The high energy intake from energy-dense foods among children in developed countries( Reference Bell, Kremer and Magarey 1 – Reference Piernas and Popkin 3 ) is undesirable as such foods tend to be of little nutritional value and are superfluous to a healthy diet( 4 ). Several studies suggest that snacking has increased during the last few decades, thereby contributing to excessive energy intakes( Reference Adair and Popkin 5 – Reference Wang, Zhai and Zhang 8 ). It is important to invert this trend and establish healthy dietary patterns in childhood since children’s dietary behaviours( Reference Craigie, Lake and Kelly 9 ) and overweight( Reference Singh, Mulder and Twisk 10 ) track into adulthood. Parents can have a significant impact on their children’s food consumption through their food-specific parenting practices( Reference Gerards and Kremers 11 ) (i.e. behaviours used to shape a child’s diet, such as restricting children’s access to snack foods or modelling healthy food intake( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 12 )). In an earlier publication, based on a cluster analytic approach, we demonstrated that four categories of Dutch parents could be distinguished when it comes to food parenting practices (FPP) around snacking( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 13 ) (see Table 1). Children whose parents employed the ‘high involvement and supportive’ pattern had lower energy-dense snack food intakes compared with children whose parents employed any of the other three patterns (i.e. ‘low involvement and indulgent’, ‘high covert control and rewarding’ and ‘low covert control and non-rewarding’). Involved and supportive parents are healthy role models, create a supportive home food environment and set clear boundaries regarding snacking. Parents exerting covert control try to limit unhealthy food intakes in ways that are not visible for their children, such as by confining the availability of unhealthy foods in their homes. Instrumental and emotional feeding (referred to as ‘rewarding’ in Table 1) imply that children receive food in exchange for good behaviour or to regulate their emotions.
Intervention developers aiming to promote the use of desirable FPP should know their determinants and, subsequently, fit theoretical methods and practical applications in order to evoke maximal behavioural change( Reference Bartholomew Eldredge, Markham and Ruiter 14 ). Therefore, research on the precursors of parenting practices is essential. So far, research has mainly linked demographic factors to FPP. Another potential precursor includes the general parenting style of parents: evidence shows that parenting practices, although they are domain specific (e.g. food or physical activity), find their origin in general parenting( Reference Collins, Duncanson and Burrows 15 ). Personality serves as a guiding principle of a person’s behaviour( Reference Ajzen 16 , Reference Flay and Petraitis 17 ). Consequently, personality might be reflected in general parenting( Reference Belsky 18 ) and also relate to parenting practices. Probably, the use of parenting practices also traces back to childhood experiences and is linked with practices used by one’s own parents (i.e. the practices imposed during the parent’s own childhood). This hypothesis is supported by qualitative research( Reference Mena, Gorman and Dickin 19 ) and parallels Belsky’s theory on determinants of general parenting( Reference Belsky 18 ). Besides the more distal factors such as general parenting and personality, other potential, more proximal, precursors of parenting practices include the ones specified by socio-cognitive theories of health behaviour. For instance, the Social Cognitive Theory( Reference Bandura 20 ) stresses the importance of outcome expectations as well as modelling in shaping behaviour. Parents might favour certain practices by considering their expected effectiveness (i.e. outcome expectations) or by considering practices used by other parents (i.e. vicarious learning or modelling). Self-efficacy is another core concept of the Social Cognitive Theory and should be considered here as well, in view of evidence indicating that parents of obese children had less confidence in managing children’s energy balance-related behaviours( Reference Morawska and West 21 ). In addition to modelling, another form of social influence, subjective norm( Reference Fishbein and Ajzen 22 ), might be important too. This reflects the degree to which parents find that most people, who are important to them, think they should employ particular parenting practices. Finally, parents’ attitude towards the child snacking more in general is expected to determine the type of practices used. Two sets of determinants formed our research framework: predisposing factors (e.g. general parenting, personality) and parental cognitions (e.g. self-efficacy, subjective norms; see Fig. 1). The present paper reports on the relative importance of both sets of determinants in explaining a parent’s pattern of FPP and describes the contribution of each single factor to this exploratory model.
Methods
Study design, setting and participants
The full study design has been described elsewhere( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 13 ). In sum, 1985 Dutch parents of 4- to 12-year-old children were recruited nationwide by a research agency (i.e. Flycatcher Internet Research) and invited to fill out an online questionnaire. Participants received credit points for participation that could be exchanged for gifts: completion of the current study yielded approximately 5·56 Euros. Considering the ‘Dutch Medical Research Involving Human Subjects Act’( 23 ), the study was exempt from ethical review.
Outcome variable
The outcome variable was the type of FPP pattern. These patterns were derived from a two-step cluster analysis (i.e. hierarchical cluster analysis followed by a non-hierarchical cluster analysis) on twenty-one FPP around snacking( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 13 ), including the following constructs: encouragement, rewarding, discussing, providing feedback, involving, educating, healthy modelling, unhealthy modelling avoidance, availability of healthy foods, accessibility of healthy foods, visibility of healthy foods, limited availability of unhealthy foods, limited accessibility of unhealthy foods, structure, meal routines, permissiveness, rules, monitoring, instrumental feeding, emotional feeding and pressure to eat. Four distinct patterns were derived (see Table 1), which were stable according to a cross-validation procedure (i.e. Cohen’s κ of 0·97) and characterized as indicated in Table 1( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 13 ).
Measures
Predisposing factors
Background variables
Respondents were asked to report their own age, gender, work status, postal code (to calculate a factor score for their socio-economic position (SEP); −4=low; 4=high( 24 )), body height and weight (to calculate parental BMI), and their child’s age, gender, and body height and weight (to calculate the child’s BMI Z-score using a reference population( Reference Fredriks, van Buuren and Wit 25 )). The respondents’ educational level and ethnicity were known to the research agency.
Parent’s personality
For measuring the personality dimensions of extraversion, conscientiousness, agreeableness, emotional stability and openness to experience, the Dutch thirty-item ‘Quick Big Five’ (QBF) questionnaire( Reference Gerris, Houtmans and Kwaaitaal-Roosen 26 ) was used. Parents were asked to rate personality traits on a 7-point Likert scale. Cronbach’s α values of the five personality dimensions within the current sample were all above 0·84.
General parenting
General parenting was assessed using thirty-two items from the validated Comprehensive Parenting General Parenting Questionnaire (CGPQ)( Reference Sleddens, O’Connor and Watson 27 ) on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (= ‘strongly disagree’) to 5 (= ‘strongly agree’). Five general parenting constructs were measured, including nurturance, structure, behavioural control, coercive control and overprotection. Each of these five general parenting constructs consisted of two to four sub-constructs, of which ‘physical punishment’ (a sub-construct of coercive control) was not included in the current study. One item within structure (i.e. ‘I teach my child to keep his/her bedroom clean and orderly’) and two items within behavioural control (i.e. ‘I make sure I give my child lots of freedom to make mistakes and learn from them’ and ‘I give my child a lot of freedom to make up his/her own mind’) were dropped because of low corrected item–total correlations. The Cronbach’s α values of the five general parenting factors ranged from 0·59 to 0·76.
Food parenting practices used by own parents
As an indicator of ‘practices used by own parents during childhood’, participants were asked to indicate to what extent their own parents used to employ the twenty-one FPP that were used to cluster parents into the four groups. The items were formulated as follows: ‘My own parents (caregivers) used to … when I was a child’ and had to be answered on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (=‘strongly disagree’) to 5 (= ‘strongly agree’), with the possibility to answer ‘I don’t know’. Exploratory factor analyses (i.e. principal component analysis (PCA)) using oblique rotation were performed to derive factors and create scales using mean scores from the items because they were expected to be multidimensional. ‘I don’t know’ answers were recoded to the middle value (= 3). Eigenvalue >1·0 was used as a criterion to retain factors. Items were dropped for PCA if factor loadings were <0·40 or showed cross-loadings. PCA resulted in five factors referring to parenting practices used by own parents, with moderate to good Cronbach’s α except for one (i.e. the scale ‘pressure to eat and rewarding’). Example items, reliability statistics and factor loadings of the scales used to predict parenting practices are presented in Table 2.
CITC, corrected item–total correlation; QBF, ‘Quick Big Five questionnaire’; CGPQ, Comprehensive Parenting General Parenting Questionnaire; FPP, food parenting practice; EDSF, energy-dense snack food; NA, not assessed/not applicable.
† 1=‘not at all applicable to me’; 7=‘completely applicable to me’.
‡ 1=‘strongly disagree’; 5=‘strongly agree’.
§ Scale scores reverse coded; higher scores indicate a higher self-efficacy/positive outcome expectations (i.e. lower child snack food intake).
║ 1=‘eat far less snack foods’; 5=‘eat a lot more snack foods’.
¶ Scale not included in further analyses because of a negative average covariance among items.
Parental cognitions
Perceived behaviour of other parents and subjective norms
Items used to measure social influences (i.e. perceived behaviour of other parents, subjective norms) also referred to the twenty-one individual FPP around snacking and entered a PCA in a similar fashion. ‘Perceived behaviour of other parents’ was assessed using the question: ‘Most parents …’ followed by the twenty-one individual FPP. ‘Subjective norms’ were assessed using items starting with: ‘Most people, who are important to me, think I should …’. All questions were accompanied with a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (= ‘strongly disagree’) to 5 (=‘strongly agree’) and a possibility to answer ‘I don’t know’.
Attitude
Qualitative interviews with fifteen parents from the target population informed the development of the attitude items. Two sets of five items were formulated to assess parents’ attitude towards benefits (Cronbach’s α=0·80) and negative effects (Cronbach’s α=0·63) of a high snack intake by their child. Attitude items were rated on a 5-point Likert-scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’ and loaded on two factors, thereby confirming the attitude dichotomy.
Self-efficacy and outcome expectations
In line with items used to measures social influences (i.e. perceived behaviour of other parents, subjective norms), self-efficacy and outcome expectations items referred to the twenty-one FPP and entered the PCA. Self-efficacy items started with the following phrase: ‘I think it is difficult to …’ and were also rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. Because self-efficacy tends to be of minor importance with regard to instrumental feeding and emotional feeding, we decided not to measure self-efficacy for both constructs. Outcome expectations were assessed using the statement: ‘If I …, my child will’ and a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (= ‘eat far less snack foods’) to 5 (= ‘eat a lot more snack foods’). Four self-efficacy factors (Cronbach’s α ranging from 0·50 to 0·81) and five outcome expectation factors (Cronbach’s α ranging from 0·58 to 0·71) were derived. Self-efficacy and outcome expectations were reverse recoded so that higher scores indicate a higher self-efficacy and positive outcome expectations (i.e. lower child snack food intake), respectively.
Statistical analysis
To diagnose multicollinearity, correlations between predictor variables were explored and variance inflation factors were calculated through a linear regression analysis with all predictors as independent variables. A cut-off value of >0·8 for correlations( Reference Field 28 ) and >10 for variance inflation factors( Reference Myers 29 ) was applied for multicollinearity. To predict the patterns of FPP, multinomial logistic regression analyses were run using the ‘high involvement and supportive’ pattern as the reference category. Each predictor was tested individually and in a total model by using backward elimination. Subsequently, significant predictors in the final model were entered into two separate multinomial logistic regression analyses (i.e. one including predisposing factors and one including parental cognitions) to investigate the importance of both sets of predictors by assessing Nagelkerke R 2 values. All analyses were conducted using the statistical software package IBM SPSS Statistics version 20. P values <0·05 were considered statistically significant.
Results
Sample
The sample consisted of 888 parents with a mean age of 40·6 (sd 5·8) years. Of the sample, 65·2 % were female; 45·3 % had an intermediate and 43·6 % had a high educational level. Most parents were of Dutch ethnicity (91·0 %) and were employed (79·3 %). Their mean BMI was 25·2 (sd 4·2) kg/m2 and the mean SEP score was 0·1 (sd 1·2). Regarding BMI category, 54·2 % had a healthy weight (i.e. 18·5–24·9 kg/m2) and 45·2 % were overweight (i.e. ≥25·0 kg/m2). The mean age of the children was 7·9 (sd 2·6) years with half of them being female (49·7 %). Their mean BMI Z-score was 0·19 (sd 1·4), with 73·2 % being healthy weight and 14·6 % overweight according to Barlow’s cut-off points( Reference Barlow 30 ).
Predictors of patterns of food parenting practices
Multicollinearities between the scales were not found. Results from the multinomial logistic regression analysis showed that after backward elimination, nineteen factors had a statistically significant contribution to the final model (Nagelkerke R 2=0·63; Table 3).
SEP, socio-economic position; FPP, food parenting practice; EDSF, energy-dense snack food; NA, not assessed/not applicable.
*P<0·05; **P<0·01; ***P<0·001.
† 1=‘male’; 2=‘female’.
‡ 1=‘Dutch’; 2=‘non-Dutch’.
§ 1=‘low’; 2=‘intermediate’; 3=‘high’.
║ 1=‘employed’; 2=‘non-employed’.
All parental cognitions remained in the model, except for the factors measuring perceived behaviour of other parents, one out of four factors measuring subjective norms, and two out of five factors measuring outcome expectations. Overall, self-efficacy and outcome expectations were among the strongest predictors: parents were more likely to be highly involved and supportive in food parenting, rather than employ any other pattern, if they had a high self-efficacy and more positive outcome expectations regarding the ‘availability of healthy food and encouragement’ and more positive outcome expectations regarding ‘meal practices’.
Concerning the predisposing factors, in particular nurturance most strongly predicted food parenting clusters. Parents scoring high on nurturance were less likely to have the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ and the ‘high covert control and rewarding’ pattern, rather than having the ‘high involvement and supportive’ pattern. From the background variables, child age and SEP remained significant explanatory factors of cluster membership in the final model: higher-SEP parents were more likely to have the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ pattern, rather than the opposite one (i.e. ‘high involvement and supportive’). Parents of younger children were more likely to covertly control unhealthy foods and reward, rather than being highly involved and supportive, and parents of older children were more likely to have the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ pattern, rather than being highly involved and supportive. Parents’ current use of FPP also appeared to be related to the ones used by their own parents. From those factors, the strongest predictor was the former use of covert unhealthy food control: if their own parents highly used covert unhealthy food control, parents were more likely to be highly involved and supportive, rather than being member of the ‘low covert control and non-rewarding’ cluster. The two separate models, one including predisposing factors and one including parental cognitions from the final model, yielded Nagelkerke R 2 values of 0·37 and 0·55, respectively.
Discussion
In a previous study, we identified four different categories of Dutch parents with patterns of FPP( Reference Gevers, Kremers and de Vries 13 ). The current study aimed to explain membership of these patterns and found that parental cognitions were generally stronger predictors than predisposing factors, which included, among others, personality and general parenting. The greatest effect sizes were found among comparisons between the ‘high involvement and supportive’ cluster and its opposite, the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ cluster. Self-efficacy and outcome expectations regarding ‘healthy food availability and encouragement’ were the two factors that best discriminated highly involved and supportive parents from all three other types of parents. Parents were more likely to be highly involved and supportive if they had a high self-efficacy and more positive outcome expectations regarding making healthy foods available, accessible and visible, and regarding encouraging and modelling healthy food intake. These results contribute to the expanding research focus( Reference Bohman, Nyberg and Sundblom 31 – Reference Jago, Wood and Zahra 35 ) on the role of self-efficacy in parents’ energy balance-related parenting practices. Consequently, the increased attention of interventions to address self-efficacy seems to be justified( Reference Nyberg, Sundblom and Norman 36 , Reference Ruiter, Fransen and Molleman 37 ). In addition, the results demonstrate that parents might decide not to use some parenting practices because they believe they are not effective in lowering children’s energy-dense snack food intake. In contrast to subjective norms, perceived behaviour of other parents was not predictive for parents’ use of parenting practices, probably because of the moderate-sized intercorrelations between these factors. It has previously been highlighted that parents misperceive other parents’ acceptance of children’s frequent snacking( Reference Lally, Cooke and McGowan 38 ), suggesting that parents’ subjective norms regarding the use of parenting practices around snacking might be a misperception of the actual norm too. This hypothesis might be addressed in future research. Only a few studies have used theories of health behaviour to predict the use of FPP. For instance, Theory of Planned Behaviour constructs (i.e. attitudes, social norms, perceived behavioural control and intention) and outcome expectations were significant predictors in one study, which specifically focused on parents’ tracking behaviour of snack food intake( Reference Andrews, Silk and Eneli 39 ). Another line of research focused on predicting the use of vegetable parenting practices using a model that included, among others, Theory of Planned Behaviour and Self Determination Theory constructs( Reference Baranowski, Beltran and Chen 40 ). Habits most strongly predicted effective and ineffective parenting practices related to vegetable intake( Reference Baranowski, Beltran and Chen 41 , Reference Diep, Beltran and Chen 42 ). Since the target populations and type of FPP were different across these studies and the current research, the results cannot easily be merged to indicate the most important predictors.
Besides parental cognitions, the general parenting factor nurturance was found to be a significant and strong predictor: parents were more likely to be highly involved and supportive if they demonstrated high levels of nurturance. It was also the only general parenting factor that was a significant predictor in the full model and had a very large effect size in the univariate model. Nurturance mainly represents the extent to which parents are responsive to their children’s needs, are involved with and spend time with their child, and is also referred to as ‘responsiveness’ or ‘involvement’( Reference Gerards and Kremers 11 , Reference Sleddens, O’Connor and Watson 27 ). As such, nurturance may transfer from general parenting to involvement and supportiveness in food parenting.
The finding that parents of older children were less likely to covertly control and reward than those of younger children is consistent with earlier studies reporting on negative associations between child’s age and instrumental and emotional feeding( Reference Raaijmakers, Gevers and Teuscher 43 , Reference Hendy and Williams 44 ), but different from a study finding no link between age and covert snack control( Reference Brown, Ogden and Vögele 45 ). We found older parents to be more likely employing the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ pattern, which is reasonable given that parental involvement and monitoring tend to decline from childhood to adolescence( Reference Melbye, Ogaard and Overby 46 ). Lower-SEP parents were more likely to be highly involved and supportive, rather than being in the ‘low involvement and indulgent’ cluster, which is promising. Regarding the remaining predisposing factors, personality was of minor importance in predicting FPP, but part of the practices used by own parents did have a link with the food parenting patterns. More research is necessary to establish the significance of how parents were parented themselves in determining current parenting practices around energy balance-related behaviours.
Implications for research and practice
Based on the current study, planners of health promotion programmes aiming to reduce children’s snack intake should focus on parental cognitions towards food parenting. Most importantly, self-efficacy and outcome expectations should be targeted. Although parental cognitions tend to be proximal to food parenting, interventions might be targeted at more fundamental and distal factors such as nurturance as well. Although we cannot draw conclusions on the mediation of general parenting via cognitions to parenting practices, Jago and colleagues recently found evidence for such a pathway( Reference Jago, Wood and Zahra 35 ). Consequently, more sustainable change in parenting practices might be expected if change is achieved in general parenting( Reference Kitzmann and Beech 47 ). Non-modifiable predictors including the child’s age, parent’s age and practices used by parents’ own parents should be taken into account when developing interventions.
Strengths and limitations
A strength of the current study included the use of a broad set of potentially important factors. To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first using a combination of contextual and intrapersonal factors in the prediction of food-related parenting practices. Parents were asked to reflect on their own parents’ behaviours as well, which is an understudied perspective in the literature. It should be noted, however, that the self-report measures might have biased the results. There was one predictor that showed a different relationship in the univariate (i.e. OR <1) compared with the backward procedure (i.e. OR >1). Possibly, the low internal consistency of the scale concerned (i.e. ‘self-efficacy towards permissiveness and pressure’) caused this inconsistent result. The low internal consistency of five other scales, with Cronbach’s α ranging from 0·50 to 0·59, needs to be considered as well. Finally, the study was cross-sectional, implying that no causal inferences could be drawn. More specifically, parental cognitions such as self-efficacy or outcome expectations might have been constructed ad hoc because none existed or were aligned with (current) behaviour by parents, resulting in overestimated associations between cognitions and parenting practices( Reference Armitage and Conner 48 ). In addition, we cannot draw conclusions on the causal direction between general parenting and FPP.
Conclusion
The present study showed that parental cognitions are important factors in explaining the use of FPP around snacking. It also found that the general parenting factor nurturance is a strong predictor variable to particularly distinguish highly involved parents from parents employing a pattern of low involvement. These results suggest that intervention developers should take measures aimed at increasing self-efficacy and educating parents about what constitute effective and ineffective parenting practices. Potentially, focusing on general parenting factors such as nurturance is necessary.
Acknowledgements
Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank all parents who participated in this study and are grateful to Ester Sleddens for her contribution to this study. Financial support: This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflict of interest: None. Authorship: D.W.M.G., S.P.J.K., N.K.d.V. and P.v.A. jointly designed the study. D.W.M.G. conducted the statistical analyses, made the initial interpretation of the results and wrote the draft version of the manuscript. S.P.J.K., N.K.d.V. and P.v.A. contributed to the interpretation of the results and revising the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. Ethics of human subject participation: The study was exempt from ethical review.