Kruskal–Wallis test, respectively. Tests for differences in percentages between two or more groups were performed with the Z-statistic. In order to evaluate potential correlations between continuous or ordinal variables and to reduce the dimensions of the initial information, we performed a multivariate technique, principal components analysis (PCA). It is known that in order for the results from PCA to have some value, strong correlations between the variables should exist; the correlation matrix of the variables used in the present analysis showed that there were several correlation coefficients |r|>0.2, indicating that the variables were correlated with one another and, therefore, that PCA could be effective for assessing various patterns. The selection of the number of principal components was based on the percentage of total variance explained (i.e. >60%) and Cattell’s scree test. We performed PCA on the variables that evaluated the perceived ability of students to choose certain foods (PCA1) and the variables that evaluated their perception on the support they would have when trying to make these choices (PCA2). Finally, we performed a combined PCA (PCA3) on the remaining components after the application of PCA1 and PCA2. All statistical tests were performed at a level of statistical significance of 0.01. Analyses were conducted using STATA 8 software (StataCorp.) and the pca procedure was used to perform the analysis. Orthogonal rotation (rotate with varimax option) was used to derive optimal non-correlated components (i.e. patterns). The information was rotated in order to increase the representation of each food or food group to a component.
Results
We have included 2439 students from 5th and 6th grade. The mean age of the study sample was 11.3 ± 0.77 years; 85% of the children were living with both parents, while 10% lived with just one parent. Ninety per cent of the sample was living in urban areas and almost half of those lived in the city centre. Some 40% of the students had not attended a nutrition-related health education programme. Concerning frequency of food group intake, girls consumed significantly more cereals/bread/pasta (P = 0.007) and nuts (P = 0.004) than boys, while differences in the consumption of potatoes, legumes, eggs and carbonated soft drinks were of borderline significance (P < 0.05), with girls generally reporting more frequent intakes of these food groups than boys.
Principal components analysis
PCA1 extracted four components that explained 41% of the total variation in self-efficacy for dietary change. The scores for the four self-efficacy components are presented in Table 1 (coefficients with absolute value >0.2 are shown in bold). Since higher absolute value indicates that the self-efficacy variable contributes more to the development of the componentReference Mardia, Kent and Bibby11, the following components were derived: Component 1 – readiness to make the healthier choice when competitive foods are considered, which loaded equally on all situations regarding fruits and vegetables against other competitive foods; Component 2 – readiness to choose fresh foods instead of ready-to-eat, pre-packaged choices as a main meal, but not ready to avoid ice cream or chocolate as a dessert or snack; Component 3 – readiness to choose traditional foodstuffs instead of modern choices, excluding foods such as chips, doughnuts, ice creams and sugar-sweetened beverages; and Component 4 – preference for vegetables and sugar. Component 1 was the most dominant food choice factor and explained 25% of the total variance. Each of the remaining three components explained from 5% (Components 3 and 4) to 6% (Component 2) of the variance in self-efficacy.
PCA2 extracted five components that explained 63% of the total variation in social support for dietary change. The scores for the five social support components are presented in Table 2 (coefficients with absolute value >0.2 shown in bold). Since higher absolute value indicates that the social support variable contributes more to the development of the componentReference Mardia, Kent and Bibby11, the following components were derived: Component 1 – impact of parents, friends and advertisements on students’ choices and also impact of taste, smell and cooking method on the consumption of vegetables by this age group; Component 2 – initiative to buy fruits and awareness of the health value of fruits and vegetables, together with impact of parents’ advice on vegetable choice; Component 3 – effect of external factors on the consumption of fruits and sweets; Component 4 – impact of parents’ advice for fruits and preparation methods for vegetables on their consumption; and Component 5 – impact of parents’ eating behaviour and preparation methods on fruit consumption and of advertisements on consumption of sweets. Component 1 was the most dominant food choice factor and explained 18% of the total variance. Each of the remaining four components explained from 13% (Component 2) to 10% (Components 4 and 5) of the variance in social support.
PCA3 extracted six components that explained 66% of the total variation in factors affecting dietary change. The scores for the six factors components are presented in Table 3 (coefficients with absolute value >0.2 shown in bold). Since higher absolute value indicates that the factor variable contributes more to the development of the componentReference Mardia, Kent and Bibby11, the following components were derived: Component 1 – readiness to make the healthier choice when competitive foods are considered; Component 2 – impact of parents, friends and advertisements on students’ choices and also impact of taste, smell and cooking method on the consumption of vegetables by this age group; Component 3 – readiness to choose fresh foods instead of ready-to-eat, pre-packaged choices as a main meal and awareness of the health value of fruits and vegetables; Component 4 – tendency to consume fresh fruits and vegetables as a result of the impact parents have on students’ choices (vegetable choice was the only one affected by an external factor, namely parents’ advice); Component 5 – overall effect of social support in food choice; and Component 6 – impact of parents’ eating behaviour and preparation methods on fruit consumption and of advertisements on consumption of sweets. Component 1 was the most dominant food choice factor and explained 29% of the total variance. Each of the remaining five components explained from 11% (Component 2) to 6% (Components 4, 5 and 6) of the variance in factors affecting dietary change.
Discussion
Factors affecting the dietary behaviour of children and adolescents have been thoroughly examined and reviewed during the past decade. In our sample we have observed six principal components that drive food choice. Three of them accounted for 48% of the total variation in factors affecting food choice, namely: (1) readiness to make the healthier choice when competitive foods are considered; (2) impact of ‘significant others’ (parents, friends and advertisements) on students’ choices and also impact of taste, smell and cooking method on the consumption of vegetables; and (3) readiness to choose fresh foods instead of ready-to-eat, pre-packaged choices as a main meal and awareness of the health value of fruits and vegetables.
The first component showed a tendency of students to categorise foods into two groups: the ‘healthier’ fruits, vegetables and home-made snacks group and the competitive group where snacks rich in sugar, fat and salt were included. The notion of two competitive groups of foods has been widely used in nutrition policy primarily in the USA. The American Dietetic Association has recently published12 a new position statement renewing its commitment on Nutrition Integrity in Schools, where it refers explicitly to a level of performance that assures all foods and beverages available in schools are consistent with the Dietary Guidelines for Americans. Therefore, competitive foods in schools must reach a similar level of nutritional adequacy and quality as the one characterising ‘healthy choices’. Since the students in our sample tend to categorise their choices in two groups, following such an approach with competitive foods could be most successful.
The second component deals with external factors determining choice of fruits, vegetables, school snacks and confectionery. More specifically, it expresses the impact of parents, friends and advertisements on students’ choices and the impact of taste and smell on the consumption of vegetables by this age group. Parents play a very important role in shaping children’s eating behaviour from an early age. The control they have over the quality and quantity of foods served to their offspring can modulate children’s choice of food at a later age. For example, it is very common on the parents’ side to limit the availability of sweets, such as chocolate and candies, at home. However, this confinement works in reverse and makes these foods, which are competitive to fruits and vegetables, more appealing to childrenReference Hill13. Thus they are more likely to be consumed on occasions when parents are not present. This is true for our sample too, since students reported that they were not ready to avoid ice cream or chocolate as a dessert or snack.
In addition, using foods as a reward or punishment is a tactic usually adopted by parents when trying to persuade their children to behave in a certain way. Whether it is to eat their meal or study their lessons or go to bed early, this tactic always drives a categorisation of foods as ‘good’ or ‘bad’, where ‘bad’ foods are consumed only on pleasant occasions (such as a party) or as a reward after eating a ‘good’ food (e.g. ‘if you eat your legumes you can then have the ice cream you are asking for’). This habit creates confusion in the messages children receive, since ‘good’ foods such as vegetables are only chosen obligatorily, because the parents say so, and thus play the role of a punishment to disobedient childrenReference Birch14. Again, ice cream and chocolate are typical rewards to children and this is why they report not being able to avoid them or choose an alternative nutritious choice.
Nevertheless, parents can play an important role in guiding their children’s food choices by acting as a role model for balanced nutrition through their beliefs and daily habits. Availability of fruits and vegetables at home is a primary target through which they can affect their children’s choicesReference Birch and Fisher2 and our observations support these results since parents’ impact on students’ consumption of fruits was mainly driven by availability of freshly cut fruits at home. According to Social Cognitive Theory, ‘significant others’ constitute an integral part of the influence of the external environment, through learning by observation and role modelling. Brown and OgdenReference Brown and Ogden15 found a strong positive relationship between parents’ and primary-school students’ consumption of healthy and competitive snacks.
However, the magnitude of the effect of parents and family diminishes during adolescenceReference Cheung and Richmond16. Our sample of preadolescent students was characterised by a balanced effect between parents and peers, where parents affected food choice at home and peers affected it at recess. Patterson et al.Reference Patterson, Rupp, Sallis, Atkins and Nader17 found that the correlation coefficient for nutrient intakes between parents and children was smaller at the age of 13 years than at age 11. After parents, friends and peers are the main modulators of eating habits. Peers seem to overwhelm the effect of teachers or other adults mainly in girlsReference Hendy and Raudenbush18. Hendy reported that specially trained peers were successful in acting as role models for the acceptance of new foods, such as vegetables, in their schoolmates’ dietsReference Hendy19. These results, however, did not stand for more than one month and concerned only school meals. Thus, although using peer pressure could be a successful policy to change dietary habits in school, there is another factor affecting adolescents’ food choices outside the school environment that needs to be addressed.
The single strongest factor affecting children’s food choice on the societal level is television advertising. As we observed too, students stated that they would consume less sweets and confectionery ‘if relevant TV advertisements wouldn’t be broadcast often’. Children watch television from a very young age, resulting in them receiving messages regarding nutrition mainly from advertisements. These messages concern the acceptance and consumption of certain foods, as well as the adoption of a certain dietary behaviour. Most advertised foods are rich in fat and sugar and poor in fibreReference Kotz and Story20. These foods constitute the main food choices of children and create a dietary pattern that is very difficult to change later in lifeReference Birch and Fisher2. In a sample of 4211 students in Greece, Yannakoulia et al.Reference Yannakoulia, Karayiannis, Terzidou, Kokkevi and Sidossis5 reported that television viewing was associated with the consumption of selected food items. In particular, the more hours spent in front of the television, the higher the percentage of students reporting to consume sodas (from 28.6 to 68.6%), crisps (from 15.7 to 43.4%), cakes and pastries (from 8.6 to 25.9%) and sweets and chocolates (from 31.5 to 52.5%) at least once a day, whereas the less frequent the consumption of fruits (from 92.8 to 76.3%).
Besides the impact parents, friends and television advertising have in students’ food choices, there is always the element of taste that forms the acceptance of certain foods and mainly vegetables, as was the case in our sample too. According to youngsters, they consume a food because they believe it is tasteful, appealing and convenientReference Neumark-Sztainer, Story, Perry and Casey21. In addition, they do not believe that the ‘healthy’ nature of a food is relevant to their needs, while they confess that the healthfulness of a food is not promoted among their peers or the societies in which they live and eat. Therefore, the fact that the second most prominent component, explaining 11% of the variability in our sample, included the aforementioned factors coincides with data from similar populations in other developed countries, besides Greece. Moreover, the cumulative percentage of 40% of the total variability being explained by the first two components, where the notion of competitive foods is added to external factors and taste, shows that the main public health goals, when trying to improve students’ dietary habits, must regard both family and friends, regulate television advertising and try to present all foods as appealing and tasteful as the ones rich in fat and sugar.
The third component incorporates the notion of freshness, which is typical and unique of the Greek culture. This is the reason why there are no such references in other studies investigating food choice. Owing to the tradition of the Mediterranean diet, which is promoted in schools through health education programmes and taught curriculum, Greek students and their parents have a tendency to regard anything fresh and traditional as being the healthier choice. Thus the third most important component, explaining 8% of the total variability, practically discriminated foods in two categories according to their place in the Mediterranean Diet Pyramid. As far as the remaining components are concerned, they expressed the interdependence of factors already mentioned herein, which can work in different ways and combinations, since we have not yet fully understood and described a mechanism for food choice.
An important limitation of our study is that we did not manage to collect data from all regions of Greece, where we would probably be able to discern factors affecting food choice in semi-urban and rural areas. However, Athens and Thessalloniki are the two largest cities in Greece and along with their suburbs they host 60–65% of the total population of the country. Moreover, the schools included in the study sample were schools where health education programmes have been undertaken, in order to obtain the permission and support of the head teachers to perform the study. However, the stratification of schools among the geographic regions of Attica and Thessaloniki was representative of the population’s sociodemographic characteristics.
The questionnaires regarding self-efficacy and social support were distributed only to 5th and 6th grade students, because the pre-testing showed that they would be difficult for younger students to fill in. Another limitation is that we did not use other tools for dietary evaluation (such as 24-hour recall) in addition to the FFQ; however, we believe that this fact did not influence our findings regarding food group choices of the students.
Conclusion
In conclusion, food choice in a large sample of Greek primary-school children is affected mainly by parents, peers, television advertising, and the taste and appearance of dishes. Therefore nutrition education and health promotion programmes in Greece must include family members and activities regarding food preparation, besides education and environmental changes in schools. Moreover, regulation of advertising is an important public health measure that needs to be discussed in Greece, as it is already being discussed in the European Union and the USA. Finally, Greek students have learnt to categorise foods as ‘healthier (more traditional and fresh) but less tasty’ and ‘appealing but unhealthy’. This is a notion that needs to be challenged in the future, since balanced nutrition is founded in variety and norm.
Acknowledgements
This paper was supported by the Iraklitos Program ‘Research Scholarships in Harokopio University’ co-funded by the European Union and the Greek Ministry of Education. The authors would like to acknowledge the contribution of Ms Konstantina Karasouli and Ms Stavroula Chrysanthopoulou in the data collection and data handling processes, respectively.