Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:17:36.027Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Breakfast skipping and cognitive and emotional engagement at school: a cross-sectional population-level study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2021

Hero Moller
Affiliation:
Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia, Level 7, 31 Flinders St., Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia
Alanna Sincovich*
Affiliation:
Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia, Level 7, 31 Flinders St., Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia School of Public Health, University of Adelaide, Level 5, Rundle Mall Plaza, Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia
Tess Gregory
Affiliation:
Telethon Kids Institute, University of Western Australia, Level 7, 31 Flinders St., Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia School of Public Health, University of Adelaide, Level 5, Rundle Mall Plaza, Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia
Lisa Smithers
Affiliation:
School of Public Health, University of Adelaide, Level 5, Rundle Mall Plaza, Adelaide, South Australia5000, Australia School of Health and Society, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia Robinson Research Institute, Norwich Centre, North Adelaide, Australia
*
*Corresponding author: Email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Research on the consequences of breakfast skipping among students tends to focus on academic outcomes, rather than student well-being or engagement at school. This study investigated the association between breakfast skipping and cognitive and emotional aspects of school engagement.

Design:

Cross-sectional study using data from a population-level survey of children and adolescents’ well-being and engagement at school. Linear regression with adjustment for confounders was used to estimate the effect of breakfast skipping on school engagement.

Setting:

Government schools (i.e. public schools) in South Australia.

Participants:

The participants were students, Grades 4–12, who completed the Wellbeing and Engagement Collection in 2019. The analysis sample included 61 825 students.

Results:

Approximately 9·6 % of students reported always skipping breakfast, with 35·4 % sometimes skipping and 55·0 % never skipping. In the adjusted linear regression models, children and adolescents who always skipped breakfast reported lower levels of cognitive engagement (β = −0·26 (95 % CI −0·29, −0·25)), engagement with teachers (β = −0·17 (95 % CI −0·18, −0·15)) and school climate (β = −0·17 (95 % CI −0·19, −0·15)) compared with those who never skipped breakfast, after controlling for age, gender, health, sleep, sadness and worries, parental education, socio-economic status and geographical remoteness.

Conclusion:

Consistent with our hypothesis, skipping breakfast was associated with lower cognitive and emotional engagement, which could be due to mechanisms such as short-term energy supply and long-term health impacts. Therefore, decreasing the prevalence of breakfast skipping could have a positive impact on school engagement.

Type
Research Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

A healthy breakfast is an important part of the diets of children and adolescents. Healthy breakfasts are typically high in nutrients like calcium and fibre, and going without breakfast can become a missed opportunity for the necessary energy and nutrients for growth and healthy development(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard1). Studies show that breakfast skipping is often clustered with other unhealthy behaviours, such as increased intake of discretionary food and low physical activity(Reference Deshmukh-Taskar, Nicklas and O’Neil2Reference Utter, Scragg and Mhurchu4). There is also evidence that skipping breakfast can lead to poorer academic outcomes, making research exploring the relationship between breakfast and school outcomes of particular interest to a range of stakeholders, including government departments, educators and public health researchers(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard1,Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5) .

An international systematic review of breakfast habits among children found that the prevalence of breakfast skipping ranged from 12 to 34 %(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard1). In this review, breakfast skipping was more prevalent among females, older children and children from lower socio-economic backgrounds. The prevalence of breakfast skipping was also higher among adolescents who reported smoking, had low physical activity, dieted and had body weight concerns. The most common reasons reported for skipping breakfast were lack of time, no appetite or dieting to lose weight. Further, skipping breakfast for some children may be influenced by the family level and community factors, including food insecurity and family structure(Reference Jose, Vandenberg and Williams6Reference Jørgensen, Pedersen and Meilstrup9). In Australia, the setting of this present study, the prevalence of breakfast skipping also tends to be higher among females and older children and adolescents(Reference Fayet-Moore, Kim and Sritharan10). The 2011–2012 National Nutrition and Physical Activity Survey (n 1592; 2–17 years old) found that 18·6 % of females and 13·2 % of males had skipped breakfast on at least one of two recall days, while the prevalence of skipping on both recall days was 3·8 % among females and 1·4 % among males(Reference Smith, Breslin and McNaughton3).

Breakfast consumption has been associated with school performance and academic achievement(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5,Reference Smith, Blizzard and McNaughton11,Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Champ12) . An Australian study found that breakfast skipping among 8–9-year-old children was associated with poorer academic outcomes as reported by teachers 2 years later as well as literacy and numeracy outcomes(Reference Smith, Blizzard and McNaughton11). A study in the Netherlands found that among students 11–18 years of age, skipping breakfast on any school day was associated with the poorer end of term grades(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5). A systematic review on the effect of breakfast consumption on cognitive outcomes in children and adolescents found that eating breakfast had a positive effect on certain aspects of cognitive function measured (e.g. attention, executive function, and memory) within 4 h post-breakfast, compared with skipping breakfast(Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Champ12). The authors also concluded that the effects of breakfast consumption were strongest among undernourished children, indicating that in some cases reducing the prevalence of breakfast skipping could have considerable positive effects among children most in need(Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Champ12). Many observational studies on the effects of breakfast on academic and psychosocial outcomes, however, have failed to account for potential confounding by socio-economic factors(Reference Lee, Kim and Han13Reference Kleinman, Hall and Green15).

In some countries, such as the USA, school breakfast programs have become a popular intervention to increase breakfast consumption. The evidence of school breakfast programs’ impact on academic performance and behaviour in schools remains mixed. Randomised controlled trials of school breakfast programs in the USA, UK and New Zealand found little impact on outcomes such as attention, concentration, memory, behaviour, school attendance or academic achievement(Reference Christenson, Toft and Mikkelsen16Reference Mhurchu, Gorton and Turley18). On the other hand, some studies from the USA have found positive effects of breakfast programs on maths and reading scores and school attendance(Reference Bartfeld, Berger and Men19,Reference Imberman and Kugler20) . It should also be noted that many studies of school breakfast programs report low attendance and no decrease in the prevalence of breakfast skipping(Reference Murphy, Moore and Tapper17,Reference Mhurchu, Gorton and Turley18,Reference Godin, Patte and Leatherdale21,Reference Kothe and Mullan22) .

The relationship between breakfast skipping and academic outcomes has been well studied, but the relationship between breakfast skipping and other school-related outcomes, such as school engagement, is relatively understudied(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5,Reference Smith, Blizzard and McNaughton11,Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Champ12,Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Dye23) . School engagement is a multifaceted construct that includes three main dimensions: behavioural, emotional and cognitive engagement(Reference Fredricks, Blumenfeld and Paris24). These dimensions are dynamic in themselves and capture how involved students are with school (behavioural), effort applied to learning (cognitive), attitudes to peers and teachers at school and how aspects of school are valued (emotional). School engagement has been identified as an important and potentially malleable predictor of positive school and later life outcomes(Reference Xing and Gordon25,Reference Johnson, McGue and Iacono26) . In a longitudinal Australian study, school engagement was associated with continuing education post-school and higher status occupations, after adjusting for socio-economic status (SES) in childhood and school-level variables (e.g. government or private, number of students)(Reference Abbott-Chapman, Martin and Ollington27). Thus, school engagement may contribute to students’ well-being while they are still at school and reduce rates of school dropout, making it an important outcome to study in addition to academic performance(Reference Van Ryzin, Gravely and Roseth28,Reference Archambault, Janosz and Fallu29) .

Using data from a population-level survey of children’s well-being and engagement at school, the current study aimed to investigate the association between breakfast skipping and school engagement. In light of the studies that have previously found associations between breakfast skipping and other school outcomes, we hypothesised that there would be an association between breakfast skipping and school engagement, with children who skip breakfast experiencing lower cognitive and emotional engagement at school. Furthermore, since the systematic review identified differences in the prevalence of breakfast skipping by age, sex and SES(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard1), we investigated whether these factors also modified the association between breakfast skipping and school engagement, as this information may be important for targeting future breakfast policies and interventions.

Methods

Data source

The Wellbeing and Engagement Collection (WEC) is a population-level survey conducted annually in South Australian schools. The current study utilised data from the 2019 WEC. All South Australian schools (n 715) were invited to participate in the 2019 WEC, and school-level participation rates of 89 % (government/public schools), 52 % (Catholic schools) and 19 % (independent schools) were achieved. A total of 95 973 students, from Grade 4 to 12, completed the WEC in 2019. In the current study, only WEC data from the students in government/public schools was utilised because this could be linked to enrolment census data held by the South Australian Department for Education to provide information on a range of child and family-level socio-demographic confounders.

The WEC survey is designed to be completed over one to two class periods at school, taking approximately 25–45 min to complete. The WEC measures four domains of student well-being and engagement: Emotional Wellbeing, Engagement with School, Learning Readiness, and Health and Wellbeing out of school, and many different constructs within each domain. These constructs (e.g. happiness, sadness, optimism) are measured using a combination of multi-item scales and single items(Reference Gregory and Brinkman30). In this study, one measure of cognitive engagement and two measures of emotional engagement (Emotional Engagement with Teachers and School Climate) were used as outcomes. These are described in more detail below.

Ethical approval and consent

All identifying information was removed from the dataset (i.e. name, address, date of birth) before receiving it for analysis to ensure confidentiality. The sampling method consisted of inviting all primary and secondary schools in South Australia to participate. Schools were then free to decide if all or some classes would complete the WEC, and the parents and guardians of children in these classes received an information letter. This gave them time to withdraw their child if they so wished, and children could opt out themselves before or during the survey at any time.

Participants

The participants were students from government schools in South Australia, Grades 4–12, who completed the WEC in 2019. Figure 1 shows the number of schools and students in the 2019 enrolment census (n 118 910). A total of 453 government schools participated in the WEC, and 77 322 students from these schools completed the WEC survey (67·6 % student participation rate). A small number of these students (n 1005, 1·3 %) started the survey but did not complete enough items such that their responses were deemed invalid. The response sample was 76 317 students, of which 14 492 students were excluded as they had missing data on one or more of the outcome, exposure or confounder variables used in the analysis (see online Supplemental Table 1 for characteristics of response sample). The analysis sample included 61 825 students who had observations on all items used in this study (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants/sample

Measures

Exposure

Breakfast skipping

Breakfast consumption was measured using a single item that asked students ‘how often do you eat breakfast?’ Students responded using an eight-point scale (Never, Once a week, 2 times a week, …, 6 times a week, Every day). This item was recoded into 1 = never skippers (eat breakfast every day), 2 = sometimes skippers (eat breakfast 1 to 6 times a week) and 3 = always skippers (never eats breakfast) to compare children who eat breakfast everyday with children who skip breakfast. Similar self-report measures of breakfast consumption have been used in previous studies(Reference DeJong, van Lenthe and van der Horst31,Reference Gebremariam, Henjum and Hurum32) .

Outcomes

Cognitive engagement

The five-item cognitive engagement scale measures how students engage with learning and how they apply themselves. It includes items such as ‘I work hard on learning’, ‘When I found something hard I tried another way’ and ‘No matter who you are, you can change your intelligence’. Students responded using a five-point Likert scale (Never = 1 to Always = 5), and the mean of the five items was calculated. This item was adapted from the twelve-item cognitive engagement subscale, from the Teaching for Effective Learning School Engagement survey (created by the SA Department for Education), for the WEC to expand its measures of student engagement(Reference Gregory and Brinkman30).

Emotional engagement with teachers

The five-item emotional engagement with teachers scale includes items such as ‘I get along well with most of my teachers’, ‘Most of my teachers are interested in my wellbeing’ and ‘If I need extra help, I will receive it from my teachers’. Students responded using a four-point Likert scale (Strongly Disagree = 1 to Strongly Agree = 4), and the mean of the five items was calculated. It was originally used in the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) Student Context Questionnaire, which was developed by Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD) and has been used across OECD countries since 2000(33).

School climate

The three-item school climate scale includes the items: ‘Teachers and students treat each other with respect in this school’, ‘People care about each other in this school’ and ‘Students in this school help each other, even if they are not friends’. Students responded using a five-point Likert scale (Strongly Disagree = 1 to Strongly Agree = 5), and the mean of the three items was calculated. Originally adapted from the Self-Beliefs/Academic Self-efficacy scale, this item was used in this study to capture emotional engagement to the school more broadly(Reference Schonert-Reichl, Guhn and Hymel34,Reference Roeser, Midgley and Urdan35) .

All three of these scales have been psychometrically tested, showing high internal reliability among children in all grades with Cronbach’s α statistics ranging from 0 80 to 0 89(Reference Gregory and Brinkman30).

Confounding

Potential confounders of the relationship between breakfast skipping and school engagement were selected prior to the analysis based on a causal model using directed acyclic graphs and consulting the literature (see online Supplemental Fig. 1). These included age, gender, overall health, sadness and worry; highest level of parent education; community level SES and geographical remoteness. Overall health was based on the item ‘In general, how would you describe your health?’ with a four-point response scale (poor = 1 to excellent = 4) categorised into low (poor and fair), medium (good) and high (excellent). Sleep was measured using a single item that asked students ‘How often do you get a good night’s sleep?’, with response options ‘0 = Never’ through to ‘7 = Everyday’. Sadness was measured using a three-item scale, which was adapted from the Depression subscale in the Seattle Personality Questionnaire(Reference Schonert-Reichl, Guhn and Hymel34,Reference Kusche, Greenberg and Beilke36) . The sadness scale includes items such as ‘I feel unhappy a lot of the time’ and a five-point Likert scale (Strongly Disagree = 1 to Strongly Agree = 5). The three-item worry/anxiety scale was used to measure students’ worries, including items such as ‘I worry about things at home’ and a five-point Likert scale (Strongly Disagree = 1 to Strongly Agree = 5). The following variables were made available through the linked school enrolment census data, which is based on the information given by parents or caregivers. Parent education was based on the highest level of education attained by a student’s parents/caregivers (i.e. year 9 or equivalent or below; year 10 or equivalent; year 11 or equivalent; year 12 or equivalent; Certificate I to IV; Advanced Diploma or Diploma or Bachelor Degree or above). SES was based on the 2016 Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas’ Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage for students’ postcodes, which compares areas according to different aspects of socio-economic disadvantage or advantage. The Accessibility and Remoteness Index of Australia, according to students’ residential postcodes (i.e. zipcodes), was used to distinguish students who live in major cities, inner regional, outer regional, remote or very remote areas of South Australia. The remote and very remote categories were combined due to small numbers. The constructs and measures used in this study are measured the same way (i.e. using the same items/scales) across all age groups.

Analysis

A series of linear regression models were used to test the effect of breakfast skipping on cognitive and emotional engagement before and after adjustment for confounders (described above). The children and adolescents who never skip breakfast were the reference category for the exposure variables in all analyses because most children were in this category and eating breakfast everyday is considered to be the ideal frequency. All three school engagement outcomes were confirmed to be normally distributed prior to analysis. A small number of students did not have data on all three outcomes. We explored if the effect estimates differed according to whether the analysis included students only with all three outcomes, exposures and confounders (main analysis; complete case sample), from an analysis where students had valid scores on one specific outcome but missing scores on another outcomes (response sample; see online Supplemental Table 2). The sample size for these three analyses varied between 63 441 and 64 001, depending on the amount of missing data on each outcome. The effect estimates using the complete case sample and the response sample were the same, suggesting non-completion on some outcomes did not influence the results. The effect measure modification analyses (see online Supplemental Appendix A) were conducted according to best epidemiological practice(Reference Knol and VanderWeele37). The relative excess risk due to interaction was calculated to estimate the extent of effect measure modification on the risk difference scale, as this is considered most relevant for public health. The exposure, outcomes and effect modification variables were dichotomised for these analyses (see online Supplemental Appendix A).

Analyses were conducted using Stata se version 16.

Results

Table 1 describes the demographic characteristics of students included in the analysis according to breakfast skipping categories (i.e. never, sometimes or always skips breakfast). In the analysis sample, 55·0 % of students reported that they ate breakfast everyday, 35·4 % reported sometimes skipping breakfast and 9·6 % reported always skipping breakfast (Table 1). Always skippers tended to be older and female, with lower parent education and a higher proportion lived in lower socio-economic areas and regional areas. Children who reported always skipping breakfast often reported less nights of good sleep and increased sadness and worry compared with sometimes and never skippers. Of note is that the proportion reporting high overall health among never skippers (41·4 %) was similar to the proportion reporting low overall health among always skippers (48·5 %). All outcome variables showed a gradient across never, sometimes and always skipping, with students that never skip breakfast reporting higher levels of cognitive engagement, engagement with teachers and school climate. Students that never skip breakfast reported mean (sd) scores for cognitive engagement, emotional engagement and school climate of 4·0 (0·8), 3·2 (0·5) and 3·7 (0·8), respectively, while children who always skip breakfast reported scores of 3·2 (1·0), 2·8 (0·7) and 3·0 (1·0). Mean scores of students that sometimes skip breakfast were always in the middle. The demographic characteristics of the analysis sample were similar to the response sample (see online Supplemental Table 1).

Table 1 Characteristics of analysis sample according to exposure of breakfast skipping (n 61 825)

* Sleep measures how many nights, on average, do students feel they get a good night’s sleep (0–7).

Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage is a set of measures derived from Australian Bureau of Statistics census information that summarise different aspects of socio-economic conditions in an area.

Accessibility and Remoteness Index of Australia (i.e. geographical remoteness).

The adjusted and unadjusted results of the linear regression of the association between breakfast skipping and school engagement outcomes are shown in Table 2. The results are presented as unstandardised regression coefficients (i.e. the mean differences in outcome(s) relative to the reference group of never breakfast skippers) and 95 % CI. In the adjusted models, children who always skipped breakfast reported lower levels of cognitive engagement (β = −0·26 (95 % CI −0·29, −0·25), P < 0·0001), emotional engagement with teachers (β = −0·17 (95 % CI −0·18, −0·15), P < 0·0001) and school climate (β = −0·17 (95 % CI −0·19, −0·15), P < 0·0001), compared with those who never skipped breakfast. Children who sometimes skipped breakfast also reported lower levels of engagement compared with children who never skipped breakfast, but not as low as always skippers. For example, after adjustment, the effect of always skipping on cognitive engagement was −0·26 (95 % CI −0·29, −0·25), P < 0·0001)), but the effect of sometimes skipping was only −0·08 (95 % CI −0·09, −0·07), P < 0·0001)).

Table 2 Linear regression results for the effect of skipping breakfast on school engagement (n 61 825)

Adjusted for: age, gender, overall health, sleep, sadness and worries scales, highest level of parent education, student Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage and student Accessibility and Remoteness Index of Australia.

β is unstandardised beta-coefficient.

Across all school engagement outcomes, we found limited evidence of effect measure modification by sex, socio-economic position or age (see online Supplemental Tables 36). For example, the within-stratum effects suggest a 19 % higher risk of poor cognitive engagement among males who sometimes/always skip breakfast compared with those who never skip breakfast (RR 1·19 (95 % CI 1·12, 1·26)) and a 33 % higher risk among females (RR 1·33 (95 % CI 1·24, 1·43)). However, the relative excess risk due to interaction of 0·01 (95 % CI −0·09, 0·12) indicates no effect measure modification by sex on the risk difference scale. That is, the combined risks of both breakfast skipping and sex was not greater than the sum of the individual risks of breakfast skipping and sex. There was limited evidence of effect measure modification by socio-economic position or age, with five of the six relative excess risk due to interaction estimates close to zero with 95 % CI that included zero. The single exception was the effect of breakfast skipping on school climate by age, with results suggesting this effect was larger in primary school compared with high school students. Full results for the effect measure modification analyses can be found in Supplemental Appendix A.

Discussion

There has been a great deal of research on the association between breakfast consumption and school outcomes, but these studies tend to focus on academic achievement and cognitive function (e.g. memory tests) rather than how students engage with schooling and teachers. This current study explored the latter, focusing on the relationship between breakfast skipping and two dimensions of school engagement; cognitive and emotional engagement. Consistent with our hypothesis, we found that skipping breakfast was associated with lower cognitive and emotional engagement at school. Children who reported always skipping breakfast had lower engagement scores compared with those that sometimes skipped breakfast or never skipped breakfast. These findings build on prior observational research of breakfast and school outcomes that used small sample sizes (ranging from n 97 to n 294) and/or did not adjust for potential confounding(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5,Reference Lee, Kim and Han13Reference Kleinman, Hall and Green15,Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Dye23) . For instance, one study found that regular breakfast consumption was associated with an improved emotional state among adolescents in South Korea (n 62 276), but had not adjusted for any confounding(Reference Lee, Kim and Han13). Although the prevalence of breakfast skipping is reported to differ according to age, sex and socio-economic position(Reference Rampersaud, Pereira and Girard1), we found limited evidence that this extended to the association between skipping and most outcome measurements of cognitive and school engagement. The only exception was the association between breakfast skipping and school climate, where the effect of breakfast skipping on school climate appeared larger for a primary school than high school students.

Considering that research has shown there is a substantial decline in school engagement as students move from primary school to high school(Reference Fredricks, Blumenfeld and Paris24,Reference Fredricks and Eccles38) , comparing the effect of this transition on school engagement with the effects observed in our study might offer a helpful comparison to put the magnitude of these effects in perspective(Reference Kraft39). Normative data from the WEC suggests that the difference in mean scores observed for each outcome between children in their last 2 years of primary school compared with their first 2 years of high school in the 2019 WEC were −0·23 for cognitive engagement, −0·19 for emotional engagement with teachers and −0·27 for school climate(Reference Gregory and Brinkman30). This suggests the adjusted effect sizes observed, for cognitive engagement (−0·26, (95 % CI −0·29, −0·24)) and emotional engagement with teachers (−0·17, (95 % CI −0·18, −0·15)) especially, are similar in magnitude to the decrease in engagement already experienced by students as they move through school, which is substantial.

These findings are consistent with previous research on the association between breakfast and school outcomes, such as academic achievement, which often require high levels of cognitive engagement to succeed(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5,Reference Stea and Torstveit40,Reference Littlecott, Moore and Moore41) . A study in the UK (n 294, 14–15 years) found that after controlling for age, sex, BMI, ethnicity and SES, students who rarely (never or once a week) consumed breakfast on school days scored lower General Certificate of Secondary Education grades(Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Dye23). In an Australian study, 8–9-year-olds who skipped breakfast (n 2280) had poorer teacher-reported academic outcomes than children who did not skip breakfast. However, because there was only a slight difference observed for objective National Assessment Program – Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) results, the authors argued the results might be due to unmeasured cofounding between children who regularly skipped breakfast and teacher-reported outcomes(Reference Smith, Blizzard and McNaughton11). Similar to our study, these findings both suggest there is something about eating breakfast that can indeed impact school outcomes. Furthermore, levels of school engagement might be an indirect pathway through which breakfast impacts academic outcomes(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5).

One possible mechanism for how breakfast influences school outcomes is that eating breakfast promotes glucose uptake in the brain. Glucose is the brain’s main fuel source and provides energy to concentrate at school(Reference Bellisle42). Hence, if skipping breakfast means that children arrive at school hungry, are distracted and do not have much energy, they may be less emotionally and cognitively engaged(Reference Boschloo, Ouwehand and Dekker5). The quality of the breakfast may be pertinent if skipping breakfast was compared with eating a healthy breakfast. For instance, evidence shows low glycaemic index foods sustain blood glucose levels for longer periods, which can improve attention at school, compared with fasting or eating a high glycaemic index breakfast(Reference Edefonti, Rosato and Parpinel43). Over a longer period of time, through improving the overall quality of children’s diets, breakfast could impact school outcomes through long-term overall health promotion, which itself has been found to be associated with school engagement(Reference Carter, McGee and Taylor44). A combination of these mechanisms could have contributed to the effects observed in this study. Increased breakfast consumption has been shown to have a stronger impact on school outcomes among undernourished children or children from lower socio-economic backgrounds, suggesting where gains can be made (i.e. in nutritional quality or less discretionary foods) it could have an impact on how students engage with school(Reference Adolphus, Lawton and Champ12,Reference Bartfeld, Berger and Men19,Reference Hoyland, Dye and Lawton45) .

Strengths of this study were adjusting for a range of confounders and the large population-based sample, which allowed for the detection of small differences in engagement levels. There are some limitations that should be considered. While we adjusted for a community-level measure of SES and parental education, it is possible that the effect estimates remain residually confounded through factors such as socioeconomically patterned attitudes to health and education or family structure(Reference Levin, Kirby and Currie8,Reference Moore, Tapper and Murphy46,Reference Madarasova, Tavel and van Dijk47) . Some measurement bias (e.g. low content validity) is common when using large population-level surveys, which can lead to unmeasured confounding. Research on the 2019 WEC has shown there is some bias in the WEC sample with children from more socio-economically disadvantaged backgrounds under-represented(Reference Gregory, Lewkowicz and Engelhardt48). Further, there was no information on why children skipped breakfast. This information may provide a clearer relationship between breakfast skipping and school engagement, especially if the reason represents a confounding relationship, such as food insecurity. On the other hand, if most breakfast skippers reported it is because they are not hungry in the mornings, appetite is less likely to be a direct influence on school engagement.

The possibility that children report skipping breakfast because of food insecurity has driven much of the concern over breakfast skipping, however it is rarely given as a reason for skipping breakfast(Reference Sweeney and Horishita49). Reasons that are commonly given by adolescents for skipping breakfast include lack of time, lack of appetite and/or dieting, and attitudes towards breakfast tend to be the strongest predictor of breakfast consumption(Reference Mullan, Wong and Kothe50Reference Shaw52). While interventions (e.g. school breakfast programs) aiming to increase breakfast consumption rarely achieve their goal, the most successful among them include persuasive messaging that increases positive attitudes towards breakfast(Reference Kothe and Mullan22,Reference Kothe, Mullan and Amaratunga53,Reference Bruening, Larson and Story54) . As such, schools and education departments may want to explore the impact of low cost, health promotion interventions in classrooms focussed on attitudes towards breakfast on breakfast consumption and school outcomes, including levels of engagement.

To conclude, this study demonstrated an association between skipping breakfast and cognitive and emotional engagement at school, among a large sample of school students in Australia, after adjustment for a comprehensive set of child and family-level confounders. Similar to studies on the effects of breakfast on academic performance, our study shows there could also be an important link between breakfast and students’ engagement. Short-term energy supply and long-term health impacts are two possible mechanisms that may explain this association. Considering these findings, decreasing the prevalence of breakfast skipping could have a positive impact on school engagement.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: None. Financial support: Authors received no specific funding for this work. Authorship: H.M.: conceptualisation, methodology, formal analysis, writing – original draft and editing. A.S.: conceptualisation, methodology, writing – review and editing and supervision. T.G.: methodology, writing – review and editing, supervision. L.S.: methodology, writing – review and editing and supervision. Ethics of human subject participation: Ethics approval for this study was granted by the University of Western Australia, and research approval was received from the data custodian (South Australian Department for Education).

Conflict of interest:

There are no conflicts of interest.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021004870

References

Rampersaud, G, Pereira, M, Girard, B et al. (2005) Breakfast habits, nutritional status, body weight, and academic performance in children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 105, 543760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deshmukh-Taskar, P, Nicklas, T, O’Neil, C et al. (2010) The relationship of breakfast skipping and type of breakfast consumption with nutrient intake and weight status in children and adolescents: the national health and nutrition examination survey 1999–2006. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 868878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, KJ, Breslin, MC, McNaughton, SA et al. (2017) Skipping breakfast among Australian children and adolescents; findings from the 2011–2012 national nutrition and physical activity survey. Aust N Z J Public Health 41, 572578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Utter, J, Scragg, R, Mhurchu, C et al. (2007) At-home breakfast consumption among New Zealand children: associations with body mass index and related nutrition behaviors. J Am Diet Assoc 107, 570576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boschloo, A, Ouwehand, C, Dekker, S et al. (2012) The relation between breakfast skipping and school performance in adolescents. Int Mind Brain Educ Soc 6, 8188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jose, K, Vandenberg, M, Williams, J et al. (2019) The changing role of Australian primary schools in providing breakfast to students: a qualitative study. Health Promot J Austr 31, 5867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, R, Giskes, K, Turrell, G et al. (2011) Food insecurity among Australian children: potential determinants, health and developmental consequences. J Child Health Care 15, 401416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, KA, Kirby, J & Currie, C (2012) Family structure and breakfast consumption of 11–15 year old boys and girls in Scotland, 1994–2010: a repeated cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 12, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jørgensen, A, Pedersen, TP, Meilstrup, CR et al. (2011) The influence of family structure on breakfast habits among adolescents. Dan Med Bull 58, A4262.Google ScholarPubMed
Fayet-Moore, F, Kim, J, Sritharan, N et al. (2016) Impact of breakfast skipping and breakfast choice on the nutrient intake and body mass index of Australian children. Nutrients 8, 487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, KJ, Blizzard, L, McNaughton, SA et al. (2017) Skipping breakfast among 8–9 year old children is associated with teacher-reported but not objectively measured academic performance 2 years later. BMC Nutr 3, 86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adolphus, K, Lawton, CL, Champ, CL et al. (2016) The effects of breakfast and breakfast composition on cognition in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Adv Nutr 7, 590S612S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, H, Kim, C, Han, I et al. (2019) Emotional state according to breakfast consumption in 62276 South Korean adolescents. Iran J Pediatr 29, e92193.Google Scholar
Murphy, JM, Pagano, M, Nachmani, J et al. (1998) The relationship of school breakfast to psychosocial and academic functioning. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 152, 899907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, RE, Hall, S, Green, H et al. (2002) Diet, breakfast, and academic performance in children. Ann Nutr Metab 46, 2430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christenson, C, Toft, U & Mikkelsen, BE (2017) Food provision in a breakfast club does not impact concentration and sustained attention performance among young people at vocational school. In 10th International Conference on Culinary Arts and Sciences. Aalborg University Copenhagen, Captive Food Studies, pp. 161.Google Scholar
Murphy, S, Moore, GF, Tapper, K et al. (2010) Free healthy breakfasts in primary schools: a cluster randomised controlled trial of a policy intervention in Wales, UK. Public Health Nutr 14, 219226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mhurchu, C, Gorton, D, Turley, M et al. (2013) Effects of a free school breakfast programme on children’s attendance, academic achievement and short-term hunger: results from a stepped-wedge, cluster randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Commun Health 67, 257264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartfeld, J, Berger, L, Men, F et al. (2019) Access to the school breakfast program is associated with higher attendance and test scores among elementary school students. J Nutr 149, 336343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imberman, SA & Kugler, AD (2014) The effect of providing breakfast in class on student performance. J Policy Anal Manag 33, 669699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godin, KM, Patte, KA & Leatherdale, ST (2018) Examining predictors of breakfast skipping and breakfast program use among secondary school students in the COMPASS study. J Sch Health 88, 150158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kothe, EJ & Mullan, B (2011) Increasing the frequency of breakfast consumption. Br Food J 113, 784796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphus, K, Lawton, CL & Dye, L (2019) Associations between habitual school-day breakfast consumption frequency and academic performance in British adolescents. Front Public Health 7, 283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredricks, JA, Blumenfeld, PC & Paris, AH (2004) School engagement: potential of the concept, state of the evidence. Rev Educ Res 74, 59109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xing, X & Gordon, HRD (2020) Mediating effects of school engagement between high school on-time completion and career and technical education. Vocat Learn, 121. doi: 10.1007/s12186-020-09252-2.Google Scholar
Johnson, W, McGue, M & Iacono, WG (2006) Genetic and environmental influences on academic achievement trajectories during adolescence. Dev Psychol 42, 514532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abbott-Chapman, J, Martin, K, Ollington, N et al. (2014) The longitudinal association of childhood school engagement with adult educational and occupational achievement: findings from an Australian national study. Br Educ Res J 40, 102120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Ryzin, MJ, Gravely, AA & Roseth, CJ (2009) Autonomy, belongingness, and engagement in school as contributors to adolescent psychological well-being. J Youth Adolesc 38, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Archambault, I, Janosz, M, Fallu, J et al. (2009) Student engagement and its relationship with early high school dropout. J Adolesc 32, 651670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregory, T & Brinkman, S (2020) Wellbeing and Engagement Collection (WEC): History of the WEC in the South Australian School System and Psychometric Properties of the WEC Survey Instrument. Adelaide: The University of Western Australia.Google Scholar
DeJong, CS, van Lenthe, FJ, van der Horst, K et al. (2009) Environmental and cognitive correlates of adolescent breakfast consumption. Prev Med 48, 372377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gebremariam, MK, Henjum, S, Hurum, E et al. (2017) Mediators of the association between parental education and breakfast consumption among adolescents: the ESSENS study. BMC Pediatr 17, 61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OECD (2012) OECD Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA). www.pisa.oecd.org (accessed October 2020).Google Scholar
Schonert-Reichl, KA, Guhn, M, Hymel, S et al. (2010) Our Children’s Voices: The Middle Years Development Instrument. Burnaby, BC: United Way of the Lower Mainland.Google Scholar
Roeser, WR, Midgley, C & Urdan, TC (1996) Percpetions of the school psychological environment and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioural functioning in school: the mediating role of goals and belongings. J Educ Psychol 88, 408422.Google Scholar
Kusche, CA, Greenberg, MT & Beilke, R (1988) Seattle Personality Questionnaire for Young School-Aged Children. Unpublished Personality Questionnaire. Seattle: University of Washington, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
Knol, MJ & VanderWeele, TJ (2012) Recommendations for presenting analyses of effect modification and interaction. Int J Epidemiol 41, 514520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredricks, JA & Eccles, J (2002) Children’s competence and value beliefs from childhood to adolescence: growth trajectories in two “male-typed” domains. J Dev Psychol 38, 519533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraft, MA (2020) Interpreting effect sizes of education interventions. Educ Res 49, 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stea, T & Torstveit, M (2014) Association of lifestyle habits and academic achievement in Norwegian adolescents: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 14, 829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Littlecott, HJ, Moore, GF, Moore, L et al. (2015) Association between breakfast consumption and educational outcomes in 9–11-year-old children. Public Health Nutr 19, 15751582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellisle, F (2004) Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children. Br J Nutr 92, S227S232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edefonti, V, Rosato, V, Parpinel, M et al. (2014) The effect of breakfast composition and energy contribution on cognitive and academic performance: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 100, 626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, M, McGee, R, Taylor, B et al. (2007) Health outcomes in adolescence: associations with family, friends and school engagement. J Adolesc 30, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyland, A, Dye, L & Lawton, CL (2009) A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev 22, 220243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, G, Tapper, K, Murphy, S et al. (2007) Associations between deprivation, attitudes towards eating breakfast and breakfast eating behaviours in 9–11-year-olds. Public Health Nutr 10, 582589.Google ScholarPubMed
Madarasova, GA, Tavel, P, van Dijk, JP et al. (2010) Factors associated with educational aspirations among adolescents: cues to counteract socioeconomic differences? BMC Public Health 10, 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gregory, T, Lewkowicz, A, Engelhardt, D et al. (2021) Data resource profile: the South Australian Well-being and Engagement Collection (WEC). Int J Epidemiol, dyab103. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyab103.Google Scholar
Sweeney, NM & Horishita, N (2005) The breakfast-eating habits of inner city high school students. J Sch Nurs 21, 100105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mullan, B, Wong, C, Kothe, E et al. (2014) An examination of the demographic predictors of adolescent breakfast consumption, content, and context. BMC Public Health 14, 264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullan, B, Wong, C, Kothe, E et al. (2013) Predicting breakfast consumption: a comparison of the theory of planned behaviour and the health action process approach. Br Food J 115, 16381657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, M (1998) Adolescent breakfast skipping: an Australian study. Adolescence 33, 851.Google Scholar
Kothe, EJ, Mullan, BA & Amaratunga, R (2011) Randomised controlled trial of a brief theory-based intervention promoting breakfast consumption. Appetite 56, 148155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruening, M, Larson, N, Story, M et al. (2011) Predictors of adolescent breakfast consumption: longitudinal findings from project EAT. J Nutr Educ Behav 43, 390395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow chart of participants/sample

Figure 1

Table 1 Characteristics of analysis sample according to exposure of breakfast skipping (n 61 825)

Figure 2

Table 2 Linear regression results for the effect of skipping breakfast on school engagement (n 61 825)

Supplementary material: File

Moller et al. supplementary material

Moller et al. supplementary material

Download Moller et al. supplementary material(File)
File 333.8 KB