Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T06:30:27.052Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predicting Rank Attainment in Political Science: What Else Besides Publications Affects Promotion?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 June 2012

Vicki L. Hesli
Affiliation:
University of Iowa
Jae Mook Lee
Affiliation:
University of Iowa
Sara McLaughlin Mitchell
Affiliation:
University of Iowa
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

We report the results of hypotheses tests about the effects of several measures of research, teaching, and service on the likelihood of achieving the ranks of associate and full professor. In conducting these tests, we control for institutional and individual background characteristics. We focus our tests on the link between productivity and academic rank and explore whether this relationship reveals a gender dimension. The analyses are based on an APSA-sponsored survey of all faculty members in departments of political science (government, public affairs, and international relations) in the United States.

Type
The Profession
Copyright
Copyright © American Political Science Association 2012

Promotion decisions are among the most important choices that academic departments make. Generally, promotion from assistant to associate professor brings the decisive reward of tenure—an almost certain guarantee of continuing employment. Wise promotion decisions enhance a department's prestige, while failure to promote a capable scholar means losing talent to another university or possibly an end to a promising academic career (Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993, 703). Higher rank yields better salaries and more influence within the department.

An extensive literature exists on the subject of academic promotion. This literature is based on studies of several different disciplines, from medicine and economics to the social sciences and humanities. Our own analyses of the factors affecting promotion, presented in the second part of this article, are based solely on the discipline of political science. For these analyses, we rely on a 2009 APSA-sponsored survey of all faculty employed in political science departments (including departments of government and public affairs) throughout the United States. (Appendix A provides a description of the survey methodology.) We find that although age (or years of experience) is the best predictor of rank, productivity in terms of publications is a consistently reliable predictor of promotion, except when comparing female assistant to female associate professors. We provide evidence that women are less likely than men to move from assistant professor to associate professor rank. When a woman does achieve associate professor rank, she is as likely as her male colleagues to move on to the rank of full professor.

LITERATURE REVIEW

According to nearly 40-year-old interviews of chairpersons and heads of departments, the criteria used to judge individuals at promotion time are “teaching, research, and public service to the university” (Katz Reference Katz1973, 470). Such criteria are now enshrined in faculty handbooks and operations manuals for all colleges and universities. Katz's survey also revealed that “research ability, publication record, and national reputation” were “the most important factors influencing salary and promotion decisions” (470). Doering (Reference Doering1972), however, explored the idea that seniority, years of experience, or simply age is the best predictor of promotion—as one generally cannot advance through the ranks without spending a certain amount of time in each rank (see also Lewis Reference Lewis1967).

Generally findings confirm “that most institutions of higher education ‘continue to promote or retain faculty members largely on the basis of publications’” (Woodring Reference Woodring1964, 45 as quoted in Doering Reference Doering1972, 11). The number of books authored and the number of journal articles published have positive effects on academic career advancement (Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes1999, 400; Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 201; Lewis Reference Lewis1998; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993, 718; Over Reference Over1993, 318; Tien Reference Tien2007). Publications have become even more important for promotion than they were in the past given the “increased competition for permanent academic positions” (Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 62).

Publications, however, are only one part of faculty productivity. The receipt of grants is also linked to promotion (Fang et al. Reference Fang, Moy, Colburn and Hurley2000, 1090; Lee Reference Lee2002, 703). Whether one's primary work activity is teaching or research influences the duration and probability of promotion (Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes1999 and Reference Ginther and Hayes2003; Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004). Committee service is also an important variable in explaining rank (Katz Reference Katz1973, 476; Lee Reference Lee2002, 703).

The type of institution where the faculty member is employed may also affect promotion (Lee Reference Lee2002, 704). Indeed, Ornstein, Stewart, and Drakich (Reference Ornstein, Stewart and Drakich2007, 19) find that “institutional differences in [median time to promotion] are greater than disciplinary differences and much greater than the effect of gender.”Footnote 1 Rothgeb and Burger (Reference Rothgeb and Burger2009) compare US political science departments and find differences between BA and PhD departments in the general standards and procedures used when evaluating tenure. Other work indicates that the likelihood of promotion varies between public and private institutions (Fang et al. Reference Fang, Moy, Colburn and Hurley2000, 1087; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes1999, 400; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 50; Perna Reference Perna2005, 284; Tien Reference Tien2007). Long, Allison, and McGinnis (Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993) find that the more prestigious the department of current employment, the lower the odds of promotion, although the effects may be different for women and men (719; see also Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 202; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993, 797; McDowell and Smith Reference McDowell and Smith1992, 78; Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011, 545). Ginther and Kahn (Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 201) also report that the prestige or tier (rank) of the PhD-granting institution can influence the probability of promotion, as may time-to-degree and the field or area of study (see also Lee Reference Lee2002, 307; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993, 713–714, 719; Morrison et al. Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011, 545; Over Reference Over1993, 318).

In addition to efforts to link productivity, other career information, and institutional characteristics to academic rank, a body of research has focused on the question of whether faculty promotion reveals a gender dimension. Studies have also attempted to determine whether minorities face greater hurdles than their nonminority counterparts. Fueling these debates is evidence of larger numbers of women and racially diverse undergraduate and graduate student populations, yet the proportion of women and minorities among faculty in higher education has not caught up with these trends at the undergraduate and graduate levels.

As of 2009, 59% of the total postbaccalaureate fall enrollment in degree-granting institutions was female.Footnote 2 We compare this to faculty at the same point in time: In fall 2009, 47% of faculty were female.Footnote 3 Among the total 2009 fall enrollment in degree-granting institutions, 33% were of a nonwhite race or ethnicity.Footnote 4 For the same year, 18% of college and university faculty were African American, Asian/Pacific Islander, Hispanic, or American Indian/Alaska Native (based on a faculty count that excludes persons whose race/ethnicity was unknown).Footnote 5

Looking specifically at political science (and government), 45% of bachelor's degrees conferred by degree-granting institutions in 2008–09 were given to women, and 38.5% of doctoral degrees conferred by degree-granting institutions in 2008–09 were given to women.Footnote 6 According to the National Science Foundation “Survey of Earned Doctorates,” 40% of doctoral degrees earned in political science in 2009 went to women.Footnote 7 According to APSA data, the percentage of women among all political science faculty members in the United States in 2009 was 28%. The percentage of female political scientists by academic rank for 2009 was 39% for assistant professors, 30% for associate professors and 19% for full professors. So it appears that at the entry level, women are receiving academic appointments at the same rate as men, but they are not moving up the academic ranks at the same rate as male faculty.Footnote 8

In the 1970s and 1980s, an argument was made that not enough PhDs had yet been granted to women, thus the available pool of qualified female candidates was small, and the number of female tenured faculty simply reflected the smaller available pool. Such an argument is now less convincing given that the proportion of political science doctorate degrees awarded to women has increased from 23% during 1981–85, to 27% between 1986 and 1990, to 30% between 1991 and 1995, to 35% between 1996 and 2000, and to 39% between 2001 and 2005.Footnote 9 Enough time has passed for more women to have matriculated to full professor. Therefore, the numbers do lead to questions about what is happening to the women.

Numerous studies show that female academics are less likely to be promoted (or take longer to be promoted) than male academics (Allen and Castleman Reference Allen, Castleman, Brooks and Mackinnon2001; Fang et al. Reference Fang, Moy, Colburn and Hurley2000; Farber Reference Farber1977, 204; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes1999, 400; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 50; Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 200; Johnson and Stafford Reference Johnson and Stafford1974, 892; Kahn Reference Kahn1993, Reference Kahn1995; Katz Reference Katz1973, 471; Krefting Reference Krefting2003; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993; Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995; McDowell, Singell, and Ziliak Reference McDowell, Singell and Ziliak1999a, Reference McDowell, Singell and Ziliak1999b; McDowell and Smith Reference McDowell and Smith1992, 78; Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011; National Science Foundation 2004; Ornstein, Stewart, and Drakich Reference Ornstein, Stewart and Drakich2007, 15; Perna Reference Perna2001, Reference Perna2005; Roos and Gatta Reference Roos and Gatta2009; Rudd et al. Reference Rudd, Morrison, Sadrozinski, Nerad and Cerny2008; Sax et al. Reference Sax, Hagedorn, Arredondo and Dicrisi2002; Stewart, Ornstein, and Drakich Reference Stewart, Ornstein and Drakich2009; Tien Reference Tien2007, 113; Toutkoushian Reference Toutkoushian1999; Ward Reference Ward2001).Footnote 10 These findings are well-established, although Jackson and O'Callaghan (Reference Jackson and O'Callaghan2009, 472) argue against the majority in saying that in one-third of the studies that found gender disparities in position attainment and promotion, the differences could be attributed to differences other than gender (i.e., “cohort affect” noted by Morgan Reference Morgan1998). Fewer studies have linked race or ethnicity to a lower probability of promotion, although some studies have done so (Fang et al. Reference Fang, Moy, Colburn and Hurley2000, 1090; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 50; Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 206; Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995; Toutkoushian Reference Toutkoushian1999). Data from the 1990s reveal that both women and minorities were less likely to be employed in prestigious research universities (Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995, 51).

THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES

According to normative theory, “universalism” should characterize science. Universalism requires that the scientific communities' assessment of any contribution to scientific knowledge be based on “pre-established impersonal criteria” (Merton [1942] Reference Merton1973, 270 as described in Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995). “Particularism, in contrast, involves the use of functionally irrelevant characteristics, such as sex and race, as a basis for making claims and gaining rewards in science” (Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995, 46). The question is whether the underrepresentation of women and minorities in the higher academic ranks of US college and university faculty (as described in the above literature review) is the result of universalistic or particularistic criteria.

A variety of theoretical perspectives have attempted to account for different levels of career attainment for women as compared with men. Social capital theories argue that the resources needed to obtain tenure and promotion, such as “information and knowledge about institutional norms, expectations, and opportunities; access to and influence on key decision makers; certification and endorsement of an individual's qualifications; and emotional support and recognition” are less available to women than to men because women lack access to the collegial and social networks that convey critical job-related knowledge (Lin Reference Lin2001 as quoted in Perna Reference Perna2005, 280. See also Milem et al. Reference Milem, Sherlin, Irwin and Creamer2001; O'Leary and Mitchell Reference O'Leary, Mitchell, Lie and O'Leary1990; Tierney and Bensimon Reference Tierney and Bensimon1996; Yoder Reference Yoder1985). Networks are important at tenure time because they can result in more adulatory outside reference letters.

Other theories explain differential career progress by focusing on life cycle differences in labor force participation between men and women. A woman who is out of the labor force because of family responsibilities is not acquiring needed human capital (Becker Reference Becker1993; Farber Reference Farber1977; Johnson and Stafford Reference Johnson and Stafford1974; Mincer and Polachek Reference Mincer and Polachek1974; Zuckerman Reference Zuckerman and Dix1987). Noting additionally that women work fewer hours per year than men when they do work, the effect is that women accumulate fewer years of work experience (Johnson and Stafford Reference Johnson and Stafford1974, 892). Family responsibilities may cause women to pursue different types of jobs (for example, parttime work) or less demanding work (Becker Reference Becker1985), and the stress of childcare and household responsibilities may be greater for women than for men (Dey Reference Dey1994). Women may also be less mobile than men (Rosenfeld and Jones Reference Rosenfeld and Jones1987).Footnote 11

Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad (Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011, 526) observe that because of the need to earn tenure within a set time, “academic careers may be exceptionally demanding during the family formation phase of life” (Acker and Armenti, Reference Acker and Armenti2004; Jacobs Reference Jacobs2004; Jacobs and Winslow Reference Jacobs and Winslow2004a, Reference Jacobs and Winslow2004b; Over Reference Over1993, 318). Researchers have noted that for men, having children has a positive effect on promotion, although for women, children have a negative effect on promotion (Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 63–66; Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993; Mason and Goulden Reference Mason and Goulden2002, Reference Mason and Goulden2004; Perna Reference Perna2005; Ward and Wolf-Wendel Reference Ward and Wolf-Wendel2004). Other studies, however, do not find an independent effect of parenting on the likelihood of achieving tenure (Kulis and Sicotte Reference Kulis and Sicotte2002; Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011; Rudd et al. Reference Rudd, Morrison, Sadrozinski, Nerad and Cerny2008). Being married (with or without children) may affect the likelihood of being promoted (Long Reference Long2001; Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993; Perna Reference Perna2005, 285; Ward Reference Ward2001, 286), although the effect is likely to be different for men and women (Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes1999; Kulis and Sicotte Reference Kulis and Sicotte2002; Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011; Wolfinger, Mason, and Goulden Reference Wolfinger, Mason and Goulden2008). The repercussions of the effects of marriage and children are revealed in 2004 statistics from the National Study of Postsecondary Faculty that show that female faculty members are less likely to be married and much less likely to be both married and have children than their male counterparts.Footnote 12

Critics of social/human capital and life cycle theories argue that these do not adequately explain the lower returns on investment for women and minorities and the segregation of women into lower status occupations (England et al. Reference England, Farkas, Kilbourne and Dou1988). Dual or split labor market theories advanced by Feagin and Feagin (Reference Feagin and Feagin1986) argue that institutional barriers that were constructed historically to exclude women and minorities from core or primary sector employment have persisted even after social changes have rendered these barriers illegal (Lee Reference Lee2002, 697). Conflict theories assume that “dominant groups use their monopoly over resources to maintain their privileges” (Reskin Reference Reskin2003, 2).

More subtle cognitive processes may also operate to favor in-groups and disfavor out-groups (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel, Turner, Worchel and Austin1986). Rather than experiencing overt discrimination in the workplace, out-groups encounter “consensual status hierarchies” that operate structurally to produce inequality (DiTomaso et al. Reference DiTomaso, Post, Smith, Farris and Cordero2007, 176). Such processes, which perpetuate inequalities, persist not because of conscious efforts, but because individual actions “are complicit with previously established norms” (Bird and Rhoton Reference Bird, Rhoton, Jeans, Knights and Martin2011, 352, see also Rowe Reference Rowe1990). Social psychologists assert that common stereotypes about gender differences in a larger society (a hierarchy of gender status beliefs) are reproduced within organizations such as universities—and important consequences follow, such as differential access to resources and decisions about competence (Roos and Gatta Reference Roos and Gatta2009, 179; for a review see Heilman Reference Heilman2001). Because academic judgments of the quality of a colleague's work are inherently subjective, the tendency of evaluators is to fall back on existing schema, stereotypes, and personal biases (Eveline Reference Eveline2004). Sexism in peer review may be a more overt manifestation of such processes (Wenneras and Wold Reference Wenneras and Wold1997). Other examples of subtle or unconscious discrimination include encouragement of early promotion for men but not for women, more impressive language used to describe the records of men than for women, and promotion to senior professor largely on the basis of departmental administrative needs for men but not for women (Roos and Gatta Reference Roos and Gatta2009, 188).

THE DATA, ANALYSES, AND FINDINGS

Given that figures from the APSA, the National Science Foundation, and the US Department of Education all reveal the underrepresentation of women in higher academic ranks of political science, the Committee on the Status of Women within the APSA felt a responsibility to check whether the norms of universalism or particularism were operating in the process of rank attainment within our discipline. The committee approached the APSA Council, and the council allocated funds for a survey of all faculty members in departments of political science (and departments of government, public affairs, and international relations) in the United States. We base our analyses on the responses to this survey.

The dependent variable is academic rank. Academic rank has three categories: assistant professor, associate professor, and full professor. We are excluding from these analyses faculty with another rank (e.g., lecturer, instructor) and we selected only those respondents who were in a tenure-track position, which represents the lion's share of respondents (92%).Footnote 13 Across the entire set of 1,399 respondents to the 2009 APSA survey of political science faculty, 28.3% were assistant professors, 25.5% were associate professors, and 37.2% were full professors.Footnote 14 For more information on the representativeness of respondents in relation to the target population, see appendix A.

Bivariate Analyses

The hypotheses that we tested emerge from the literature review above. For example, we expect that a larger number of publications will be associated with a greater likelihood of being at a higher rank. We consider several different measures of research, teaching, and service. We also look at a variety of characteristics of one's current job—as well as background characteristics, such as where one received his or her PhD. Tables 1A and 1B show the predictor variables that we explored.

Table 1A Descriptive Statistics for Differences in Means across Ranks (among men only and among women only)

Asterisks in the assistant professor column indicate a significant difference between mean for the assistant professors compared with the associate professors (within gender). Asterisks in the full professor column indicate a significant difference between mean for the full professors compared with the associate professors (within gender).

Table 1B Percentage Differences across Ranks (among men and women separately)

The first row of table 1A shows the mean for total publications for the different ranks with men and women listed separately. The asterisks in table 1A reveal when the differences in means across ranks are statistically significant. Our measure of total productivity counts the number of articles, monographs, chapters, and edited books published to date in one's career. (Appendix B provides coding information for all variables.) The average on the measure of total productivity for assistant professors is 4.9, for associates it is 11.4, and for full professors it is 28.5. In terms of the individual components of our total productivity scale, assistant professors had published an average of 3.4 articles, associates had published 6.9 articles, and full professors had published 16.2 articles. Assistants had published on average of 0.3 monographs, associates had published 0.73, and full professors had published 2.3.

Women have a lower average number of article publications at every rank. Women also publish fewer monographs and book chapters than men at the associate and full professor levels. At the assistant professor rank, however, women publish more monographs and an equal number of book chapters compared with men.Footnote 15 For a discussion of reasons behind differential publication rates, see Hesli and Lee (Reference Hesli and Lee2011).

In terms of other research-related activities, among both men and women, those at higher ranks have more frequently reviewed book and article manuscripts, served on editorial boards, and received external grant awards (table 1A). In contrast, those at higher ranks attend conferences less frequently than those at lower ranks. Teaching loads are similar across ranks: assistants on average teach 4.5 undergraduate courses per year, associates teach 4.4, and full professors teach 4.0 undergraduate courses per year. Table 1A also reveals that one's overall resources tend to increase significantly as one moves up the academic ranks. Notice also that the mean age ranges from 37 to 58 years across the academic ranks.

In table 1B we report percentage differences across ranks for the categorical variables used in our analyses. Note, we do not see significant differences across ranks in the proportion whose work is primarily coauthored. Another interesting finding that corresponds with figures reported by the US Department of Education is that women are less likely to be married than men; this is most dramatically apparent at the level of full professor. Several other interesting contrasts are in tables 1A and 1B, but none of these comparisons includes controls for other relevant factors. Thus, we turn to our multivariate analyses.

Multivariate Analyses

For multivariate analyses, we use logistic regression models to examine the likelihood of being an associate professor as compared with an assistant professor and then to examine the likelihood of being a full professor as compared with an associate professor—based on demographic, institutional, and professional attributes.Footnote 16 Because these dependent variables are dichotomous, binary logit regression is used. These models estimate the log-odds of the higher rank occurring relative to the lower rank for each of the independent variables after controlling for the other variables in the model.Footnote 17

We tested the models with a reduced sample where we exclude from the analyses respondents who missed an answer to one or more questions (variables) in the model. We also tested the same models using imputed data and holding the number of cases in the analysis constant at 706 for the comparison between assistant and associate professors and at 823 for the comparison between associate and full professors. The purpose of using imputed estimates of missing responses is to increase the number of observations taken into consideration in the analysis. For example, out of 1,399 survey respondents, 141 did not identify the institution from which they received their PhD. A more significant missing value problem arises with the question: “In what year did you obtain your PhD degree?” Two-hundred and seventy-three people either did not answer or made a mistake when typing in a year. We did not feel comfortable simply dropping these 273 people (19.5% of all respondents) from all of our analyses. Therefore, we decided to use the multiple imputation Amelia II program to impute estimates of the missing responses on each of the independent variables used in the analysis (Honaker, King, and Blackwell Reference Honaker, King and Blackwell2011).Footnote 18 The results using multiple imputation are reported in the tables presented here.Footnote 19

We also note that we omitted some variables from the models that we originally considered because repeated preliminary testing revealed that these variables were not statistically significant in the academic rank models (given the other controls in the model). Omitted variables include whether the undergraduate major was political science; the type of undergraduate school (four-year private liberal arts college, private research university, flagship state university, or other state university); citizenship status; number of independent, honors, and senior projects supervised; frequency of committee membership; and whether one's work is generally sole-authored or co-authored. We excluded a few other variables because they were highly correlated with or represented a concept already included in the model with a different indicator. For example, we did not include the number of years in one's current position; rather we included responses to a question of whether one had been in his or her current position for more than seven years. We did not include frequency of conference attendance nor the number of grants awarded as these are highly correlated with the number of publications. We did not include number of graduate courses taught, as this is directly tied to whether one works in a PhD-granting, MA-granting, or bachelor-degree-only granting department. Each of the variables listed in this paragraph is included in tables 1A and 1B; readers can see what differences do exist (bivariately) across ranks on these measures.

We attempted to include all relevant variables as identified in the literature in our models. We do this so that when we test hypothesized relationships, such as that between gender and rank, we control for important predictors (such as age and the number of publications). This method of assessing the existence of particularistic criteria in the determination of academic rank is referred to as “sophisticated residualism” (Cole Reference Cole1979, 29, as quoted in Long and Fox Reference Long and Fox1995, 54). If sex or race differences remain significant after controlling for relevant variables, we have evidence of particularism. Cotter et al. (Reference Cotter, Hermsen, Ovadia and Vanneman2001) use a similar approach in evaluating the “glass ceiling effect,” which exists when gender or racial difference cannot be explained by other job-relevant characteristics of the employee.

In interpreting the multivariate analysis, we start with table 2. This table looks only at people in the rank of either assistant or associate professor, and models the likelihood of being an associate professor over an assistant professor.Footnote 20 Because promotion to associate professor occurs usually after five to seven years at the assistant professor rank, we include age as a control variable in all of the models: the older one is, the more likely he or she will be at a higher rank.Footnote 21 Related variables are entered into the logistic regression analysis in blocks to check the contributions of specific categories of predictors.

Table 2 Predicting Academic Rank: Factors Affecting the Likelihood of Being an Associate Professor in Contrast with an Assistant Professor (binary logistic models via multiple imputation)

Note.

*** p < 0.01,

** p < 0.05,

* p < 0.1

Model 2A includes demographic and family status variables, plus characteristics of one's graduate training. Note that women have a significantly lower likelihood of being an associate professor (compared with an assistant professor) than do men. Having an employed partner increases the likelihood of being an associate professor. Graduates from higher ranked PhD programs are more likely to be at the associate level, while more time spent in graduate school works against movement to a higher rank.Footnote 22 Each additional year spent earning the doctoral degree reduces the likelihood of being an associate professor by 14%.

We look next at model 2B in table 2. We still predict the likelihood of being an associate professor compared with an assistant, but we now have included characteristics of the job as well as demographic characteristics and information about one's graduate school experience. We continue to consider men and women in the same model. Later we split the sample and separately look at men and women. In this model, the factors that are found to be unrelated to difference in rank are as interesting as those factors that are, so we report both. Among the factors that describe the job, considering male and female professors together, those factors that do not affect the likelihood of being an associate professor are teaching load,Footnote 23 level of resources, whether one has a joint appointment, whether one works in a private versus a public institution, whether one works in a PhD, MA, or bachelors degree-granting department, or whether one works in a highly ranked department.Footnote 24 The factors that are related to an increased likelihood of being an associate professor over an assistant professor are being more involved in student advising, chairing more committees, and being released from teaching duties. These findings about advising and committee service seem sensible as many departments attempt to protect assistant professors from too much advising and committee service so that they will have more time for their research. Indeed, associate professors advise more and chair more committees than do assistant professors.

For the last model in table 2 (model 2C), we add factors that we define as professional characteristics. We see that among men and women together, and with the other controls in the model, subfield specialization is unrelated to the likelihood of being at the associate professor rank; perceptions of departmental influence, frequency of reviewing books or articles, and serving on editorial boards are also not significant. What is related to associate versus assistant professor rank is staying in the same position for at least seven years and research productivity. We included the question of length of time in one's current position to control for the expectation of promotion from the rank of assistant to the rank of associate professor after a certain amount of time passes. We might think of the total number of publications also as an important control variable, as we expect people to be promoted based on their publications. Those at the higher rank have indeed published more articles, chapters, and books.Footnote 25 Note that with the controls for time in position and number of publications in the model 2C, we still see significant differences between men and women in rank. The odds ratio associated with the coefficient for women in model 2C reveals that the likelihood of a woman being at the associate professor rank is 51% less than it is for a man. Stated differently: the chance of a female faculty member being at the associate professor rank is only 49% of the chance that a male faculty member is at associate rank.

Table 3 looks only at people in the rank of either associate or full professor and models the likelihood of being a full professor in contrast to an associate professor. A key finding here is that being a woman does not affect the likelihood of being at one rank rather than another. Besides the control for age, the only variable from the first set (model 3A) that influences the likelihood of being a full professor is the length of time that it took the candidate to complete his or her doctoral degree. Again, spending too much time in graduate school appears to hinder later promotion through the ranks.Footnote 26

Table 3 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professors Compared with Full Professors (binary logistic models via multiple imputation)

Note.

*** p < 0.01,

** p < 0.05,

* p < 0.1

We look at model 3B to evaluate whether characteristics of the job are significant predictors of rank when professional characteristics are not yet considered. We see that the likelihood of being at the rank of full professor is higher with more frequent committee service, less likely when working in an MA program, more likely in more highly ranked departments, and more likely with more resources and release from teaching.Footnote 27 Each of these significant factors from the second set of variables in model 3B, however, loses their significance when the professional variables are added to the equation (model 3C).

According to model 3C (table 3), which includes all possible controls, the likelihood of being a full professor (over an associate professor) is lower in a PhD-granting program (rather than a MA program or a bachelor's program [the excluded category]). In addition, the greater one's perceived influence in department decision making, the greater the likelihood that one is a full professor. As expected, more publications lead to a greater likelihood of being a full professor. More frequent service on editorial boards also correlates with a greater likelihood of being a full professor.Footnote 28 We note the significance of the coefficients associated with age and having been in the same position for at least seven years; we think of these more as control variables as we expect that a certain amount of time must be spent at the associate-professor rank before one can advance to the full-professor rank.

Hypotheses Tests with a Split Sample: Men and Women Examined Separately

Given the importance of gender in table 2, we divide the sample and test the models separately on tenure track women only and on tenure track men only. These tests are found in tables 4 and 5. Using this information, we explore whether the relationships between the predictors of promotion to associate professor or to full professor vary between men and women. Table 4 presents tests of a logit model of the probability of being in the associate professor category over the assistant professor category using a split sample. Let us compare model 4A (for men) with model 4B (for women). These two models include all predictor variables except the professional variables. We remind readers that these models include a control for age, which is the best predictor of academic rank. We find that the following variables are not predictive of rank for either men or women: being married or partnered, number of children, whether one's partner is employed, and the rank of the program where one received his or her doctoral degree.Footnote 29 In other words, when we consider men and women separately, the likelihood of being tenured does not improve for either men or women based on whether one was trained by a top-ranked department.

Table 4 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professor Compared with Assistant Professor (spilt sample)

Note.

*** p < 0.01,

** p < 0.05,

* p < 0.1

Table 5 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professors Compared with Full Professors (Split sample)

Note.

*** p < 0.01,

** p < 0.05,

* p < 0.1

With regard to characteristics of the job, the following variables do not differentiate between the likelihood of being an assistant or an associate professor: teaching load, having a joint appointment, working at a private school, being in a PhD or an MA program, and amount of resources.Footnote 30 Noteworthy is that higher levels of advising and more frequent committee service (as chair) are significantly associated with being in the associate professor category for both men and women. Again, we observe that many departments make a concerted effort to reduce the amount of advising and committee service among assistant professors, so we are not arguing that more of either one will help one to become an associate professor. What we are saying is that the job is different with regard to advising and committee service when one is no longer an assistant professor.Footnote 31 Another finding that holds for both men and women concerns teaching release: the more courses from which one has been released, the more likely one is to be in the associate professor category.

Some variables' effects are different for female faculty as compared to male faculty.Footnote 32 For example, minority men are less likely than nonminority men to be in the associate professor rank compared with assistant professor. Among female faculty, taking more time to complete the PhD reduces the likelihood of being an associate professor over an assistant professor. Another difference between men and women that we see in this set is that among women only, a lower ranking of one's current department is associated with a greater likelihood of being an associate professor (rather than an assistant).

To finish our analysis of the factors related to the likelihood of being an associate professor over an assistant professor, we look at models 4C (for men) and 4D (for women) in table 4. These models include the professional characteristics of the faculty members. The difference that we observe between men and women is that perceptions of departmental influence and frequency of reviewing a book are related to the associate professor rank for women, but not for men.Footnote 33 This means that with the controls in models 4C and 4D, only female associate professors have reviewed more books and report more influence over department decision-making than female assistant professors.

Rather shockingly, the total number of publications is not related to rank (assistant to associate) for women, although the number of publications is significantly related to the rank of men. This is the only place where we find academic rank to be unrelated to publication productivity.Footnote 34 We need to highlight this substantively significant and uncomfortable finding . The lack of significance associated with the coefficient for total publications in the model for women (model 4D) means that no discernible relationship exists among women between number of publications and the likelihood of being in the rank of associate professor (over the rank of assistant professor)—which is troubling because the number of publications should be a predictor of rank. Publications, along with teaching and service, are supposed to be the criteria used to evaluate candidates at promotion time.Footnote 35 We remind readers that differences in the likelihood ratios that are significant for women and men suggest that the predictors of rank are different for men than for women. We return to this finding later in our discussion.

Now, we turn to differences between the rank of associate and full professor based on separate analyses for men and women (table 5). According to model 5B, none of the demographic or graduate program variables are useful for differentiating between associate and full-professor rank among women (except, of course, age). Among men (model 5A), however, minorities are less well represented at the full-professor rank, and taking longer to complete the doctoral degree negatively affects the likelihood of being in the full-professor rank. In addition, among women, none of the characteristics of one's current job (the second set of variables) are different for associate as compared with full professors. Among men, however, full professors are more likely to chair more committees, and less likely to be employed in a PhD or MA department. Among men only, those with more resources and course releases are more likely to be full professors than associate professors.

Turning to the professional variables (models 5C and 5D), subfield is again unrelated to being in a higher rank for both men and women. A larger number of total publications is related to full-professor rank for both men and women. Interestingly, the size of the effect of the number of publications on promotion to full professor is larger for women than for men. Among men only, more frequent service on editorial boards is related to the higher rank. To summarize, among women, it appears that the only factor that differentiates between associate and full professors is total publications (plus our controls for age and time in position).Footnote 36 Thus, standard predictors of full-professor rank, such as the type of institution where one is employed, appear to work better to explain promotion for men than for women. Here, readers should note that many differences across ranks are apparent in a bivariate sense (see tables 1A and 1B). Without controls for such important predictors as age and number of years in one's current position, we do know that both female and male full professors have reviewed more books and have served on more editorial boards than have female and male associate professors respectively (table 1A).

DISCUSSION

The analyses presented here raise some serious issues. One concern is the lower likelihood of women as compared to men of being in the associate as compared to the assistant professor rank. This means a significant advantage for men in the probability of becoming an associate professor, which usually includes tenure. Despite holding constant a variety of relevant factors such as age and number of publications, this difference between men and women in rank attainment remains significant. Stated another way, despite detailed controls for personal attributes, institutional characteristics, and professional qualifications, women are underrepresented among the tenured members of the political science profession. The evidence of the lower likelihood of women being at the associate professor rank presented here, as well as the APSA and NSF figures for numbers of women within different academic ranks of political science, provide strong evidence that women are indeed falling out of the profession around tenure time. This finding may actually represent deterioration within our profession. Data gathered around the end of the 20th century indicated that women were gaining tenure at rates relatively similar to men (Hesli and Burrell Reference Hesli and Burrell1995; McBride Stetson et al. Reference McBride Stetson, Wall, Blair, Guy, Fairchild, Canon, Brown, Women and Association1990; Van Assendelft, Gunther-Canada, and Dolan Reference Van Assendelft, Gunther-Canada and Dolan2001).

In contrast to our finding that female faculty have a lower likelihood of being at the rank of associate professor, we find no significant difference between men and women in the likelihood of achieving full-professor status after having become an associate professor. Those women who survive the tenure process are as likely as men (given relevant controls) to move up the academic ladder to full professor. This finding fits comfortably with the notion that “upon entering the rank of associate professor, men and women are more similar than they were when entering the rank of assistant professor” (Long, Allison, and McGinnis Reference Long, Allison and McGinnis1993, 715).Footnote 37 This conclusion also indicates that with regard to promotion to full professor, the political science profession has not changed that much during the last decade and a half. Studying the political science profession in the mid-1990s, Hesli and Burrell (Reference Hesli and Burrell1995) reported that when women apply for full professorship they are likely to be promoted at the same rates as men (103). Remarkably, our findings about the lower likelihood of women gaining tenure—but that female survivors of the tenure process fair similarly to men in later promotion decisions—appear similar to those published more than 30 years ago: Farber (Reference Farber1977, 203–4) found that among younger age cohorts, “females received significantly fewer rank promotions”—but that if a woman can continue in academia beyond the younger cohort, through the middle and into the older age cohort, she will receive rank promotions on par with her male counterparts.

Contrary to expectations based on life-cycle theories, being married or partnered and/or the number of children does not generally affect promotion through the ranks.Footnote 38 The number of children is not significantly different when full professors are compared with associate professors. The number of children is positively correlated with age, so we cannot fully separate the effects of these two variables on promotion to associate professor. We note that other recent research has revealed differential patterns for men and women: “neither parenting nor marriage significantly affects the rate of promotion to tenure for women. However, for men, being in a marriage to a spouse without a professional degree significantly improves the odds of transitioning to tenure” (Morrison, Rudd, and Nerad Reference Morrison, Rudd and Nerad2011, 545). Morrison and colleagues (2011, 550) mention the possibility of a selection effect: “only women who feel secure enough in their career choose to have children and therefore advance at a competitive rate.” We need to be careful in interpreting findings about the effects of children on career advancement, as women with heavy family responsibilities may have already left academia. Our panel study (referenced later) will allow us to address directly the possibility of such a phenomenon.

Another critical finding that we note with some consternation is that among women, the probability of being an associate professor over an assistant professor is unrelated (given other controls) to the total number of publications (model 4D). This confirms that the promotion process at this level involves different dynamics for men as compared with women. In all of our other models, we found, as we would expect, that the total number of publications is an important predictor for both men and women of the likelihood of being promoted from assistant to associate professor rank and from associate to full professor. Thus, the mantra of “publish or perish” is substantiated by this research with the notable exception of the movement of women from assistant to associate professor rank. The obvious question is this: why is it that the number of publications for women has no significant effect on their promotion from assistant to associate professor?

Some other findings that tie into the existing literature include the lack of significant differences in rank attainment based on the quality of the graduate program. It is often assumed that receipt of a doctoral degree from a highly ranked department improves one's career prospects. However, we find that PhD-program quality tends not to be related to the academic rank achieved. We do observe a positive correlation between PhD-program rank and current employment in a PhD-granting department. Also, the higher the ranking of the department where one received his or her doctorate, the higher the ranking of the department where one is currently employed. We find that those who take longer to earn their PhD are less likely to attain higher rank.

Rather surprisingly, we see little difference in undergraduate teaching loads across the ranks. We are wary of collinearity associated with this variable. The number of undergraduate courses taught tends to be lower in higher ranked departments. Similarly, the more undergraduate courses taught, the fewer overall resources. More teaching at the undergraduate level is also correlated with fewer total publications and a lower frequency of reviewing articles.

We do see significantly higher levels of advising at the associate and full-professor levels as compared with assistant professors. In addition, although we often think of teaching release as something offered to assistant professors so that they can concentrate on the research, this survey reveals that the higher one's rank attainment, the more likely one is to be released from teaching responsibilities. More resources are associated with higher ranked departments of current employment.

We also note that among men, being a minority decreases the odds of being a full professor over an associate professor. This finding remains significant even with all the controls included in our most comprehensive predictive model. A possible explanation for this finding comes from Tierney and Bensimon (Reference Tierney and Bensimon1996) who conclude, “institutional structures, policies, and practices that are intended to be gender- and race-neutral may be creating a working environment that is unsupportive, patronizing and even hostile” (as quoted in Perna Reference Perna2001, 563). A limitation of this study is that because the number of self-identified members of a minority ethnic or racial group is so small, we cannot reasonably study differences between, for example, African Americans, Asian Americans, or Hispanic Americans. In fact, our aggregation may mask differences among these groups. In addition, as Ginther and Hayes (Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 68) argue, we cannot conclude that discrimination is the underlying cause of gender or identity differences in promotion unless we are sure that we have controlled for all relevant factors —and we cannot be sure of this. For example, we have not controlled for quality of teaching —if indeed this can be reliably measured. The analyses presented here are also limited to cross-sectional data, which we have used to study a longitudinal promotion process. An implication is that the variables describe characteristics of respondents at one time point. To remedy this, we have collected the data for the first stage of a panel study and will report the results of longitudinal research after the second stage of the panel study is conducted. We also acknowledge the possibility of error in self-reports especially on retrospective measures in surveys. We believe, however, that the benefits of survey research outweigh the problems.

Given the lack of statistical significance for many predictors of higher academic rank among women in our multivariate models, in a future article, we will delve more deeply into the “climate” evaluations provided by the survey respondents. The “micro” climate of each scholar's home department could affect promotion decisions. “Climate” is a factor when women perceive or experience a climate different from the climate experienced by men. One argument “suggests that if affirmative action for women is applied in the admission process to PhD programs and/or at the hiring stage, but not at the tenure stage, then this factor might help explain why fewer women pass the tenure hurdle” (Ginther and Kahn Reference Ginther and Kahn2004, 212). If white men resent what they perceive as special benefits given to women and minorities in the hiring process, a backlash may occur when these colleagues are called on to vote in tenure decisions.

Additional research is needed on several questions raised by this report. If gender or race does factor into the promotion process, then development programs are still needed to overcome barriers to career advancement within the political science profession. We conclude with our two most perplexing questions: Why are women less likely than men to be associate as compared to assistant professors? Why is it that the number of publications for women appears to have no effect on their likelihood of being an associate professor over an assistant professor? Future research will seek answers to these important questions.

ACKNOWLEDGMENT

The survey on which the analyses reported herein are based was funded by the American Political Science Association. We are indebted to APSA executive director Michael Brintnall for his support, to APSA director of member services and development Sean Twombly for his assistance, and to APSA director of institutional programs Jennifer Segal Diascro for her comments. An earlier version of this paper was presented at the American Political Science Association Annual Meeting, Seattle, September 2011.

APPENDIX A: Survey Methodology

Questionnaire Design

In 2005, the APSA Committee on the Status of Women in the Profession (CSWP) proposed to the president of APSA that the association conduct research associated with the recommendations that emerged from the March 2004 Workshop on Women's Advancement in Political Science organized by Michael Brintnall and Linda Lopez (American Political Science Association), Susan Clarke (University of Colorado, Boulder), and Leonie Huddy (Stony Brook University). After the research proposal was approved, the CSWP used questionnaires that had been used in research published by Hesli and Burrell (Reference Hesli and Burrell1995), Hesli, Fink, and Duffy (Reference Hesli, Fink and Duffy2003) and Hesli, DeLaat, Youde, Mendez, and Lee (2006) to develop a new survey instrument. Additional questions were added from questionnaires developed by the National Research Council and the University of Michigan's fall 2001 Survey of Academic Climate and Activities, which was created for an NSF ADVANCE project. The following reports were also used to help generate questions.

Blau, F. 2002. Report of the Committee on the Status of Women in the Economics Profession. American Economic Review 92: 516–20.

Commission on Professionals in Science and Technology (CPST). 2000. Professional Women & Minorities: A Total Human Resource Data Compendium, 13th edition. Washington, DC: CPST.

Creamer, Elizabeth. 1998. Assessing Faculty Publication Productivity: Issues of Equity. ASHE-ERIC Higher Education Report 26 (2). Washington, DC: The George Washington University.

Fox, Mary Frank. 1995. “Women and Scientific Careers.” In Handbook of Science and Technology Studies, eds. S. Jasanoff, J. Markle, J. Petersen, and T. Pinch, 205–223. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Fox, Mary Frank. 1998. “Women in Science and Engineering: Theory, Practice, and Policy in Programs.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 24 (Autumn): 201–23.

Sarkee, Meredith Reid, and Nancy E. McGlen. 1992. Confronting Barriers: The Status of Women in Political Science. Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 12 (4): 43–86.

A draft of the questionnaire was circulated to the members of the APSA status committees. The questionnaire was revised and expanded to address the concerns of the members of the status committees. The instrument was pilot tested by distributing it to all political science faculty members at one research university and at one private four-year college. The feedback from the pilot test was used to make further revisions in the questionnaire.

Sample Selection

We used as our target population the names contained within the APSA “faculty” file. We used this file of 11,559 names to create a sample population file of size 5,179 names. The original “faculty” file was stratified by department size. To ensure the adequate representation of faculty members from medium and small size schools we over-sampled from these. Names were selected randomly from the “faculty” file for the “sample” file.

Survey Procedure

Using e-mail addresses, all persons in the sample file were sent a letter of invitation to participate in the study from the executive director and the president of the APSA. Incorrect e-mail addresses (addresses that bounced back) were replaced with random selections from the “faculty” file. These persons were also mailed an invitation letter. The cleaned “survey” file was sent to the Survey Research Center at the Pennsylvania State University (SRC).

Individuals in the sample were sent an e-mail from SRC inviting them to participate in the survey. This invitation included a link to the web-based survey containing a unique identifier for each potential participant. Only one completed survey was allowed for each identifier. The initial invitation was e-mailed to respondents on August 27, 2009. Follow-up reminders were sent to nonresponders on September 10, 2009, September 24, 2009, October 8, 2009, and October 29, 2009. From among the 5,179 original addresses, 1,399 completed the survey (252 invalid addresses, 105 refusals, and 3,423 nonrespondents).

Among the total set of respondents, 68% are men and 32% are women. According APSA data, the percent of women in the population from which we drew the sample (all political science faculty members in the United States) was 28% (in 2009). Table A1 shows the percent of survey respondents at each rank alongside of the percent of faculty members in each rank throughout the United States according to APSA data for 2009. With regard to respondents' gender, among assistant professors, 45% were women; among associates, 28% were women; and among full professors, 24% were women. The corresponding numbers for the population as a whole are in the table A1.

Table A1 Table A1: Survey Respondents and the Population

APPENDIX B: Variables Included

Dependent Variable: Faculty Rank: “Title of your primary current appointment”

We created an ordinal variable using the following coding: 1 (instructors, lecturers, postdocs, and fellows), 2 (assistant professors), 3 (associate professors) and 4 (full professors, emeritus, and administrative positions).

Independent Variables:

Female: “What is your gender? a. Male, b. Female, c. Transgender” The dummy variable equals 1 if the response is b.

Minority: “Do you identify yourself as a member of an ethnic and racial minority group? a. Yes, b. No, c. Don't know” The dummy variable equals 1 if the response is a.

Married: “What is your personal status? a. Never married, b. Married (first time), c. Married (second or third time), d. Member of an unmarried opposite or same-sex partnership, e. Separated/divorced, f. Widowed” The dummy variable equals 1 if the response is b, c, or d.

Number of Children: “Do you or a spouse/partner of yours have any children? a. Yes (If yes, how many?), b. No” An interaction variable between a dummy for having children (response a.) and the number of children specified.

Number of years to complete PhD: Two questions: “In what year did you begin work on your PhD?' and “In what year did you obtain your degree?” The reported variable is the year of getting PhD degree minus the year of beginning the degree program.

PhD Program Rank: Question: “From which university did you obtain your degree?” The program is ranked based on Schmidt and Chingos (Reference Schmidt and Chingos2007); Top 25 (1), Top 26–50 (2), Top 51–75 (3), Top 76–86 (4), and Unranked (5). Foreign degrees and degrees from majors other than political science were set as missing. Then the score is reversed so that higher numbers represent higher ranked programs.

Teaching Load: “During the past five years, what is your typical teaching load each year? (If in your current position for less than five years, base this on the period since your appointment.)

________Number of undergraduate courses”

Number of Committees Chaired: “In a typical year during the past five years, how many committees do you chair?”

Amount of Student Advising: “For how many of each of the following types of individuals do you currently serve as official advisor? ___Undergraduates, ___MA students, ___PhD students, ____postdocs”

The variable was generated by following steps. First, dummy variables were created to represent higher than average advising for each student group. For example, the respondent would receive a “1” on undergraduate advising if their reported number of undergraduate students advised was higher than the overall mean for that question. The same coding rule was applied to other student groups such as MA students, doctoral students and postdocs. Next we counted the overall number of 1's from those four dummies.

Count of Overall Resources: “Have you received any of the following resources as a result of your own negotiations, the terms of an award, or as part of an offer by the university, since your initial contract at your current position? If so, please check all that apply.”

Using the count command, we added up the total number of checks for all rows and all columns.

PhD. Program: “Type of department where you are employed: a. PhD granting program, b. MA granting program, c. Department within a 4-year college, d. Department within a 2-year college, e. Other academic unit (specify)” The dummy variable equals 1 if the response is a.

MA Program: Same question as above, with the dummy variable equals to 1 if the response is b.

Private Institution: “Is this a public or a private institution? a. Public, b. Private” The dummy variable equals to 1 if the response is b.

Subfield Dummies: “Which of the following best describes your current primary field of teaching and research? a. American, b. Comparative, c. International Relations, d. Theory, e. Methods, f. Other (please specify)”

American subfield equals 1 if the response is a.

Comparative Subfield equals 1 if the response is b.

IR Subfield equals 1 if the response is c.

Theory Subfield equals 1 if the response is d.

More than 7 years in current position: “Have you been in your current position more than 7 years?”

Current Program Ranking: A ranking of the department where the respondent is currently working. The program is ranked based on Schmidt and Chingos (Reference Schmidt and Chingos2007); Top 25 (1), Top 26–50 (2), Top 51–75 (3), Top 76–86 (4), and Unranked (5). Then the score is reversed so that higher numbers represent higher ranked department.

Total Productivity: Question: “For your entire career, please give your best estimate of the number you have produced or have been awarded for each of the following.

______ number of articles published in referred academic or professional journals

______ number of monographs (books) published

______ number of books edited

______ number of book chapters published”

All missing values of articles, monographs, edited books, and book chapters are set to zero, then we took a logarithmic transformation of the sum of these items plus one.

Departmental Influence: A count of responses b and c for the following questions:

“For each item, please check the box that best corresponds to how much influence you feel you have over the following matters in your department(s). (a. Less influence than I would like, b. About the right amount of influence, c. More influence than may be appropriate)

  1. 46. Department curriculum decisions

  2. 47. Size of salary increases I receive

  3. 48. Selecting new students (graduate or undergraduate)

  4. 49. Selecting new faculty members to be hired

  5. 50. Determining who gets tenure

  6. 51. Selecting the next unit head”

Footnotes

1 Different disciplines have distinct normative and procedural practices in the promotion and tenure process (Braxton and Hargens Reference Braxton, Hargens and Smart1996) and the average time to promotion varies by discipline (Ornstein et al. Reference Ornstein, Stewart and Drakich2007, 9).

2 Table 214, National Center for Education Statistics (http://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d10/tables/dt10_214.asp?referrer=report)

3 Table 256.

5 Table 256.

7 See http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/srvydoctorates/. Among US citizens and permanent citizens, 26% of political science doctorate recipients were of a non-white race or ethnicity (http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/nsf11306/appendix/excel/tab22.xls).

8 Based on the Survey of Doctorate Recipients (SDR), “in 2001, political science had a lower percentage female who are tenured (23 percent) than social science disciplines excluding political science (29 percent) and sociology and anthropology (35 percent)” (Ginther Reference Ginther2004, 4).

9 See U.S. Doctorates in the 20th Century.

10 Note that these studies generally include controls for productivity, demographics, and employer characteristics.

11 Actually, it is immobility (staying in the same position for several years) that has been linked to promotion (Farber Reference Farber1977, 203; Ginther and Hayes Reference Ginther and Hayes2003, 50).

12 Using 2004 figures, 53% of female faculty and 47% of male faculty are single (without dependent children), 59% of female faculty and 41% of male faculty are single with dependent children, 41% of female faculty and 59% of male faculty are married without dependent children, and 37% of female faculty and 64% of male faculty are married with dependent children. Source: US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, 2004 National Study of Postsecondary Faculty (NSOPF:04).

13 Results for the same analyses reported herein for all respondents (including those not in a tenure track position) are available from the authors.

14 Two percent were instructors, lecturers, postdocs or fellows; and we lack information on rank for 6.7% of respondents. The full professor group includes a few emeriti professors.

15 Our measure of total productivity simply adds the number of publications in each category (e.g., books, articles, chapters). Books are not weighted more heavily than articles.

16 For a similar use of logistic regression models for the study of faculty promotion, see Fang et al. (Reference Fang, Moy, Colburn and Hurley2000), Ward (Reference Ward2001), and Perna (Reference Perna2001 and Reference Perna2005).

17 The odds-ratio represents the change in the odds of holding the higher rank relative to the lower rank associated with a one-unit change in a particular independent variable. An odds-ratio greater than one represents an increase in the likelihood of being at the higher rank, while an odds-ratio less than one represents a decrease in the likelihood.

18 For multiple imputation work, we used Amelia II version 1.5-2 developed by Honaker, King, and Blackwell (Reference Honaker, King and Blackwell2011). We used the stand alone program of AmeliaView in the Windows environment, downloadable from the developers' website at http://gking.harvard.edu/amelia/. We did not impute any missing values in our dependent variable (i.e., academic promotion); we only imputed the set of explanatory variables. Our data set contains several ordinal and nominal variables and the Amelia II allows users to classify those variables as having noncontinuous distributions according to their characteristics. In addition, we took a (natural) logarithm transformation to any heavily skewed variables or variables with outliers to normalize its distribution. Also, if any variable needed to be bounded by realistically possible numbers (e.g., year of getting PhD degree), we assigned bounds (maximum and minimum values) to those variables using their observed summary statistics. Finally, after AmeliaView produced 5 multiply imputed data sets in a STATA format (.dta), we used Clarify (Tomz, Wittenberg, and King Reference Tomz, Wittenberg and King2003) for data analysis to combine the results.

19 The results for the same analysis based on the smaller number of cases (respondents with missing values excluded) are available from the authors.

20 We also fitted the reported models herein using alternative estimation strategies such as ordered logistic regression and multinomial logistic regression (using associate faculty as a reference category), although we eventually chose to report the results from the binary logistic models. These two alternative methods yielded very similar estimation results across different prediction models to the binary logistic models. We chose to adopt the binary over the ordered logistic models because the former model is more suitable for evaluating which specific sets of variables are significant predictors for a different rank-ladder (i.e., assistant to associate vs. associate to full promotion). Also, we prefer the binary model to the multinomial one since we wanted to avoid reporting negative signs in regression outcomes for positive predictors to the promotion equation (associate to full professor) as we use the associate group as a reference category in the multinomial logistic regressions.

21 We could also have controlled for the year at which the doctoral degree was granted (as this more directly accounts for years of experience), but age and year of degree are highly correlated and we have fewer missing responses on the age variable.

22 Our ranking of PhD programs has five categories: graduates of one of the top 25 departments (Tier I), compared with graduates from one of the departments ranked in the top 26–50 (Tier II), compared with Tier III, Tier IV, and unranked departments using the Schmidt and Chingos ranking (2007).

23 We asked respondents to report their typical teaching load each year (during the past five years). We include in the analysis the number of undergraduate courses only, as graduate courses are only taught by those in PhD or MA granting departments.

24 Bivariately, more resources are correlated with being an associate rather than an assistant professor (table 1A).

25 Looking at bivariate relationships (see table 1A), associates compared to assistants have reviewed more books and have served on more editorial boards. These factors do not emerge as significant in Model 2C in part because they are correlated with current department rank and with total productivity.

26 Bivariately, the ranking of one's graduate program does affect promotion from associate to full-professor rank (table 1A).

27 Bivariately, being at the full-professor rank is more likely in PhD programs (for women only). We note that current department rank and PhD program are highly correlated.

28 Bivariately, reviewing more books (for men and women) and more articles (for women only) are also associated with being a full professor (table 1A).

29 Bivariately, having more children is related to higher rank (assistant compared with associate) for both men and women. With the control for age, however, this factor is not significant. Among women only, associate professors are more likely than assistant professor to be married or partnered.

30 Bivariately, the amount of resources is higher for associate than assistant professors, and among women, assistant professors are more likely than associate professors to be employed in a PhD-granting department.

31 When the professional variables are added (models 4C and 4D), the coefficient for chairing committees is no longer significant for women, although it remains significant for men. Thus, there is a gender difference in the relationship between chairing committees and rank.

32 Ginther and Hayes (Reference Ginther and Hayes1999; Reference Ginther and Hayes2003) similarly found significantly different estimates when their models were estimated separately for men and women.

33 Bivariately, the frequency of reviewing books is different for both men and women when associate professors are compared with assistant professors. We remind the reader that these are highly correlated with total publications.

34 When female associate professors are compared with female assistant professors bivariately, associates do have significantly more publications than assistants.

35 The total number of publications is not statistically significant at any level in the females-only model that accounts for their rank promotion from assistant to associate professor; while the publications variable is consistently a strong and significant predictor of academic rank across the other estimation models. Some might question whether this nonsignificance of the publication record variable in model 4D is an artifact of a relatively small number of observations created by splitting the sample, but, in fact, the same variable is statistically significant (at p < 0.1) in the other split model (female faculty promotion from associate to full), although the former sample (assistant to associate, 264) has more cases than the latter (associate to full, 228).

36 Bivariately for women, reviewing articles and serving on editorial boards are also more frequent among full professors as compared with associate professors. Bivariately, female full professors are also more likely than female associate professors to be employed in a PhD-granting department and also more likely to be employed in a higher ranked department. Female full professors teach fewer classes than female associate professors. Female full professors are also less likely to be married than female associate professors. Bivariately, for males, full professors are less likely than associate professors to specialize in IR. Also among males only, coming from a more highly ranked PhD program is bivariately related to being a full professor.

37 This “similarity” could include similarity in the type of research conducted. Earlier in their careers, women may be exploring somewhat different research questions; those who are tenured may have been socialized into a male-oriented research paradigm, or have already selected into this prior to tenure.

38 The exception is among men only, being married increases the odds of being a full professor compared to an associate professor.

References

Acker, S., and Armenti, C.. 2004. “Sleepless in Academe.” Gender and Education 16 (1): 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, M., and Castleman, T.. 2001. “Fighting the Pipeline Fallacy.” In Gender and the Restructured University: Changing Management and Culture in Higher Education, ed. Brooks, A. & Mackinnon, A., 151–65. Buckingham, England: The Society for Research into Higher Education and Open University Press.Google Scholar
Becker, G. S. 1985. “Human Capital, Effort, and the Sexual Division of LaborJournal of Labor Economics 3: S33S58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, G. S. 1993. Human Capital. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, Sharon R., and Rhoton, Laura A.. 2011. “Women Professionals' Gender Strategies.” In Handbook of Gender, Work and Organization, ed. Jeans, E. L., Knights, D., & Martin, P.Y., 245–62. United Kingdom: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Bonilla-Silva, E. 1997. “Rethinking Racism: Toward a Structural Interpretation.” American Sociological Review 62: 465–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braxton, John M., and Hargens, Lowell L.. 1996. “Variation among Academic Disciplines: Analytical Frameworks and Research.” In Higher Education: Handbook of Theory and Research, ed. Smart, John C., Volume XI, 146.Google Scholar
Cole, J. R. 1979. Fair Science: Women in the Scientific Community. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Cotter, D.A., Hermsen, J. M., Ovadia, S., and Vanneman, R.. 2001. “The Glass Ceiling Effect.” Social Forces 80 (2): 655–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dey, E. L. 1994. “Dimensions of Faculty Stress: A Recent Survey.” Review of Higher Education 17: 305–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiTomaso, Nancy, Post, Corinne, Smith, D. Randall, Farris, George F., and Cordero, Rene. 2007. “Effects of Structural Position on Allocation and Evaluation Decisions for Scientists and Engineers in Industrial R&D.” Administrative Science Quarterly 52 (2): 175207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doering, Richard. 1972. “Publish or Perish: Book Productivity and Academic Rank at Twenty-Six Elite Universities.” The American Sociologist 7 (9): 1113.Google Scholar
England, P., Farkas, G., Kilbourne, B. S., and Dou, T.. 1988. “Explaining Occupational Sex Segregation and Wages: Findings from a Model with Fixed Effects.” American Sociological Review 53: 544–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eveline, Joan. 2004. Ivory Basement Leadership: Power and Invisibility in the Changing University. Crawley, WA: University of Western Australia Press.Google Scholar
Fang, D., Moy, E., Colburn, L., and Hurley, J.. 2000. “Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Faculty Promotion in Academic Medicine.” The Journal of the American Medical Association 284 (9): 1085–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farber, Stephen. 1977. “The Earnings and Promotion of Women Faculty: Comment.” The American Economic Review 67 (2): 199206.Google Scholar
Feagin, Joe R., and Feagin, Clairece B.. 1986. Discrimination American Style: Institutional Racism and Sexism. Malabar, FL: R. E. Krieger.Google Scholar
Ginther, Donna K. 2004. “Gender Differences in Salary and Promotion in Political Science.” Paper prepared for presentation at the American Political Science Association Annual Meeting in Chicago.Google Scholar
Ginther, Donna K., and Hayes, Kathy J.. 1999. “Gender Differences in Salary and Promotion in the Humanities.” The American Economic Review 89 (2): 397402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginther, Donna K., and Hayes, Kathy J.. 2003. Gender Differences in Salary and Promotion for Faculty in the Humanities 1977–95. Journal of Human Resources 38 (1): 3473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginther, Donna K., and Kahn, Shulamit. 2004. “Women in Economics: Moving Up or Falling Off the Academic Career Ladder?The Journal of Economic Perspectives 18 (3): 193214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilman, Madeline E. 2001. “Description and Prescription: How Gender Stereotypes Prevent Women's Ascent Up the Organizational Ladder.” Journal of Social Issues 57 (4): 657–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesli, Vicki, and Burrell, Barbara. 1995. “Faculty Rank among Political Scientists and Reports on the Academic Environment: The Differential Impact of Gender on Observed Patterns.” PS: Political Science and Politics 28 (2): 101–11.Google Scholar
Hesli, Vicki, Fink, Evelyn C., and Duffy, Diane. 2003. “The Role of Faculty in Creating Student Experience: Survey Results from the Midwest Region, Part II.” PS: Political Science and Politics 36 (4): 801–04.Google Scholar
Hesli, Vicki, and Lee, Jae Mook. 2011. “Faculty Research Productivity: Why Do Some of Our Colleagues Publish More Than Others?PS: Political Science and Politics 44 (2): 393408.Google Scholar
Honaker, J., King, Gary, and Blackwell, M.. 2011. Amelia II: A Program for Missing Data. http://r.iq.harvard.edu/docs/amelia/amelia.pdf. Accessed on July 18, 2011.Google Scholar
Jackson, Jerlando F. L., and O'Callaghan, Elizabeth M.. 2009. “What Do We Know about Glass Ceiling Effects? A Taxonomy and Critical Review to Inform Higher Education Research.” Research in Higher Education 50: 460–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J. A. 2004. “The Faculty Time Divide.” Sociological Forum 19 (1): 327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J. A., and Winslow, S.E.. 2004a. “The Academic Life Course, Time Pressures, and Gender Inequality.” Community Work and Family 7 (2): 143–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J. A., and Winslow, S.E.. 2004b. “Overworked Faculty: Job Stresses and Family Demands.” Annals: American Academy of Political and Social Science 596: 104–29.Google Scholar
Johnson, G. E., and Stafford, F. P.. 1974. “The Earnings and Promotion of Women Faculty.” American Economic Review 64 (6): 888903.Google Scholar
Kahn, Shulamit. 1993. “Gender Differences in Academic Career Paths of Economists.” American Economic Review 83 (2): 5256.Google Scholar
Kahn, Shulamit. 1995. “Women in the Economics Profession.” Journal of Economic Perspectives 9 (4): 193205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, David A. 1973. “Faculty Salaries, Promotions, and Productivity at a Large University.” The American Economic Review 63 (3): 469–77.Google Scholar
Krefting, Linda A. 2003. “Intertwined Discourses of Merit and Gender: Evidence from Academic Employment in the USA.” Gender, Work & Organization 10 (2): 260–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulis, S., and Sicotte, D.. 2002. “Women Scientists in Academia: Geographically Constrained to Big Cities, College Clusters, or the Coasts?Research in Higher Education 43 (1): 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Sharon M. 2002. “Do Asian American Faculty Face a Glass Ceiling in Higher Education?American Educational Research Journal 39 (3): 695724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, L.S. 1998. Scaling the Ivory Tower: Merit and Its Limits in Academic Careers, 2nd ed. New Brunswich, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Lewis, Lionel S. 1967. “Publish or Perish: Some Comments on a Hyperbole.” Journal of Higher Education 38 (2): 8589.Google Scholar
Lin, N. 2001. Social Capital: A Theory of Social Structure and Action. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. S. 2001. From Scarcity to Visibility: Gender Differences in the Careers of Doctoral Scientists and Engineers. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Long, J. Scott, Allison, Paul D., and McGinnis, Robert. 1993. “Rank Advancement in Academic Careers: Sex Differences and the Effects of Productivity.” American Sociological Review 58: 703–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. S., and Fox, M. F.. 1995. “Scientific Careers: Universalism and Particularism.” Annual Review of Sociology 21: 4571.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, M. A., and Goulden, M.. 2002. “Do Babies Matter: The Effect of Family Formation on the Lifelong Careers of Academic Men and Women.” Academe 88 (6): 2128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mason, M. A., and Goulden, M.. 2004. “Marriage and Baby Blues: Redefining Gender Equity in the Academy.” Annals: American Academy of Political and Social Science 596: 87103.Google Scholar
McBride Stetson, Dorothy, Wall, Diane, Blair, Diane, Guy, Mary Ellen, Fairchild, Erika, Canon, David, Brown, Cheryl, and Women, Committee on the Status of, Association, Southern Political Science. 1990. “The Status of Women in PhD Department.” PS: Political Science and Politics 23 (1): 8286.Google Scholar
McDowell, John M., Singell, Larry D. Jr., and Stater, Mark. 2006. “Two to Tango: Gender Differences in the Decisions to Publish and CoAuthor.” Economic Inquiry 44 (1): 153168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowell, John M., Singell, Larry D. Jr., and Ziliak, James P.. 1999a. “Cracks in the Glass Ceiling: Gender and Promotion in the Economics Profession.” American Economic Review 89 (2): 392–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowell, John M., Singell, Larry D. Jr., and Ziliak, James P.. 1999b. “Gender and Promotion in the Economics Profession.” Industrial and Labor Relations Review 54 (2): 224–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowell, John M., and Smith, Janet Kiholm. 1992. “The Effect of Gender Sorting on the Propensity to Coauthor.” Economic Inquiry 30: 6882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merton, Robert. [1942] 1973. “The Normative Structure of Science.” In The Sociology of Science, 267–78. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Milem, J. F., Sherlin, J., and Irwin, L.. 2001. “The Importance of Collegial Networks to College and University Faculty.” In Working Equal: Academic Couples as Collaborators, ed. Creamer, E. G., 146–66. New York: Routledge Falmer.Google Scholar
Mincer, J., and Polachek, S.. 1974. “Family Investments in Human Capital.” Journal of Political Economy 82 (2): S76S108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, L. A. 1998. “Glass Ceiling Effect or Cohort Affect? A Longitudinal Study of Gender Earnings Gaps for Engineers, 1982–1989.” American Sociological Review 63 (4): 479–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, Emory, Rudd, Elizabeth, and Nerad, Maresi. 2011. “Onto, Up, Off the Academic Faculty Ladder: The Gendered Effects of Family on Career Transitions for a Cohort of Social Science PhDs.” The Review of Higher Education 34 (4): 525–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Science Foundation. 2011. “Survey of Earned Doctorates.” The National Center for Science and Engineering Statistics (NCSES) (http://www.nsf.gov/statistics/srvydoctorates/).Google Scholar
National Science Foundation, Division of Science Resources Statistics. 2004. Gender Differences in the Careers of Academic Scientists and Engineers, NSF 04-323, Project Officer Alan I. Rapoport. Arlington, VA: National Science Foundation.Google Scholar
O'Leary, V. E., and Mitchell, J. M.. 1990. “Women Connecting with Women: Networks and Mentors.” In Storming the Tower: Women in the Academic World, ed. Lie, S. and O'Leary, V. E.. London: Kogan Page.Google Scholar
Ornstein, Michael, Stewart, Penni, and Drakich, Janice. 2007. “Promotion at Canadian Universities: The Intersection of Gender, Discipline, and Institution.” Canadian Journal of Higher Education 37 (3): 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Over, Ray. 1993. “Correlates of Career Advancement in Australian Universities.” Higher Education 26: 313–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perna, Laura W. 2001. “Sex and Race Differences in Faculty Tenure and Promotion.” Research in Higher Education 42 (5): 541–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perna, Laura W. 2005. “Sex Differences in Faculty Tenure and Promotion: The Contribution of Family Ties.” Research in Higher Education 46 (3): 277307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reskin, B. F. 2003. “Including Mechanisms in Our Models of Ascriptive Inequality.” American Sociological Review 68: 121.Google Scholar
Roos, Patricia A., and Gatta, Mary L.. 2009. “Gender (In)equity in the Academy: Subtle Mechanisms and the Production of Inequality.” Research in Social Stratification and Mobility 27 (3): 77200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenfeld, R. A., and Jones, J. A.. 1987. “Patterns and Effects of Geographic Mobility for Academic Women and Men.” Journal of Higher Education 58: 493515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothgeb, John M. Jr., and Burger, Betsy. 2009. “Tenure Standards in Political Science Departments: Results from a Survey of Department Chairs.” PS: Political Science & Politics 42 (3): 513–19.Google Scholar
Rowe, Mary P. 1990. “Barriers to Equality: The Power of Subtle Discrimination to Maintain Unequal Opportunity.” Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal 3 (2): 153–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudd, E., Morrison, E., Sadrozinski, R., Nerad, M., and Cerny, J.. 2008. “Equality and Illusion: Gender and Tenure in Art History Careers.” Journal of Marriage and Family 70: 228–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sax, Linda J., Hagedorn, Linda Serra, Arredondo, Marisol, and Dicrisi, Frank A. III. 2002. “Faculty Research Productivity: Exploring the Role of Gender and Family-Related Factors,Research in Higher Education 43 (4): 423–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, B. M., and Chingos, M. M.. 2007. “Ranking Doctoral Programs by Placement: A New Method.” PS: Political Science and Politics 40 (3): 523–29.Google Scholar
Stewart, Penni, Ornstein, Michael, and Drakich, Janice. 2009. “Gender and Promotion at Canadian Universities.” CRS/RCS 46 (1): 5985.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C.. 1986. “The Social Identity Theory of Intergroup Behaviour.” In Psychology of Intergroup Elations, eds. Worchel, S. and Austin, W. G., 724. Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Tien, Flora F. 2007. “To What Degree Does the Promotion System Reward Faculty Research Productivity?British Journal of Sociology of Education 28 (1): 105123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tierney, W. G., and Bensimon, E. M.. 1996. Promotion and Tenure: Community and Socialization in Academe. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Tomz, Michael, Wittenberg, Jason, and King, Gary. 2003. “CLARIFY: Software for Interpreting and Presenting Statistical Results.” Journal of Statistical Software 8 (1): 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toutkoushian, R. 1999. “The Status of Academic Women in the 1990s. No Longer Outsiders, but Not Yet Equals.” Quarterly Review of Economics and Finance 39: 679–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Assendelft, Laura, Gunther-Canada, Wendy, and Dolan, Julie. 2001. “The Status of Women in Political Science Departments in the South: Results of the Millennium Survey.” PS: Political Science and Politics 34 (2): 333–38.Google Scholar
Ward, K., and Wolf-Wendel, L.. 2004. “Academic Motherhood: Managing Complex Roles in Research Universities.” The Review of Higher Education 27 (2): 233–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, Melanie E. 2001. “Gender and Promotion in the Academic Profession.” Scottish Journal of Political Economy 48 (1): 283302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenneras, C., and Wold, A.. 1997. “Nepotism and Sexism in Peer-Review.” Nature 387: 341–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfinger, N. H., Mason, M. A., and Goulden, M.. 2008. “Problems in the Pipeline: Gender, Marriage, and Fertility in the Ivory Tower.” Journal of Higher Education 79 (4): 389405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodring, Paul. 1964. “Must College Teachers Publish or Perish?Saturday Review 48 (June 20): 4546.Google Scholar
Yoder, J. D. 1985. “An Academic Woman as Token: A Case Study.” Journal of Social Issues 41: 6172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, H. 1987. “The Careers of Men and Women Scientists: A Review of Current Research.” In Women: Their Underrepresentation and Career Differentials in Science and Engineering, ed. Dix, L. S.. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1A Descriptive Statistics for Differences in Means across Ranks (among men only and among women only)

Figure 1

Table 1B Percentage Differences across Ranks (among men and women separately)

Figure 2

Table 2 Predicting Academic Rank: Factors Affecting the Likelihood of Being an Associate Professor in Contrast with an Assistant Professor (binary logistic models via multiple imputation)

Figure 3

Table 3 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professors Compared with Full Professors (binary logistic models via multiple imputation)

Figure 4

Table 4 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professor Compared with Assistant Professor (spilt sample)

Figure 5

Table 5 Predicting Academic Rank: Associate Professors Compared with Full Professors (Split sample)

Figure 6

Table A1 Table A1: Survey Respondents and the Population