Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T03:17:50.247Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ageing and apoE change DHA homeostasis: relevance to age-related cognitive decline

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 October 2013

Marie Hennebelle
Affiliation:
Research Center on Aging, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Physiology and Biophysics, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
Mélanie Plourde
Affiliation:
Research Center on Aging, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Physiology and Biophysics, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Departments of Medicine, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
Raphaël Chouinard-Watkins
Affiliation:
Research Center on Aging, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Physiology and Biophysics, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
Christian-Alexandre Castellano
Affiliation:
Research Center on Aging, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Physiology and Biophysics, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
Pascale Barberger-Gateau
Affiliation:
INSERM, ISPED, Centre INSERM U897-Epidemiologie-Biostatistique, F-33000 Bordeaux, France
Stephen C. Cunnane*
Affiliation:
Research Center on Aging, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Physiology and Biophysics, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada Departments of Medicine, Université de Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
*
*Corresponding author: Professor S.C. Cunnane, fax (+1) 819-829-7141, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Epidemiological studies fairly convincingly suggest that higher intakes of fatty fish and n-3 fatty acids are associated with reduced risk of Alzheimer's disease (AD). DHA in plasma is normally positively associated with DHA intake. However, despite being associated with lower fish and DHA intake, unexpectedly, plasma (or brain) DHA is frequently not lower in AD. This review will highlight some metabolic and physiological factors such as ageing and apoE polymorphism that influence DHA homeostasis. Compared with young adults, blood DHA is often slightly but significantly higher in older adults without any age-related cognitive decline. Higher plasma DHA in older adults could be a sign that their fish or DHA intake is higher. However, our supplementation and carbon-13 tracer studies also show that DHA metabolism, e.g. transit through the plasma, apparent retroconversion and β-oxidation, is altered in healthy older compared with healthy young adults. ApoE4 increases the risk of AD, possibly in part because it too changes DHA homeostasis. Therefore, independent of differences in fish intake, changing DHA homeostasis may tend to obscure the relationship between DHA intake and plasma DHA which, in turn, may contribute to making older adults more susceptible to cognitive decline despite older adults having similar or sometimes higher plasma DHA than in younger adults. In conclusion, recent development of new tools such as isotopically labelled DHA to study DHA metabolism in human subjects highlights some promising avenues to evaluate how and why DHA metabolism changes during ageing and AD.

Type
Conference on ‘PUFA mediators: implications for human health’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2013 

The cognitive and psychological health of older adults is now a major preoccupation for healthcare services and researchers alike. Alzheimer's disease (AD) is the main form of cognitive decline in older persons in Western countries( Reference Blennow, de Leon and Zetterberg 1 ). Age is the main risk factor associated with AD( Reference Blennow, de Leon and Zetterberg 1 ), but other factors also have an effect such as a predisposing genetic polymorphism, i.e. ε4 allele of apoE4 ( Reference Corder, Saunders and Strittmatter 2 ), vascular risk factors including hypertension, obesity and type 2 diabetes( Reference Blennow, de Leon and Zetterberg 1 ), and lifestyle including physical activity and dietary habits( Reference Gomez-Pinilla 3 , Reference Alles, Samieri and Feart 4 ). Among the nutrients closely associated with brain function, the n-3 fatty acids, especially DHA, have attracted special attention. Fatty fish and seafood are the most important dietary sources of both DHA and EPA. DHA is by far the predominant n-3 fatty acid in the brain and is present mostly in various membrane phospholipids (PL) of neurons, especially in synapses( Reference Crawford, Bloom and Broadhurst 5 ). In contrast to other common dietary long-chain fatty acids, DHA is highly conserved and poorly β-oxidised( Reference Cunnane, Ryan and Nadeau 6 Reference Leyton, Drury and Crawford 8 ). In human subjects, DHA synthesis is relatively inefficient, especially in comparison to rodents( Reference Plourde and Cunnane 9 ).

Low intake of n-3 fatty acids has long been associated with higher risk of CVD( Reference Skeaff and Miller 10 ), and also of suboptimal brain development( Reference Innis 11 ). Much effort has been focused over the past decade on whether a higher DHA intake could decrease the risk of cognitive decline in older adults, or reduce the progression from mild cognitive impairment towards AD. In general, these studies polarise in two directions: randomised clinical trials that are largely negative and epidemiological studies that are more positive( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 ) about the DHA's role in maintaining cognition during ageing. Thus, in general, DHA supplementation trials in AD (with or without EPA) have not so far produced any truly positive results( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 Reference Sydenham, Dangour and Lim 15 ). Methodological issues such as dose of n-3 fatty acid, duration of treatment or selection criteria may well have affected the outcomes of these trials. DHA supplementation may have a greater positive effect on memory and learning in healthy adults( Reference Stonehouse, Conlon and Podd 16 ), elderly with subjective cognitive complaints( Reference Yurko-Mauro, McCarthy and Rom 17 ) or with mild cognitive impairment( Reference Sinn, Milte and Street 18 ) than in those with AD( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 , Reference Mazereeuw, Lanctot and Chau 14 , Reference Quinn, Raman and Thomas 19 ). However, prospective epidemiological studies have been more positive; they broadly show that habitually low intake of fish and/or DHA is associated with higher risk of developing AD( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 , Reference Fotuhi, Mohassel and Yaffe 20 , Reference Barberger-Gateau, Feart, Letenneur and Yaffe 21 ). These results are supported by the neuroprotective role of DHA reported for non-human models of neurodegenerative disease( Reference Kim, Akbar and Kim 22 Reference Boudrault, Bazinet and Ma 26 ).

Studies with biological samples (human blood and brain) may be able to provide useful leads to explain the divergent results between randomised clinical trials and epidemiological studies. We have previously reviewed at some length the methodological limits on observational or intervention studies on DHA supplementation in older adults or in AD( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 Reference Sydenham, Dangour and Lim 15 , Reference Fotuhi, Mohassel and Yaffe 20 , Reference Barberger-Gateau, Feart, Letenneur and Yaffe 21 , Reference Barberger-Gateau, Samieri and Feart 27 ); so we will review them here only briefly. We will also present an emerging framework showing that DHA homoeostasis changes in older adults and differs in carriers from non-carriers of apoE4, probably before the onset of cognitive decline.

DHA in plasma and post-mortem human brain

Brain DHA

In primate, pig and rodent models, when n-3 intake is severely deficient for extended periods, brain DHA also decreases across all cell types and regions, in association with lower scores on cognitive and behavioural tests( Reference Calon and Cole 25 , Reference Brenna and Diau 28 Reference Novak, Dyer and Innis 31 ). AD is now widely associated with lower fish and DHA intake, so it would be logical that post-mortem brain samples of patients with a definitive diagnosis of AD also contained lower DHA. Indeed, in the hippocampus, which is central to memory processing and learning, AD patients reportedly do have lower DHA( Reference Soderberg, Edlund and Kristensson 32 , Reference Lukiw, Cui and Marcheselli 33 ). However, in the temporal and frontal cortices which are also affected in AD, DHA is almost always the same as in the controls (Fig. 1). Studies reporting lower DHA in the AD brain show that other fatty acids are also lower, particularly n-6 PUFA( Reference Astarita, Jung and Berchtold 35 , Reference Corrigan, Horrobin and Skinner 41 , Reference Guan, Wang and Cairns 44 , Reference Prasad, Lovell and Yatin 46 ). Thus, the effect of AD is not specific to DHA which is contrary to what would be expected if only n-3 fatty acid intake were deficient.

Fig. 1. Summary of the published literature on brain and blood DHA in Alzheimer's disease. The symbols represent the results of individual studies using each study's control group as the reference (100 %; dotted line). The papers from which these DHA data are obtained are as follows: A, Arsenault et al.( Reference Arsenault, Matthan and Scott 34 ); B, Astarita et al.( Reference Astarita, Jung and Berchtold 35 ); C, Boston et al.( Reference Boston, Bennett and Horrobin 36 ); D, Brooksbank et al.( Reference Brooksbank and Martinez 37 ); E, Cherubini et al.( Reference Cherubini, Andres-Lacueva and Martin 38 ); F, Conquer et al.( Reference Conquer, Tierney and Zecevic 39 ); G, Corrigan et al.( Reference Corrigan, Van Rhijn and Ijomah 40 ); H, Corrigan et al.( Reference Corrigan, Horrobin and Skinner 41 ); I, Cunnane et al.( Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ); J, Fraser et al.( Reference Fraser, Tayler and Love 43 ); K, Guan et al.( Reference Guan, Wang and Cairns 44 ); L, Laurin et al.( Reference Laurin, Verreault and Lindsay 45 ); M, Prasad et al.( Reference Prasad, Lovell and Yatin 46 ); N, Selley et al.( Reference Selley 47 ); O, Skinner et al.( Reference Skinner, Watt and Besson 48 ); P, Söderberg et al.( Reference Soderberg, Edlund and Kristensson 32 ); Q, Tully et al.( Reference Tully, Roche and Doyle 49 ). F,T,P, frontal, temporal and/or parietal cortex; P-H, para-hippocampus; H, hippocampus; P-TFA, plasma total fatty acids; P-PL, plasma phospholipids; P-CE, plasma cholesteryl esters; RBC, red blood cells.

There are many potential methodological reasons for the observed lack of agreement between the apparently low DHA intakes in AD yet frequently normal DHA levels in the brain( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 ). Crucial among these are the ‘healthy’ controls against which the AD cases are compared as well as the very marked extent of regional brain atrophy associated with ageing regardless of the presence of neurological disease( Reference Guan, Wang and Cairns 44 , Reference Svennerholm, Boström and Jungbjer 50 ). Furthermore, the basis for classifying a patient as having AD, i.e. whether on clinical cognitive criteria or on neuropathological score, may not give consistent results since senile plaques are increasingly recognised as being present in a significant proportion of cognitively normal elderly persons( Reference Aizenstein, Nebes and Saxton 51 Reference Duyckaerts and Hauw 53 ). Hence, there is a risk that post-mortem samples from brain banks for which cognitive status is not known at the time of death could be misclassified if based solely on neuropathological scores. It also appears that membrane PL in the cortex can tenaciously retain DHA and that a more discrete and specific subcellular pool or membrane pool of DHA may have to be measured( Reference Fraser, Tayler and Love 43 ). Brain membrane DHA cycles rapidly between PL and free DHA via DHA-CoA( Reference Umhau, Zhou and Carson 54 , Reference Chen, Green and Orr 55 ), and the deteriorating efficacy of this process could theoretically contribute to the neurodegenerative processes. Thus, the key issue in relation to post-mortem tissue analysis is that the time to lipid extraction is rarely less than 4–5 h yet DHA turnover is on the order of minutes, if not seconds. Since the turnover of DHA towards resolvins and neuroprotectins are orders of magnitude lower than the amount of DHA in the brain NEFA pool, truly ‘physiological’ amounts of these products are extremely difficult to measure, especially in human subjects( Reference Bazan, Molina and Gordon 23 , Reference Serhan and Petasis 56 ). As also noted elsewhere, these and other issues severely constrain the validity and hence the utility of DHA measurements on human post-mortem brain samples( Reference Cunnane, Chouinard-Watkins and Castellano 57 ).

Blood DHA

Lower DHA would normally be expected in the blood of those with habitually low DHA intake (whether diagnosed with AD or not). In some AD studies, lower DHA is indeed reported for plasma total lipids( Reference Cherubini, Andres-Lacueva and Martin 38 ), PL( Reference Conquer, Tierney and Zecevic 39 , Reference Corrigan, Van Rhijn and Ijomah 40 , Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ), cholesteryl esters( Reference Tully, Roche and Doyle 49 ) and NEFA( Reference Wang, Sun and Liu 58 ). However, many other AD studies show no difference in plasma DHA, whether in PL or total fatty acids( Reference Arsenault, Matthan and Scott 34 , Reference Cherubini, Andres-Lacueva and Martin 38 , Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 , Reference Laurin, Verreault and Lindsay 45 , Reference Ronnemaa, Zethelius and Vessby 59 ). Some even report higher DHA in plasma PL( Reference Laurin, Verreault and Lindsay 45 ) or cholesteryl esters (CE)( Reference Corrigan, Van Rhijn and Ijomah 40 , Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ). Similar inconsistencies are present across DHA levels reported for the erythrocytes in AD (Fig. 1)( Reference Boston, Bennett and Horrobin 36 , Reference Corrigan, Van Rhijn and Ijomah 40 , Reference Selley 47 ). Prospective studies also show this inconsistency: some found a strong association between lower blood DHA level and slower cognitive decline( Reference Heude, Ducimetiere and Berr 60 ) or lower risk of dementia( Reference Schaefer, Bongard and Beiser 61 ), whereas other did not( Reference Laurin, Verreault and Lindsay 45 , Reference Ronnemaa, Zethelius and Vessby 59 , Reference Kroger, Verreault and Carmichael 62 ). It may be that the cognitive domain studied( Reference Dullemeijer, Durga and Brouwer 63 , Reference Beydoun, Kaufman and Satia 64 ) and apoE4 genotype( Reference Whalley, Deary and Starr 65 , Reference Samieri, Feart and Proust-Lima 66 ) contribute to this scatter in the data.

DHA homeostasis during ageing and apoE

We propose that even when collected under hypothetically ideal conditions (zero delay; perfectly matched, cognitively healthy controls, etc.), data obtained from single blood samples are too limited to fully understand possible changes in DHA metabolism due to genotype, ageing or neurodegenerative disease. However, isotopically labelled DHA is emerging as a useful tool to assess how the metabolism of DHA changes with age. Indeed, in a relatively simple study design, it was clear that the clearance of a 50 mg oral dose of uniformly carbon-13 labelled DHA (13C-DHA) from the blood over 1 month was much slower in healthy 76-year-old compared with 27-year-old adults( Reference Plourde, Chouinard-Watkins and Vandal 67 ). These results were similar to our earlier report that the increase in plasma DHA during a short-term treatment with fish oil was higher in healthy older persons( Reference Vandal, Freemantle and Tremblay-Mercier 68 ). 13C-DHA enrichment in plasma NEFA and TAG of older adults was most affected (four- to fivefold higher than in the young adults) but its enrichment in PL and CE 13C-DHA was also affected. The doubling of 13C-DHA enrichment in plasma PL and CE emerged only after about 7 d post-dose, suggesting slower DHA clearance through plasma lipid classes, i.e. an altered plasma ‘DHA wave’ in older adults (Fig. 2)( Reference Plourde, Chouinard-Watkins and Vandal 67 ).

Fig. 2. Delayed plasma clearance of carbon 13-labelled DHA (13C-DHA) during healthy ageing, adapted from Plourde et al.( Reference Plourde, Chouinard-Watkins and Vandal 67 ). Plasma 13C-DHA concentration was followed over 28 d after the oral administration of a single 50 mg dose of 13C-DHA in young (27 years; n 6) and elderly (76 years; n 6) participants. In older adults, plasma tracer concentration in NEFA and TAG was four to fivefold higher 4 h after giving the oral dose and about twofold higher 1–4 weeks later in phospholipids (PL) and cholesteryl esters (CE).

Clearly, therefore, healthy ageing seems to change DHA metabolism and, hence, homeostasis in human subjects. Notwithstanding the limited extent to which the kinetic behaviour of a tracer can be compared with a single plasma fatty acid measurement, the difference in 13C-DHA homeostasis in the elderly seems to reflect the results observed in two studies in which lower DHA was reported in plasma PL yet higher DHA was reported in plasma CE of AD patients( Reference Corrigan, Van Rhijn and Ijomah 40 , Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ). The minimally invasive nature of this type of experiment makes it difficult to invoke a particular mechanism but one could speculate that the changing sensitivity of endothelial lipoprotein lipase could be involved in this age-associated difference in DHA homeostasis( Reference Millar, Lichtenstein and Cuchel 69 ).

ApoE4 carriers are at significantly higher risk of AD( Reference Jack, Knopman and Jagust 70 , Reference Jack, Petersen and Xu 71 ). It is now emerging that the apoE4 status also affects DHA metabolism in human subjects( Reference Barberger-Gateau, Samieri and Feart 27 , Reference Chouinard-Watkins, Rioux-Perreault and Fortier 72 , Reference Plourde, Vohl and Vandal 73 ). This interaction may help explain why the protective association of higher dietary intake of fish( Reference Huang, Zandi and Tucker 74 , Reference Barberger-Gateau, Raffaitin and Letenneur 75 ) or higher erythrocyte total n-3 fatty acids( Reference Whalley, Deary and Starr 65 ) is generally limited to non-carriers of apoE4. Measuring expired 13C-CO2 after dosing with 13C-DHA permits the estimation of the whole body half-life of DHA in healthy older adults, which is of the order of 32 d in carriers of apoE4 and 140 d in non-carriers of apoE4 ( Reference Chouinard-Watkins, Rioux-Perreault and Fortier 72 ). Ageing seems not to affect the whole body half-life of DHA although the small sample size makes these results still somewhat preliminary( Reference Plourde, Chouinard-Watkins and Vandal 67 ).

Using positron emission tomography and the tracer, 11C-DHA, human brain turnover of DHA has been estimated to be about 4 mg/d, giving rise to a half-life of brain DHA of about 2·5 years( Reference Umhau, Zhou and Carson 54 ). Hence, the half-life of brain DHA is much longer than its whole body half-life. Perhaps further assessments of the brain or whole body half-life of DHA could provide some insight into the current ineffectiveness of DHA supplements in AD despite the fact that these supplements typically supply several fold the brain's apparent daily turnover of DHA( Reference Cunnane, Plourde and Pifferi 12 ).

ApoE4 seems also to supress the plasma DHA response to a fish oil supplement( Reference Plourde, Vohl and Vandal 73 ) and the metabolism of an oral dose of 13C-DHA( Reference Chouinard-Watkins, Rioux-Perreault and Fortier 72 ), an effect somewhat opposite to that observed with healthy ageing. For up to 28 d after a single oral dose of 13C-DHA, carriers of apoE4 have a slightly lower concentration of plasma 13C-DHA compared with non-carriers( Reference Chouinard-Watkins, Rioux-Perreault and Fortier 72 ). When the tracer is given both before and again after a 5-month period of DHA+EPA supplementation, the apoE4 carriers had a greater accumulation of the tracer in plasma after supplementation compared with the non-carriers, again suggesting slower clearance of DHA to and/or use by tissues (Fig. 3). Hence, two established risk factors for AD (ageing and apoE4) both significantly change DHA homeostasis but possibly in different ways.

Fig. 3. Plasma carbon 13-labelled DHA (13C-DHA) concentration over 28 d after a single oral dose of 40 mg 13C-DHA. Results expressed as means (sem) show the plasma 13C-DHA status (a) pre- (n 6) and (b) post- (n 4) supplementation of 5 months with 1·8 g/d EPA +1·4 g/d DHA in apoE ɛ4 carriers (apoE4+; ○) and non-carriers (apoE4-; ●).

Linking dietary and plasma DHA

Habitual DHA intake is commonly estimated to be <250 mg/d( Reference Harris, Mozaffarian and Lefevre 76 Reference Meyer 78 ) but this is a difficult and laborious measurement and subject to high day-to-day variability depending on the frequency of fish or shellfish consumption. Hence, it would be useful if DHA intake and plasma DHA were highly correlated because plasma DHA measurement is now technically simple and reliable, so it could potentially be a surrogate for dietary DHA measurement. There is indeed a good positive correlation between dietary and plasma intake for DHA consumption, especially at DHA intakes towards 1000 mg/d, during which plasma DHA rises to a maximum of about 4 % in plasma total lipids. However, DHA always seems to be present in plasma, even when DHA intake is negligible; thus, vegans consuming no known dietary sources of DHA still have about 0·5 % DHA in plasma total lipids( Reference Sanders, Hinds and Pereira 79 ). The problem is that there are relatively few reports on which to build the relationship of dietary to plasma DHA. At DHA intakes between 0 and 50 mg/d, DHA is between 0·5 and 1·2 % of plasma total lipids, but the spread in these data is large( Reference Cunnane, Chouinard-Watkins and Castellano 57 ). A value of 0·5 % DHA in plasma total lipids therefore seems to be at or close to the lower limit possible of plasma DHA in healthy adults.

The AD cases we have studied had 1·0 % DHA in plasma total lipids, which empirically corroborates very low DHA intake, yet they had ‘normal’ DHA in the PL of brain cortical grey matter( Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ). Plasma DHA does not rise in human subjects given EPA or α-linolenic acid supplements, even in vegans( Reference James, Ursin and Cleland 80 Reference Horrobin, Fokkema and Muskiet 82 ). The conundrum therefore is: how are plasma (or brain) DHA levels maintained when DHA intakes are very low to negligible? We speculate that with changes in DHA metabolism and homeostasis during age-related cognitive decline, the diet–plasma relation of DHA may shift, explaining why with lower DHA intake, a population with age-related cognitive decline appears to have the same plasma DHA concentration as healthy elderly even though the availability of DHA to the tissues may be reduced( Reference Lopez, Kritz-Silverstein and Barrett Connor 83 ). The lack of an established reference lipid class in blood (PL, CE, TAG or NEFA, erythrocytes, etc.) for DHA measurements relative to intake still hampers the extent to which plasma DHA data from various reports can be compared in relation to ageing, genotype and risk of cognitive decline. This area clearly needs further research but suffice it to say that it is becoming increasingly important to take into account changing DHA homeostasis in the study of ageing population, especially in a context of age-related cognitive decline or AD.

Conclusion

We have sought to briefly highlight some of the methodological challenges and potential future directions for the study of DHA in ageing and AD. The emerging evidence for changing DHA half-life in older adults and in carriers of apoE4 should encourage more basic research on DHA metabolism in human subjects. Molecular, cellular and animal models have contributed enormously to understanding the complexity of DHA biology, but none of them seem to represent the changes in DHA homeostasis reported in elderly human subjects. The human brain is able to strongly retain DHA in membrane PL despite very low DHA intake and advanced AD( Reference Cunnane, Schneider and Tangney 42 ), so the classical dietary n-3 deficiency model used to probe the function of DHA in the animal brain appears inappropriate for research into AD. In human subjects, DHA in the post-mortem brain is unlikely to correctly reflect what is happening during ageing and AD, due to its fast turnover in neuronal membrane PL. Ageing- and apoE4-associated changes in DHA metabolism strongly suggest altered DHA homeostasis involving a decrease in plasma DHA clearance during age-related cognitive decline and AD. Therefore, a shift in the relationship between plasma and dietary DHA may be occurring during age-related cognitive decline and AD, one that needs to be considered when looking at plasma DHA as a measure of dietary DHA intake. In the future, the availability of innovative tools for studies of DHA half-life and metabolism in human subjects will be needed to understand in a better manner the changes in DHA metabolism occurring during human ageing and AD and the potential protective role of DHA on cognitive decline. As shown in prospective studies, a protective role of DHA in cognitive health of older persons may depend on consuming a healthy diet throughout adult life.

Acknowledgements

Mélanie Fortier, Conrad Filteau, Christine Rioux-Perreault, Jennifer Tremblay-Mercier and Sébastien Tremblay provided excellent technical support.

Financial Support

CIHR, NSERC, FQRNT (CFQCU), INSERM, CFI and CRC provided financial support for S. C. C.'s research.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Authorship

M. H. and S. C. C. conceived and wrote the first draft with all the authors contributing to the revisions and final version of the manuscript.

References

1. Blennow, K, de Leon, MJ & Zetterberg, H (2006) Alzheimer's disease. Lancet 368, 387403.Google Scholar
2. Corder, EH, Saunders, AM, Strittmatter, WJ et al. (1993) Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science 261, 921923.Google Scholar
3. Gomez-Pinilla, F (2008) Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 568578.Google Scholar
4. Alles, B, Samieri, C, Feart, C et al. (2012) Dietary patterns: a novel approach to examine the link between nutrition and cognitive function in older individuals. Nutr Res Rev 25, 207222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Crawford, MA, Bloom, M, Broadhurst, CL et al. (1999) Evidence for the unique function of docosahexaenoic acid during the evolution of the modern hominid brain. Lipids 34, Suppl, S39S47.Google Scholar
6. Cunnane, SC, Ryan, MA, Nadeau, CR et al. (2003) Why is carbon from some polyunsaturates extensively recycled into lipid synthesis? Lipids 38, 477484.Google Scholar
7. Gavino, GR & Gavino, VC (1991) Rat liver outer mitochondrial carnitine palmitoyltransferase activity towards long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and their CoA esters. Lipids 26, 266270.Google Scholar
8. Leyton, J, Drury, PJ & Crawford, MA (1987) Differential oxidation of saturated and unsaturated fatty acids in vivo in the rat. Br J Nutr 57, 383393.Google Scholar
9. Plourde, M & Cunnane, SC (2007) Extremely limited synthesis of long chain polyunsaturates in adults: implications for their dietary essentiality and use as supplements. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab 32, 619634.Google Scholar
10. Skeaff, CM & Miller, J (2009) Dietary fat and coronary heart disease: summary of evidence from prospective cohort and randomised controlled trials. Ann Nutr Metab 55, 173201.Google Scholar
11. Innis, SM (2007) Dietary (n-3) fatty acids and brain development. J Nutr 137, 855859.Google Scholar
12. Cunnane, SC, Plourde, M, Pifferi, F et al. (2009) Fish, docosahexaenoic acid and Alzheimer's disease. Prog Lipid Res 48, 239256.Google Scholar
13. Yaffe, K (2010) Treatment of Alzheimer disease and prognosis of dementia: time to translate research to results. JAMA 304, 19521953.Google Scholar
14. Mazereeuw, G, Lanctot, KL, Chau, SA et al. (2012) Effects of omega-3 fatty acids on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging 33, 1482, e1417e1429.Google Scholar
15. Sydenham, E, Dangour, AD & Lim, WS (2012) Omega 3 fatty acid for the prevention of cognitive decline and dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 6, CD005379.Google Scholar
16. Stonehouse, W, Conlon, CA, Podd, J et al. (2013) DHA supplementation improved both memory and reaction time in healthy young adults: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 97, 11341143.Google Scholar
17. Yurko-Mauro, K, McCarthy, D, Rom, D et al. (2010) Beneficial effects of docosahexaenoic acid on cognition in age-related cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement 6, 456464.Google Scholar
18. Sinn, N, Milte, CM, Street, SJ et al. (2012) Effects of n-3 fatty acids, EPA v. DHA, on depressive symptoms, quality of life, memory and executive function in older adults with mild cognitive impairment: a 6-month randomised controlled trial. Br J Nutr 107, 16821693.Google Scholar
19. Quinn, JF, Raman, R, Thomas, RG et al. (2010) Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA 304, 19031911.Google Scholar
20. Fotuhi, M, Mohassel, P & Yaffe, K (2009) Fish consumption, long-chain omega-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive decline or Alzheimer disease: a complex association. Nat Clin Pract Neurol 5, 140152.Google Scholar
21. Barberger-Gateau, P, Feart, C, Letenneur, et al. (2013) Dietary patterns and dementia. In Chronic Medical Disease and Cognitive Aging: Toward a Healthy Body and Brain pp. 197224 [Yaffe, K, editor]. United States: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
22. Kim, HY, Akbar, M & Kim, YS (2010) Phosphatidylserine-dependent neuroprotective signaling promoted by docosahexaenoic acid. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 82, 165172.Google Scholar
23. Bazan, NG, Molina, MF & Gordon, WC (2011) Docosahexaenoic acid signalolipidomics in nutrition: significance in aging, neuroinflammation, macular degeneration, Alzheimer's, and other neurodegenerative diseases. Annu Rev Nutr 31, 321351.Google Scholar
24. Sidhu, VK, Huang, BX & Kim, HY (2011) Effects of docosahexaenoic acid on mouse brain synaptic plasma membrane proteome analyzed by mass spectrometry and (16)O/(18)O labeling. J Proteome Res 10, 54725480.Google Scholar
25. Calon, F & Cole, G (2007) Neuroprotective action of omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids against neurodegenerative diseases: evidence from animal studies. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 77, 287293.Google Scholar
26. Boudrault, C, Bazinet, RP & Ma, DW (2009) Experimental models and mechanisms underlying the protective effects of n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in Alzheimer's disease. J Nutr Biochem 20, 110.Google Scholar
27. Barberger-Gateau, P, Samieri, C, Feart, C et al. (2011) Dietary omega 3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and Alzheimer's disease: interaction with apolipoprotein E genotype. Curr Alzheimer Res 8, 479491.Google Scholar
28. Brenna, JT & Diau, GY (2007) The influence of dietary docosahexaenoic acid and arachidonic acid on central nervous system polyunsaturated fatty acid composition. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 77, 247250.Google Scholar
29. Oster, T & Pillot, T (2010) Docosahexaenoic acid and synaptic protection in Alzheimer's disease mice. Biochimica et Biophysica Acta – Mol Cell Biol Lipids 1801, 791798.Google Scholar
30. Connor, WE, Neuringer, M & Reisbick, S (1991) Essentiality of omega 3 fatty acids: evidence from the primate model and implications for human nutrition. World Rev Nutr Diet 66, 118132.Google Scholar
31. Novak, EM, Dyer, RA & Innis, SM (2008) High dietary omega-6 fatty acids contribute to reduced docosahexaenoic acid in the developing brain and inhibit secondary neurite growth. Brain Res 1237, 136145.Google Scholar
32. Soderberg, M, Edlund, C, Kristensson, K et al. (1991) Fatty acid composition of brain phospholipids in aging and in Alzheimer's disease. Lipids 26, 421425.Google Scholar
33. Lukiw, WJ, Cui, JG, Marcheselli, VL et al. (2005) A role for docosahexaenoic acid-derived neuroprotectin D1 in neural cell survival and Alzheimer disease. J Clin Invest 115, 27742783.Google Scholar
34. Arsenault, LN, Matthan, N, Scott, TM et al. (2009) Validity of estimated dietary eicosapentaenoic acid and docosahexaenoic acid intakes determined by interviewer-administered food frequency questionnaire among older adults with mild-to-moderate cognitive impairment or dementia. Am J Epidemiol 170, 95103.Google Scholar
35. Astarita, G, Jung, KM, Berchtold, NC et al. (2010) Deficient liver biosynthesis of docosahexaenoic acid correlates with cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. PLoS ONE 5, e12538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. Boston, PF, Bennett, A, Horrobin, DF et al. (2004) Ethyl-EPA in Alzheimer's disease–a pilot study. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 71, 341346.Google Scholar
37. Brooksbank, BW & Martinez, M (1989) Lipid abnormalities in the brain in adult Down's syndrome and Alzheimer's disease. Mol Chem Neuropathol 11, 157185.Google Scholar
38. Cherubini, A, Andres-Lacueva, C, Martin, A et al. (2007) Low plasma N-3 fatty acids and dementia in older persons: The InCHIANTI study. J Gerontol A, Biol Sci Med Sci 62, 11201126.Google Scholar
39. Conquer, JA, Tierney, MC, Zecevic, J et al. (2000) Fatty acid analysis of blood plasma of patients with Alzheimer's disease, other types of dementia, and cognitive impairment. Lipids 35, 13051312.Google Scholar
40. Corrigan, FM, Van Rhijn, AG, Ijomah, G et al. (1991) Tin and fatty acids in dementia. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 43, 229238.Google Scholar
41. Corrigan, FM, Horrobin, DF, Skinner, ER et al. (1998) Abnormal content of n-6 and n-3 long-chain unsaturated fatty acids in the phosphoglycerides and cholesterol esters of parahippocampal cortex from Alzheimer's disease patients and its relationship to acetyl CoA content. Int J Biochem Cell Biol 30, 197207.Google Scholar
42. Cunnane, S, Schneider, J, Tangney, C et al. (2012) Plasma and brain fatty acid profiles in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis 29, 691697.Google Scholar
43. Fraser, T, Tayler, H & Love, S (2010) Fatty acid composition of frontal, temporal and parietal neocortex in the normal human brain and in Alzheimer's disease. Neurochem Res 35, 503513.Google Scholar
44. Guan, Z, Wang, Y, Cairns, NJ et al. (1999) Decrease and structural modifications of phosphatidylethanolamine plasmalogen in the brain with Alzheimer disease. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 58, 740747.Google Scholar
45. Laurin, D, Verreault, R, Lindsay, J et al. (2003) Omega-3 fatty acids and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. J Alzheimer's Dis 5, 315322.Google Scholar
46. Prasad, MR, Lovell, MA, Yatin, M et al. (1998) Regional membrane phospholipid alterations in Alzheimer's disease. Neurochem Res 23, 8188.Google Scholar
47. Selley, ML (2007) A metabolic link between S-adenosylhomocysteine and polyunsaturated fatty acid metabolism in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 28, 18341839.Google Scholar
48. Skinner, ER, Watt, C, Besson, JA et al. (1993) Differences in the fatty acid composition of the grey and white matter of different regions of the brains of patients with Alzheimer's disease and control subjects. Brain 116(Pt 3), 717725.Google Scholar
49. Tully, AM, Roche, HM, Doyle, R et al. (2003) Low serum cholesteryl ester-docosahexaenoic acid levels in Alzheimer's disease: a case-control study. Br J Nutr 89, 483489.Google Scholar
50. Svennerholm, L, Boström, K & Jungbjer, B (1997) Changes in weight and compositions of major membrane components of human brain during the span of adult human life of Swedes. Acta Neuropathol 94, 345352.Google Scholar
51. Aizenstein, HJ, Nebes, RD, Saxton, JA et al. (2008) Frequent amyloid deposition without significant cognitive impairment among the elderly. Arch Neurol 65, 15091517.Google Scholar
52. Braak, H & Braak, E (1997) Frequency of stages of Alzheimer-related lesions in different age categories. Neurobiol Aging 18, 351357.Google Scholar
53. Duyckaerts, C & Hauw, JJ (1997) Prevalence, incidence and duration of Braak's stages in the general population: can we know? Neurobiol Aging 18, 362369; discussion 389–392.Google Scholar
54. Umhau, JC, Zhou, W, Carson, RE et al. (2009) Imaging incorporation of circulating docosahexaenoic acid into the human brain using positron emission tomography. J Lipid Res 50, 12591268.Google Scholar
55. Chen, CT, Green, JT, Orr, SK et al. (2008) Regulation of brain polyunsaturated fatty acid uptake and turnover. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 79, 8591.Google Scholar
56. Serhan, CN & Petasis, NA (2011) Resolvins and protectins in inflammation resolution. Chem Rev 111, 59225943.Google Scholar
57. Cunnane, SC, Chouinard-Watkins, R, Castellano, CA et al. (2013) Docosahexaenoic acid homeostasis, brain aging and Alzheimer's disease: can we reconcile the evidence? Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 88, 6170.Google Scholar
58. Wang, DC, Sun, CH, Liu, LY et al. (2012) Serum fatty acid profiles using GC-MS and multivariate statistical analysis: potential biomarkers of Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging 33, 10571066.Google Scholar
59. Ronnemaa, E, Zethelius, B, Vessby, B et al. (2012) Serum fatty-acid composition and the risk of Alzheimer's disease: a longitudinal population-based study. Eur J Clin Nutr 66, 885890.Google Scholar
60. Heude, B, Ducimetiere, P & Berr, C (2003) Cognitive decline and fatty acid composition of erythrocyte membranes–the EVA Study. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 803808.Google Scholar
61. Schaefer, EJ, Bongard, V, Beiser, AS et al. (2006) Plasma phosphatidylcholine docosahexaenoic acid content and risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease: the Framingham Heart Study. Arch Neurol 63, 15451550.Google Scholar
62. Kroger, E, Verreault, R, Carmichael, PH et al. (2009) Omega-3 fatty acids and risk of dementia: the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 184192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63. Dullemeijer, C, Durga, J, Brouwer, IA et al. (2007) n 3 fatty acid proportions in plasma and cognitive performance in older adults. Am J Clin Nutr 86, 14791485.Google Scholar
64. Beydoun, MA, Kaufman, JS, Satia, JA et al. (2007) Plasma n-3 fatty acids and the risk of cognitive decline in older adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Am J Clin Nutr 85, 11031111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65. Whalley, LJ, Deary, IJ, Starr, JM et al. (2008) n-3 Fatty acid erythrocyte membrane content, APOE varepsilon4, and cognitive variation: an observational follow-up study in late adulthood. Am J Clin Nutr 87, 449454.Google Scholar
66. Samieri, C, Feart, C, Proust-Lima, C et al. (2011) omega-3 fatty acids and cognitive decline: modulation by ApoEepsilon4 allele and depression. Neurobiol Aging 32, 2317, e2313e2322.Google Scholar
67. Plourde, M, Chouinard-Watkins, R, Vandal, M et al. (2011) Plasma incorporation, apparent retroconversion and beta-oxidation of 13C-docosahexaenoic acid in the elderly. Nutr Metab (Lond) 8, 5.Google Scholar
68. Vandal, M, Freemantle, E, Tremblay-Mercier, J et al. (2008) Plasma omega-3 fatty acid response to a fish oil supplement in the healthy elderly. Lipids 43, 10851089.Google Scholar
69. Millar, JS, Lichtenstein, AH, Cuchel, M et al. (1995) Impact of age on the metabolism of VLDL, IDL, and LDL apolipoprotein B-100 in men. J Lipid Res 36, 11551167.Google Scholar
70. Jack, CR Jr, Knopman, DS, Jagust, WJ et al. (2010) Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer's pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol 9, 119128.Google Scholar
71. Jack, CR Jr, Petersen, RC, Xu, YC et al. (1998) Hippocampal atrophy and apolipoprotein E genotype are independently associated with Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol 43, 303310.Google Scholar
72. Chouinard-Watkins, R, Rioux-Perreault, C, Fortier, M et al. (2013) Disturbance in uniformly 13C-labelled docosahexaenoic acid metabolism in elderly humans carrying apolipoprotein E epsilon 4 allele. Br J Nutr 30, 19.Google Scholar
73. Plourde, M, Vohl, MC, Vandal, M et al. (2009) Plasma n-3 fatty acid response to an n-3 fatty acid supplement is modulated by apoE epsilon4 but not by the common PPAR-alpha L162 V polymorphism in men. Br J Nutr 102, 11211124.Google Scholar
74. Huang, TL, Zandi, PP, Tucker, KL et al. (2005) Benefits of fatty fish on dementia risk are stronger for those without APOE epsilon4. Neurology 65, 14091414.Google Scholar
75. Barberger-Gateau, P, Raffaitin, C, Letenneur, L et al. (2007) Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort study. Neurology 69, 19211930.Google Scholar
76. Harris, WS, Mozaffarian, D, Lefevre, M et al. (2009) Towards establishing dietary reference intakes for eicosapentaenoic and docosahexaenoic acids. J Nutr 139, 804S819S.Google Scholar
77. Kris-Etherton, PM, Grieger, JA & Etherton, TD (2009) Dietary reference intakes for DHA and EPA. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 81, 99104.Google Scholar
78. Meyer, BJ (2011) Are we consuming enough long chain omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids for optimal health? Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 85, 275280.Google Scholar
79. Sanders, TA, Hinds, A & Pereira, CC (1989) Influence of n-3 fatty acids on blood lipids in normal subjects. J Intern Med Suppl 731, 99104.Google Scholar
80. James, MJ, Ursin, VM & Cleland, LG (2003) Metabolism of stearidonic acid in human subjects: comparison with the metabolism of other n-3 fatty acids. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 11401145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
81. Fokkema, MR, Brouwer, DA, Hasperhoven, MB et al. (2000) Short-term supplementation of low-dose gamma-linolenic acid (GLA), alpha-linolenic acid (ALA), or GLA plus ALA does not augment LCP omega 3 status of Dutch vegans to an appreciable extent. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 63, 287292.Google Scholar
82. Horrobin, D, Fokkema, MR & Muskiet, FA (2003) The effects on plasma, red cell and platelet fatty acids of taking 12 g/day of ethyl-eicosapentaenoate for 16 months: dihomogammalinolenic, arachidonic and docosahexaenoic acids and relevance to Inuit metabolism. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 68, 301304.Google Scholar
83. Lopez, LB, Kritz-Silverstein, D & Barrett Connor, E (2011) High dietary and plasma levels of the omega-3 fatty acid docosahexaenoic acid are associated with decreased dementia risk: the Rancho Bernardo study. J Nutr Health Aging 15, 2531.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Summary of the published literature on brain and blood DHA in Alzheimer's disease. The symbols represent the results of individual studies using each study's control group as the reference (100 %; dotted line). The papers from which these DHA data are obtained are as follows: A, Arsenault et al.(34); B, Astarita et al.(35); C, Boston et al.(36); D, Brooksbank et al.(37); E, Cherubini et al.(38); F, Conquer et al.(39); G, Corrigan et al.(40); H, Corrigan et al.(41); I, Cunnane et al.(42); J, Fraser et al.(43); K, Guan et al.(44); L, Laurin et al.(45); M, Prasad et al.(46); N, Selley et al.(47); O, Skinner et al.(48); P, Söderberg et al.(32); Q, Tully et al.(49). F,T,P, frontal, temporal and/or parietal cortex; P-H, para-hippocampus; H, hippocampus; P-TFA, plasma total fatty acids; P-PL, plasma phospholipids; P-CE, plasma cholesteryl esters; RBC, red blood cells.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Delayed plasma clearance of carbon 13-labelled DHA (13C-DHA) during healthy ageing, adapted from Plourde et al.(67). Plasma 13C-DHA concentration was followed over 28 d after the oral administration of a single 50 mg dose of 13C-DHA in young (27 years; n 6) and elderly (76 years; n 6) participants. In older adults, plasma tracer concentration in NEFA and TAG was four to fivefold higher 4 h after giving the oral dose and about twofold higher 1–4 weeks later in phospholipids (PL) and cholesteryl esters (CE).

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Plasma carbon 13-labelled DHA (13C-DHA) concentration over 28 d after a single oral dose of 40 mg 13C-DHA. Results expressed as means (sem) show the plasma 13C-DHA status (a) pre- (n 6) and (b) post- (n 4) supplementation of 5 months with 1·8 g/d EPA +1·4 g/d DHA in apoE ɛ4 carriers (apoE4+; ○) and non-carriers (apoE4-; ●).