Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:33:51.424Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Polarity in a four-level tone language: tone features in Tenyidie

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2021

Savio M. Meyase*
Affiliation:
University of Leipzig
*
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This paper reports a new kind of tone polarity, where the phenomenon is seen in a language with four level tones, Tenyidie (also known as Angami). I show that the polarity is in the features of the tones, i.e. at a subtonal level. The data also provide evidence that tones themselves can be broken down into smaller features. Furthermore, I demonstrate that the polarity pattern observed in the language is an epiphenomenon, a reflex of the Obligatory Contour Principle, not a phonological process in its own right. I show this with the help of a new type of tonal representation. Theoretical discussions of tone polarity have so far been almost entirely restricted to African tone systems, and to languages with just two tones. This paper brings into the discussion a Tibeto-Burman language with four tones.

Type
Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

1 Introduction

Tone polarity is the pattern where a tone-bearing unit (TBU) y has a tonal value which is the opposite of that of an immediately adjacent TBU x; TBU y is generally an affix. Mainstream theoretical discussions on tone polarity have so far been almost exclusively based on African languages.Footnote 1 The classic example is Mòoré (also known as Mossi), reported by Kenstowicz et al. (Reference Kenstowicz, Nikiema and Ourso1988), where the tone on the number suffix is always the opposite of that on the noun, as in (1). (Mòoré has two tones: high, indicated in (1) by the diacritic /´/, and low, which is left unmarked.)

  1. (1)

Despite tone polarity being considered typically an African property, there are other tone languages that display the phenomenon. Mundurukú (Picanço Reference Picanço2002), spoken in Brazil, is probably the only one reported in the Americas. Sylheti (Mahanta & Gope Reference Mahanta and Gope2018), an Indo-European language spoken in eastern India, has also been reported to display tone polarity. Three Tibeto-Burman languages have recently been identified as displaying the same phenomenon: Anál (Ozerov Reference Ozerov2018), Myebon Sumtu Chin (Watkins Reference Watkins2013) and Thadou (also known as Kuki-Thadou; Hyman Reference Hyman2007). The Qiangic languages Mianchi Qiang (Evans Reference Evans2008) and Zhuokeji Rgyalrong (also known as Jiarong; Lin Reference Lin2012) have been claimed to have tone polarity, but these display a tone change in the same morpheme in word formation, rather than surface tone polarity, and are not really an example of the same phenomenon.

Tone polarity has almost always been discussed with respect to two-way tone systems. All the above languages, African and non-African, are systems with only two tones, apart from three which have three tones: Yoruba, Mono and Gokana. Of these, Yoruba has been argued to have only two phonological tones, with the ‘third’ tone being the absence of phonological tone (Akinlabi & Liberman Reference Akinlabi, Liberman, Gerlach and Grijzenhout2000), while the tone polarity patterns in both Mono and Gokana are rather restricted, and are seen in only one morpheme in each language.

In this paper I show that tone polarity is seen in the Tibeto-Burman language Tenyidie (/tēɲīdiē/;Footnote 2 also known as Angami), which has a four-tone system. The analysis of polarity in a four-tone language is not as straightforward as it is in a two-tone language, because the very notion of polarity entails just two categories. I argue that, if the four tones of Tenyidie are decomposed into two polar features, polarity can be shown to operate at a subtonal level. The data provides evidence that tones can be broken down into smaller features, supporting McPherson (Reference McPherson, Payne, Pacchiarotti and Bosire2010), but arguing against Hyman (Reference Hyman2010) and Clements et al. (Reference Clements, Michaud and Patin2010), who are sceptical of tone features.

There is a long-standing debate in phonology over whether polarity is a process sui generis or just an epiphenomenon of other linguistic processes. While scholars like Gregersen (Reference Gregersen and Voeltz1974) see polarity as a phonological operation relying on powerful mechanisms like alpha rules, others, like de Lacy (Reference de Lacy and Trommer2012), have argued that it is merely a secondary consequence of other phonological operations, such as the Obligatory Contour Principle (OCP). In this paper, I show that the Tenyidie case is better analysed as an epiphenomenon resulting from the operation of the OCP. Crucially, I will also argue that Tenyidie provides new evidence for the use of subtonal autosegmental representation, and show the derivation of the various tonal phenomena simply falls into place using already available phonological tools.

In §2, I present the language and the data illustrating tonal natural classes and the polarity pattern. §3 provides my analysis, with the help of a novel autosegmental tonal representation based on two hierarchically organised nodal tone tiers (register and melody), each associated to the same subtonal feature set, H and L, which allows for the interaction between tiers without disrupting their structural positions. I present my analysis in the framework of standard parallel Optimality Theory (OT). In §4, I discuss other possible approaches and tone representations for the polarity data. In §5, I consider the case of Tenyidie as an addition to the empirical and theoretical discussion. §6 provides more data on the interaction of the tones in the language with respect to multiple suffixes, showing that the observed polarity is independent of the stem–affix environment, against the general view on polarity.

2 The language and data

Tenyidie, also known by the exonym Angami (ISO: njm), is a Tibeto-Burman language spoken by the Angami tribe in the district of Kohima in the Indian state of Nagaland, which is part of the Himalayan range separating India from Myanmar. All the data is from the author's fieldwork from the last ten years, and are presented in Meyase (Reference Meyase2014, Reference Meyase2016); the polarity pattern is also presented there, but not mentioned explicitly. The 2014 study includes data from two female Tenyidie speakers born and raised in Kohima Town, who were then university students aged 25 and 29; the 2016 study includes data from the previous two speakers, as well as acoustic data from six other Kohima-resident Tenyidie speakers, aged between 20 and 25 at the time of recording. The author is a native speaker of Tenyidie, also born and raised in Kohima Town, and their native speaker intuitions also contributed to the data collection process. Speakers of Tenyidie from Kohima over the age of 50 were also consulted to check the data.

There are four lexically distinctive level tones in Tenyidie, as shown in (2).

  1. (2)

Grammarians of the language (e.g. Kuolie Reference Kuolie2006) describe it as having five tones, but only four tones are acoustically reported in phonetic studies (Dutta et al. Reference Dutta, Ezung, Mahanta and Meyase2012), just as in the related variety of Khonoma (Blankenship et al. 1992). However, phonologically it has a fifth contour/cluster tone, comprised of High followed by a floating Mid, where the complex tone sounds like a High tone but behaves like a Mid tone (see Meyase Reference Meyase2014 for discussion and analysis). The fifth tone is not considered in this study, since it behaves in the same way as the Mid tone with respect to suffixes.

Moving on to the data under discussion in this paper, there are three types of suffixes in Tenyidie, based on their tonal behaviour. I refer to them as the ‘non-alternating suffixes’ (see (3) below), the ‘assimilating suffixes’ (4) and the ‘quirky alternating suffixes’ (5).

Firstly, the set of non-alternating suffixes in (3) display no tonal alternations at all, and suffixes with all the four lexical tones are found. We see all the tones in the stem on the syllable ze /ze/, giving four distinctive words, followed by the prospective suffix -zhie /ʒie̋/, which bears an Extra High tone, the perfective -shü /ʃə́/, bearing a High tone, the imperative -cie /ciē/, with a Mid tone, and the progressive -tha /tʰà/, with a Low tone.

  1. (3)

In (4) we have the same set of stems as in (3), but now followed by the progressive suffix -zhü /ʒə/ and the perfective suffix -ta /ta/. Unlike the set of non-alternating suffixes, the tone of these suffixes alternates between Extra High and High. The suffixes shows partial adoption of tonal height from the stem, hence the term ‘assimilating suffixes’. In the first two forms in each subset, the tone of the suffix is an exact copy of that of the stem. This is not the case in the other two forms, but even in these two the tonally higher stem also has a higher tone on the suffix, i.e. Mid on the stem triggers Extra High on the suffix, and Low triggers High. I will show below that this is a case of assimilation at a subtonal level.

  1. (4)

Other suffixes of this type are another perfective marker, -te, and another progressive marker, -ba.

The set of ‘quirky alternating suffixes’ in (5) represents the essential data for tone polarity. As pointed out in §1, this set seems to display some kind of polarity – Extra High and High on the stem triggers Low on the suffix, and Low triggers Mid. The Mid tone on the stem triggers the same tone in the suffix. Thus the irrealis suffix -lie /lie/ in (5a) alternates in tone between Mid and Low, but displays a pattern which is different from (4). The negation marker -mo and the prohibitive -hie are also suffixes of this type, as is the progressive marker -tuo and the homophonous future marker -tuo. The alternation is again seen at a lower tone level; this will be discussed in §3.

  1. (5)

The alternation in (5) is not restricted to the verb–suffix environment, but can also be found in nominal suffixes. For example, the singular non-female (default gender) definite marker -u shows the same pattern of alternation, as shown in (6). In addition, the negation marker -mo, which also displays ‘quirky’ tonal behaviour, can follow both verbals and nominals.

  1. (6)

An inference that can be drawn from the datasets in (3)–(6) is that the non-alternating suffixes in (3) are fully specified for tone, and are therefore immune to tonal changes, regardless of the tone in the stem. On the other hand, the two kinds of suffixes in (4) and (5) are underspecified for some subtonal feature(s), and alternate based on the type of underspecification they have, influenced by the tone in the stem. This forms the basis of my argument for tone features.

3 Analysis

3.1 Representation

The data in the assimilating suffix set in (4) and the quirky alternating suffix set in (5) shows complex tonal change patterns; the tone changes are, however, systematic. In the assimilating suffix set, while there are cases of complete assimilation of tones, there are also cases in which the phonetic height of the tones is incompletely assimilated.

To analyse the intricacies of the patterns, let us first break the two sets down into smaller sets. The first two instances in the assimilation suffix data in (4a) are repeated as (7a).

  1. (7)

As noted in §2, there is an apparently complete assimilation of the tones from the stem to the suffix: Extra High and High trigger the same tones in the suffix.

The case is different in the other two examples from (4a), repeated in (7b), where a Mid stem triggers an Extra High in the suffix, and Low triggers a High. However, there is still a partial assimilation of phonetic height – the higher tone in the stem triggers the higher tone in the suffix, and the lower tone in the stem trigger the lower one in the suffix.

In both (7a) and (b), it can be observed that the tones in the resultant suffix, although not identical, are consistently in the upper half of the tonal spectrum. The inference is that the two higher tones of the language have something in common. At the same time, the two higher stem tones in (7a) and (7b) are related (since they trigger the same tone on the suffix), as are the two lower suffix tones in the sets. This allows us to identify the following natural distinctions: (a) Extra High and High vs. Mid and Low, and (b) Extra High and Mid vs. High and Low. In other words, each of these pairs forms a natural class. As a result, representing the four tones of the language as four individual indivisible atoms is inadequate. This leads in turn to the need to decompose tones into smaller features, as has been previously argued for four-way tonal systems by, for example, Yip (Reference Yip1980), Clements (Reference Clements and Dihoff1983), Pulleyblank (Reference Pulleyblank1986) and Snider (Reference Snider1999)

For the purpose of this analysis, I propose the use of a tonal model in which the register tier and the pitch/tone/melody tier (henceforth the ‘melody’ tier, in order to avoid confusion between the lexical tones and the ‘tone’ tier) can interact with each other. A similar model can be found in Hyman's (Reference Hyman, van der Hulst and Snider1993) autosegmental representation of tones. However, Hyman's model is for a two-way tone system, and does not involve the decomposition of tones into smaller features. In the model proposed here, the TBU dominates the tonal root node (TRN), represented as •, which in turn dominates and is associated to the tonal node (TN), represented as ◦. The TRN and the TN are then associated to the register feature and the melody feature respectively. In the representation in (8), the four tones of Tenyidie are raised and lowered versions of the melodies H and L. H and L are subtonal features, not whole tones. Register H raises the pitch, and register L lowers the pitch.

  1. (8)

In (8), Extra High and High have a melody feature H each linked to the TN (which forms the melody feature); this is the feature common to both tones. They are distinguished by the different features linked to the TRN. If the TRN is linked to another occurrence of H (the register feature), this raises the H associated with the TN, forming an Extra High tone. On the other hand, an L linked to the TRN lowers the H associated with the TN, resulting in a High tone.

The crucial difference between my proposed representation and Hyman's is that, for Hyman, the tone/feature on the register node is not an inherent property of the tone the TBU is associated to; rather, it is an independent whole tone that plays a role only when it is in contact with neighbouring tones. In my representation, however, the feature (rather than the tone) on the register node is inherently a property of the tone.

The analysis of Mid and Low tones is similar, the difference being that the TN is associated with an L. For Mid, the L is raised by the H associated with the TRN; for Low, it is lowered by the L associated with the TRN. We then have four distinct configurations for the four tones in the language, using the traditional features of tone, H and L.

An important aspect of these configurations, following Hyman, is that the projections of the nodes TRN and TN are on different planes, and therefore the linear order of the tone features within a syllable (i.e. within the TBU) does not play a role.

3.1.1 The representation of assimilating suffixes

Using the representations proposed above, if we assume that an assimilating suffix is specified for the TN but has no specification for the TRN, we can derive the observed alternation as a spreading of the feature in the TRN from the stem to the suffix, as for [zē-ʒə̋] in (9). Since the register and the melody are on different planes, the H in the register of the stem syllable is free to spread onto the next empty register node without crossing any association line.

  1. (9)

The process of spreading is the same for [zè-ʒə́], where the TRN is L. This spreads on to the same underspecified suffix, yielding a High tone, rather than Extra High.

3.1.2 The representation of quirky alternating suffixes

The behaviour of the quirky alternating suffixes in (5) is in some respects the reverse of that of the assimilating suffixes in (4). In (5) the tone of the suffix is always in the lower regions of the tonal domain – Mid and Low – regardless of the tone in the stem. This provides further support for the proposed model, in which the two lower tones form a natural class which excludes the two higher tones.

Using the representations proposed here, this pattern can be analysed as a case of epenthesis-driven surface dissimilation. In (10a) (repeated from (5a) above), the two higher tones trigger the lowest tone, Low, and in (b), the two lower tones trigger a higher tone, Mid.

  1. (10)

Like the assimilating suffix, the suffix here is unspecified for the TRN, but it is specified as L for the TN, as is evident from the fact that the tone on the suffix is always in the lower range of the spectrum.

This set behaves oddly in not having a straightforward operation to explain the outcome. For the forms in (10b), we have the output structure in (11a), where the empty TRN is associated to H.

  1. (11)

I propose that this epenthesis of H, and ultimately the polarity pattern, is because of the OCP restriction between the TNs bearing the L feature. This comes into play only when the stem is a Mid or a Low – one with an L in the TN. In order to satisfy the OCP, we epenthesise a TN bearing an H, as shown in (11b). The advantage of the representation proposed here is that we can assume a constraint, OCP(◦–L), which prohibits two adjacent TNs dominating Ls, and is satisfied by the insertion of a TN associated with H, which is linked to the unspecified TRN to give the desired output. (A formal definition of OCP(◦–L) is given in (20) below.) In a representation without nodes, a simple OCP(L) constraint would force the insertion of just an H, which would then have to somehow move to a different plane or tier altogether. The advantage of a representation with nodes is that they always remain on their tiers; it is only the associations that change. It is the OCP effect and the cross-plane interaction which make this set ‘quirky’.

For the two cases in (10a), the OCP is not relevant, and the requirement to fill the empty node is simply fulfilled by epenthesis of a default L, as in (12).

  1. (12)

A question that arises is whether we are still dealing with polarity in this case. On the surface, we are. In (11b) the epenthesised tone feature is H in the suffix, a result of the OCP violation in the stem. In (12), the epenthesised tone feature is L in the suffix, a default insertion, because the presence of H in the stem does not lead to an OCP violation. Therefore, even in this case, H triggers an L, and L triggers an H – the standard case of polarity. The difference from the usual case of polarity is that this is subtonal: the polarity holds between tone features, not between whole tones.

The case discussed above provides further evidence for the use of tone features. Not only are there natural classes showing the subtonal divisions, but the subtonal features, like any other features, are also involved in the phonology, in this case by showing dissimilation amongst themselves.

3.2 OT implementation

In order to present the OT analysis of the data, I first look at the easier pattern, the assimilating suffixes, which requires a smaller list of constraints. Building on this, I will then present the more complex data with the quirky assimilating suffixes. Finally, I consider how the proposed constraint ranking accounts for the non-alternating suffixes, to verify that the OT grammar works well with all the data presented in this paper.

3.2.1 Analysis of the assimilating suffixes

I start by considering /zé-ʒə/ → [zé-ʒə́] from the assimilating set in (7a). As we saw in (9), the suffix lacks a TRN specification, and the unspecified node becomes associated to the register feature from the previous syllable. This association is triggered by the high-ranked constraint in (13), equivalent to Yip's (Reference Yip2002) Specify-T.

  1. (13)

We also require the *NodeShare constraint in (14), which specifies that a tone feature associated to the TRN in one syllable should not be linked to a TN from another syllable. As straightforward spreading of features between planes or tiers is not found, this constraint is needed to prohibit such interactions. The representations proposed in this paper allow interaction between features (or nodes) on the register and the melody tiers, as a result of features being able to occur on more than one tier. Other approaches have used separate features in each tier – for example, Yip (Reference Yip1980) uses [±Upper] for register and [±high] for melody – meaning that this interaction is not possible. The constraint in (14) dictates that when a feature is specified, its function as either a melody or a register feature must be retained. A register feature may therefore be shared between the TRNs of two TBUs – as in the case of the assimilating suffixes in (9) – and theoretically even between the TNs of two TBUs; however, it is marked for a feature to be shared between the TN of one TBU and the TRN of another.

  1. (14)

These constraints in (14) and (15) are more highly ranked than the faithfulness constraints Dep(H) and Dep(L), which prohibit the epenthesis of the H and L features respectively, as well as *Assoc, which is a faithfulness constraint prohibiting any additional association lines. These constraints, from Yip (Reference Yip2002), are formally defined in (15).

  1. (15)

The evaluation for /zé-ʒə/ → [zé-ʒə́] in (17) requires the ranking in (16).

  1. (16)

The faithful candidate (a) has a fully specified stem and an underspecified suffix, with the TN specified as H. The unspecified TRN in the suffix needs to be specified, and (a) is therefore eliminated by Spec(•). The need for specification triggers the association of the register feature of the stem to the TRN of the suffix, as in (b). This association is better than the epenthesis of a new feature, as in (c) and (d). The association of the melody feature on the TN of the stem to the empty TRN of the suffix (e) is blocked by *NodeShare. Candidate (b) ultimately emerges as the winning candidate.

  1. (17)

Similar evaluations will generate the other forms in (4), where the tone feature on the TRN of the stem is linked to the TRN of the underspecified suffix, resulting in raising or lowering of the suffix, depending on the register feature of the stem.

3.2.2 Analysis of the quirky alternating suffixes: default epenthesis

I now turn to the more complex pattern found in the quirky alternating suffixes in (10). I start by considering the instances in (10a), [ze̋-liè] and [zé-liè], with the higher stem tones and a Low suffix tone, as in (12). A new constraint is required here, one which prevents the association of a tone feature to the TRN when the TRN dominates a TN specified an L. In other words, this constraint, given in (18), says ‘don't associate to a syllable with L specified to its TN’, and captures the observation that suffixes like -lie do not share tone features with the stem.

  1. (18)

(18) is a language-specific constraint, and is a conjunction of a faithfulness constraint, with regards to the association, and a markedness constraint, with regards to the marked output structure.

The evaluation for /zé-lie/ → [zé-liè] is given in (19). As in (17), the faithful candidate (a) has an underspecified suffix. Here, epenthesis of the default feature L generates the winning candidate, (b), because association of any feature from the stem to the suffix is not possible. This results in the overall Low tone in the suffix. *T/TNL is also high-ranked, along with Spec(•) and *NodeShare. However, we need to tweak the ranking in (19) by ranking Dep(H) above Dep(L), to reflect the default epenthesis of the L feature.

  1. (19)

The evaluation for /ze̋-lie/ → [ze̋-liè] works in exactly the same way, with insertion of L in the underspecified suffix.

*T/TNL differentiates the two kinds of suffixes on the basis of the specified feature. For a suffix bearing an H on the TN, this constraint is vacuously satisfied and therefore enables association, making it assimilatory, while, for a suffix bearing an L in the TN, it prevents association, thereby eventually enabling epenthesis.

3.2.3 Analysis of the quirky alternating suffixes: the OCP

I now turn to the central case of this research, where we formalise the OCP as a constraint explaining the polar tone output. The case in point is the set in (10b), [zē-liē] and [zè-liē], where a Mid tone is found after the two lower tones. Here we need a constraint that disallows the insertion of L, instead inserting H, and thereby specifying the suffix as Mid. In order to achieve this, we need a constraint that is ranked higher than Dep(H). For this, we employ the OCP. If we assume a constraint OCP(◦–L), which bars adjacent TNs associated with L (crucially not just a constraint OCP(L)), the insertion of a TN bearing an H (as in (11b) above) generates the desired output. The OCP constraint is given in (20).

  1. (20)

A related constraint, OCP(◦–H), is also assumed, but is much lower-ranked in the language, and is therefore not included in the tableaux.

The constraint in (20) forms the crux of the investigation here. I use the OCP to explain the tone polarity pattern involving the insertion of (◦–H), thereby showing why an H is found in the presence of the polar environment L. In so doing, I avoid the use of all-powerful constraints such as Polar (Cahill Reference Cahill2004) (discussed in §4.3) to produce the pattern.

This constraint is paired with the lower-ranked constraint Dep(◦), which assigns violations for instances of epenthesis to the TN (◦).

At this point, we can generalise Spec(•) so that it holds not just for the TRN but also for the TN, as in (21).

  1. (21)

This constraint also prevents the insertion of any node which does not have a tone feature specified to it.

The evaluation for /zè-lie/ → [zè-liē] is presented in (22). In this case, the faithful candidate violates a second high-ranked constraint, OCP(◦–L), and a TN associated with H has to be inserted to avoid violating it. This H is linked to the empty node, thereby resulting in the suffix tone being a fully specified Mid. In the tableau, (c), which does not violate the OCP, is the winning candidate.

  1. (22)

A potential candidate not included in (22) involves the epenthesis of just (◦) instead of (◦–H). This avoids violating OCP(◦–L), but the candidate would be immediately rejected as an optimal candidate because it violates the highest-ranking constraint, Spec(•/◦), as it is not specified for any tone feature. Epenthesis of just (◦–H) without adding association lines, as in candidate (d) (which, like (c), does not violate OCP(◦–L)), violates *Float, defined in (23). This constraint is high-ranked, but is lower than Spec(•/◦) and OCP(◦–L).

  1. (23)

3.2.4 OT analysis for non-alternating suffixes

Finally, I consider the first dataset, the non-alternating suffixes (3), which involve no tonal changes. I assume that these suffixes are fully specified for tone, and so their tones do not change. The question then arises as to how the OCP behaves in this case. Given the constraint ranking, OCP(◦–L) is satisfied in the same way as in (23), by inserting (◦–H), as in candidate (24c). However, in this case there are no unspecified nodes available in the suffix for the epenthesised segment to associate to. It remains floating, without causing any phonetic change to the suffix.

The faithful candidate, (a), violates OCP(◦–L), with both syllables having an L associated with the TN. Repairing the OCP by inserting just the TN, as in (b), violates the constraint demanding specification of the TN. Candidate (c) satisfies OCP(◦–L) by inserting a TN associated with an H, creating a floating node which is not associated to any syllable; the phonetic output tone is not affected.

  1. (24)

4 Comparison with other approaches

4.1 Representations with features only

The tone features of Yip (Reference Yip1980) and Pulleyblank (Reference Pulleyblank1986) can be applied to Tenyidie by first dividing the four tones into two registers consisting of the higher two tones and the lower two tones respectively, using the feature [±Upper]. Each register is then further subdivided by the feature [±high], the melody (or the pitch/tone) feature. As shown in Table I, this provides a representation which correctly characterises for the natural classes identified above.

Table I Tonal decomposition (Yip 1980).

Using this representation, one can account for the assimilating suffixes with a rule spreading of the melody feature, as in (25).

  1. (25)

However, the quirky alternating suffixes prove difficult, as we would have to resort to the use of an alpha rule comparing values across different features, as in Gregersen (Reference Gregersen and Voeltz1974), where the value of the register is flipped and applied to the melody, giving (26).

  1. (26)

As we have seen, the formulation in (26) entails that polarity is a phonological process in itself. It employs a powerful mechanism where polarity is seen as a does-it-all tool to derive the end product. The alpha notation can be used for any number of combinations between any two unrelated features in the system in the formulation of a rule. This makes the rule too powerful, and the notation has been frowned upon in recent literature.

4.2 Autosegmental representation without nodes

If the tones in Tenyidie were to be represented without nodes, they would look like the autosegmental representations of Clements (Reference Clements and Dihoff1983). In such a model, the four tones have the representations in (27).

  1. (27)

The underspecified suffixes are represented as syllables consisting of a single feature, as in (28).

  1. (28)

The analysis of assimilating suffixes is similar even in this case, with the feature of the stem spreading to replace the unspecified feature in the assimilating suffix, as in (29a) for /zè-ʒə/ → [zè-ʒə́].

  1. (29)

The analysis of the quirky alternating suffix, on the other hand, is more complicated. Given OCP(L), epenthesis of H will take place on the register tier, as shown in (29b) for /zè-lie/ → [zè-liē]. The epenthesised H is moved down to the melody tier, which is realised in the suffix, giving [liē], with a Mid tone. This, however, presents a problem, in that it allows a feature to change its affiliation from one tier to another. Although the use of nodes, as in this paper, adds more machinery to the representation and therefore makes the model more powerful, it avoids the problem of features switching tiers, as the features are associated to fixed nodes.

Another disadvantage of the analysis without nodes is that it requires a two-step process. The first process is the default or OCP-triggered epenthesis, depending on the context. The second step, as in (29b), is the relocation of the epenthesised H. Such an analysis has also been proposed in Stratal Phonology and Stratal OT (Kiparsky Reference Kiparsky2000, Bermúdez-Otero Reference Bermúdez-Otero, Hannahs and Bosch2018). However, there is no other morphological evidence in Tenyidie to suggest that the use of strata is needed.

4.3 Non-phonological morphological alternations

An alternative way of looking at the polarity pattern could be that the tone alternation is just a case of special allomorphs being sensitive to the class of the stems. However, there is no evidence for the presence of classes in Tenyidie suffixes, and suffixes can be stacked up to show that the tonal pattern observed is purely phonological in nature. This is shown in §6.

The tone polarity analyses of Cahill (Reference Cahill2004) and Hantgan (Reference Hantgan2009) depend on the constraint Polar, which is defined very specifically on the plural morpheme. Cahill's definition is: ‘in a Noun Class 1 plural, the last tone of the plural is opposite in value to the immediately preceding tone’. The Agreement by Correspondence analysis of tone polarity in Shih & Inkelas (2019) also depends on a morpheme-specific constraint, [αT][αT]-Edge (Pl) (where ‘Pl’ = ‘plural’), which prohibits correspondence across a plural morpheme boundary. Related to these analyses are approaches in which polarity can be accounted for in terms of cophonologies (Orgun Reference Orgun1996, Anttila Reference Anttila2002, Inkelas & Zoll Reference Inkelas and Zoll2007) or indexed constraints in Morpheme-Specific Phonology (Pater Reference Pater, Bateman, O'Keefe, Reilly and Werle2007).

The advantage of my approach to tone polarity over the above-mentioned approaches is that, with the help of the tonal representation, the phenomenon can be analysed as purely phonological.

5 Polarity as an epiphenomenon

I have shown that the tone polarity observed in Tenyidie is actually an epiphenonemon, a reflex of OCP-driven epenthesis. The claim in this paper is that polarity in Tenyidie is merely a surface phenomenon, and that a linguistic mechanism of polarity triggering opposite values of adjacent elements is not necessary. This analysis is thus more economical. In addition, treating polarity as a reflex rather than a mechanism is preferable because the phenomenon is analysable on purely phonological terms, and there is no need to make reference to the morphology of the constituents. The Tenyidie case thus contributes to a body of evidence showing that many apparent cases of polarity are in fact best analysed by other means.

The debate on the existence of ‘true polarity’ (i.e. polarity as a mechanism) began with Kenstowicz et al. (Reference Kenstowicz, Nikiema and Ourso1988), where tone polarity was analysed as a reflex of the OCP. The absence, or at least the rarity, of true polarity in the world's languages raises doubts about the need for such a phenomenon (Trommer Reference Trommer2007, Bye Reference Bye, Kula, Botma and Nasukawa2010, de Lacy Reference de Lacy and Trommer2012). The use of alpha notation is considered too ‘powerful’ in de Lacy (2012). In the context of Tenyidie tone, the use of this notation can also be seen as too powerful, as this would imply that that features in different tiers can polarise each other, or as shown in §4.1, that polarity is found across features. A grammatical model of this sort would have wider undesirable consequences, such as predicting that any two different features could interact: for example [+fricative] might trigger [―voice], or vice versa. This would be too powerful, and also arbitrary, on the assumption that feature values are only conventions based on the names chosen for the feature – for instance, [–voice] could well have been [+voiceless]. The debate on phenomenon vs. epiphenomenon continues in morphophonological studies, with DiCanio et al. (Reference DiCanio, Cruz, Martínez and Cruz2020) arguing for an exchange rule (non-tonal) in Itunyoso Triqui, a claim which is countered by de Lacy (Reference de Lacy2020).

Mòoré, the classic case of tone polarity as an epiphenomenon (see the data in (1) above), was analysed with the use of a Meeussen's Rule type of dissimilation, where HH → HL, on the assumption that all suffixes were specified as underlyingly H. As a result, we find the sequence LH by default when the stem is L, but HL when the stem is H, resulting in surface tone polarity. Here, polarity is a result of the OCP changing an underlyingly given structure. Tenyidie is slightly different, since we have assumed that the suffix is underspecified. In this case, surface polarity arises because of the epenthesis of the polar feature as a result of the OCP, filling up an underlyingly unspecified unit. This is akin to Anttila & Bodomo's (Reference Anttila, Bodomo, Carstens and Parkinson2000) analysis of Dagaare, which has a similar polarity pattern to Mòoré. In their analysis, LH is the result of an L stem and a tonally unspecified suffix, with default epenthesis of H, while HL is the result of an H stem and a tonally unspecified suffix, with epenthesis of a non-default L as a consequence of an OCP(H) violation.

6 Stacking suffixes

The proposed representation also makes predictions as to how tones will change when suffixes are stacked, and these predictions are correctly borne out. (30) gives the derivation of a form containing the perfective -ta, an assimilating suffix, and two occurrences of the irrealis -lie, a quirky alternating suffix.

  1. (30)

Only the stem is fully specified; the TRNs of the three suffixes are unspecified. All the suffixes behave individually as they do when they are in a stem–suffix pair. The spreading of the register tone is seen in the suffix -ta, the default insertion of L in the first -lie, and the insertion of the TN with an H in the last suffix.

The language allows for the suffixes in (30) to be rearranged as /lè-lie-ta-lie/, as in (31), with a slight change in meaning. In this construction, tonal change in the suffixes is again based only on the tone of the immediately preceding syllable, showing that the tonal change is purely phonological.

  1. (31)

Here OCP(◦–L) applies between the stem and the first suffix, hence the insertion of the TN associated with H. This forms the register feature of the first suffix. The second suffix, -ta, an assimilating suffix, borrows the register feature of the first suffix by spreading of H. The third suffix prohibits spreading, and the OCP is not applicable here, so that we find default L-insertion.

Changing only the stem melody tone in (31) from Low in /lè/ to Extra High in /lə̋/ produces a different sequence of tones in the suffixes, as there is now no OCP-triggered H, rather default L epenthesis. This is demonstrated in (32).

  1. (32)

The examples in (33) show sequences of all the three kinds of suffix. The non-alternating (fully specified) suffix immediately follows the stem, and acts as the trigger for the rest of the suffixes, blocking the stems. This is followed by an assimilating suffix and finally a quirky alternating suffix.

  1. (33)

The stacking data here also proves problematic for the analysis of polarity with morpheme-specific constraints. Such approaches are myopic with respect to the data here, as they only see one suffix after the stem. The Tenyidie data shows that tone polarity is iterative, and not necessarily dependent on the stem only.

7 Conclusion

Tenyidie has four lexical tones, which can be represented distinctively using only two tone features, H and L, in a recursive autosegmental representation with a division between register and melody. The need for such a representation has been put forward in this paper, as opposed to using four independent individual features for each of the four tones.

Tenyidie has, broadly speaking, two kinds of suffixes. One kind of suffix, the non-alternating suffix, comes with its tone features specified, and displays no tonal changes on the surface. For the other kind, melody features are specified and register features are unspecified. Based on the tonal changes which these underspecified suffixes undergo, they can be further divided into two. Underspecified suffixes bearing an H in the melody simply borrow the register from the stem by spreading; these are the assimilating suffixes. The other kind, the quirky alternating suffixes, have an L in the melody. This set shows some kind of polarity, and therefore provides the core data for this paper. The data presented here adds further evidence for the analysis of tone polarity, in particular for analysing polarity in linguistics as being an epiphenomenon of the OCP, as suggested in previous work (Kenstowicz et al. Reference Kenstowicz, Nikiema and Ourso1988, Anttila & Bodomo Reference Anttila, Bodomo, Carstens and Parkinson2000). At the same time, this paper has also provided data showing a new kind of tone polarity, subtonal polarity. The data also shows that tone polarity is not confined to languages with two or three tones.

Using evidence from Seenku, McPherson (Reference McPherson, Payne, Pacchiarotti and Bosire2010) makes the claim that tone features should be preferred to tone primitives, against Hyman (Reference Hyman2010) and Clements et al. (Reference Clements, Michaud and Patin2010). This paper provides further evidence for tone features by showing that there are clear natural classes of tones supported by the data. The Tenyidie data also shows that these tone features drive processes of dissimilation patterns in the tone system.

Footnotes

Many thanks to the anonymous reviewers, the associate editor and the editors of Phonology for constructive comments and recommendations. I thank the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft for funding my research. Thanks to the researchers at Leipzig for their help, and special thanks to Yuni Kim for all the support and the additional help given.

1 African languages reported to show tone polarity are the following: Afro-Asiatic: Hausa (Newman Reference Newman and Goldsmith1995), Margi (Hoffmann Reference Hoffmann1963, Pulleyblank Reference Pulleyblank1983), Ngizim (Schuh Reference Schuh1971); Niger-Congo: Dagbani (Olawsky Reference Olawsky1996), Dagaare (Anttila & Bodomo Reference Anttila, Bodomo, Carstens and Parkinson2000), Kɔnni (Cahill Reference Cahill2004), Lama (Kenstowicz et al. Reference Kenstowicz, Nikiema and Ourso1988), Mòoré (Kenstowicz et al. Reference Kenstowicz, Nikiema and Ourso1988), Bambara (also known as Bamana; Dwyer Reference Dwyer, Hyman, Jacobson and Schuh1976, Creissels & Grégoire Reference Creissels and Grégoire1993), Mono (Olson Reference Olson2001), Gokana (Hyman Reference Hyman1985), Igbo (Welmers & Welmers Reference Welmers and Welmers1968), Yoruba (Akinlabi & Liberman Reference Akinlabi, Liberman, Gerlach and Grijzenhout2000); Nilo-Saharan: Fur (also known as Konjara; Lojenga Reference Kutsch Lojenga2002), Kanuri (Cyffer Reference Cyffer1991, Trommer Reference Trommer2005); Bangime (isolate; Hantgan Reference Hantgan2009).

2 In diphthongs such as /iē/, tone is marked on the second element, which is more prominent.

References

Akinlabi, Akinbiyi & Liberman, Mark (2000). The tonal phonology of Yoruba clitics. In Gerlach, Birgit & Grijzenhout, Janet (eds.) Clitics in phonology, morphology and syntax. Amsterdam: Benjamins. 3162.Google Scholar
Anttila, Arto (2002). Morphologically conditioned phonological alternations. NLLT 20. 142.Google Scholar
Anttila, Arto & Bodomo, Adams (2000). Tonal polarity in Dagaare. In Carstens, Vicki & Parkinson, Frederick (eds.) Advances in African linguistics. Trenton, NJ: Africa World Press. 119134.Google Scholar
Bermúdez-Otero, Ricardo (2018). Stratal Phonology. In Hannahs, S. J. & Bosch, Anna R. K. (eds.) The Routledge handbook of phonological theory. Abingdon & New York: Routledge. 100134.Google Scholar
Blankenship, Barbara, Ladefoged, Peter, Bhaskararao, Peri & Chase, Nichumeno (1993). Phonetic structures of Khonoma Angami. Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area 16:2. 6988.Google Scholar
Bye, Patrik (2010). Derivations. In Kula, Nancy C., Botma, Bert & Nasukawa, Kuniya (eds.) The Continuum companion to phonology. London: Continuum. 135173.Google Scholar
Cahill, Michael (2004). Tone polarity in Kɔnni nouns. Studies in African Linguistics 33. 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1983). The hierarchical representation of tone features. In Dihoff, Ivan R. (ed.) Current approaches to African linguistics. Vol. 1. Dordrecht: Foris. 145176.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N., Michaud, Alexis & Patin, Cédric (2010). Do we need tone features? In Goldsmith et al. () 324.Google Scholar
Creissels, Denis & Grégoire, Claire (1993). La notion de ton marqué dans l'analyse d'une opposition tonale binaire: le cas du mandingue. Journal of African Languages and Linguistics 14. 107154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cyffer, Norbert (1991). We learn Kanuri. Cologne: Rüdiger Köppe.Google Scholar
de Lacy, Paul (2012). Morphophonological polarity. In Trommer, Jochen (ed.) The morphology and phonology of exponence. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 121159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Lacy, Paul (2020). Do morphophonological exchange rules exist? A reply to Dicanio et al. (2020). Phonological Data and Analysis 2:4. 2943. https://doi.org/10.3765/pda.v2no4.46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DiCanio, Christian, Cruz, Basileo Martínez, Martínez, Benigno Cruz & Cruz, Wilberto Martínez (2020). Glottal toggling in Itunyoso Triqui. Phonological Data and Analysis 2:4. 128. https://doi.org/10.3765/pda.v2art4.3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutta, Indranil, Ezung, Mimi Kevichüsa, Mahanta, Shakuntala & Meyase, Savio M. (2012). Lexical tones of Tenyidie: fundamental frequency, voice quality and pitch modeling. Paper presented at the Workshop on Tone and Intonation, IIT Guwahati.Google Scholar
Dwyer, David (1976). The analysis of Bambara polarization. In Hyman, Larry M., Jacobson, Leon C. & Schuh, Russell G. (eds.) Papers in African linguistics in honor of Wm. E. Welmers. Studies in African Linguistics. Supplement 6. 2738.Google Scholar
Evans, Jonathan P. (2008). ‘African’ tone in the Sinosphere. Language and Linguistics 9. 463490.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, John A., Hume, Elizabeth & Leo Wetzels, W. (eds.) (2010). Tones and features: phonetic and phonological perspectives. Berlin & Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Gregersen, Edgar A. (1974). Consonant polarity in Nilotic. In Voeltz, Erhard (ed.) 3rd Annual Conference on African Linguistics. Bloomington: Indiana University. 105109.Google Scholar
Hantgan, Abbie (2009). Does tone polarity exist? Evidence from plural formation among Bangime nouns. Indiana University Working Papers in Linguistics 8. 6983.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, Carl (1963). A grammar of the Margi language. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (1985). A theory of phonological weight. Dordrecht: Foris. Reprinted 2003, Stanford: CSLI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (1993). Register tones and tonal geometry. In van der Hulst, Harry & Snider, Keith (eds.) The phonology of tone: the representation of tonal register. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 75108.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2007). Kuki-Thaadow: an African tone system in Southeast Asia. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report. 119.Google Scholar
Hyman, Larry M. (2010). Do tones have features? In Goldsmith et al. (). 5080.Google Scholar
Inkelas, Sharon & Zoll, Cheryl (2007). Is grammar dependence real? A comparison between cophonological and indexed constraint approaches to morphologically conditioned phonology. Linguistics 45. 133171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael, Nikiema, Emmanuel & Ourso, Meterwa (1988). Tonal polarity in two Gur languages. Studies in the Linguistic Sciences 18. 77103.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul (2000). Opacity and cyclicity. The Linguistic Review 17. 351365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutsch Lojenga, Constance (2002). Tone polarity in Fur. Paper presented at the 33rd Annual Conference on African Linguistics, Athens, OH.Google Scholar
Kuolie, Duovituo (2006). Structural description of Tenyidie: a Tibeto-Burman language of Nagaland. Kohima: Ura Academy Publication Division.Google Scholar
Lin, You-Jing (2012). By no means marginal: privative tone in Zhuokeji Rgyalrong. Language and Linguistics 13. 625662.Google Scholar
McPherson, Laura (2010). Tone features revisited: evidence from Seenku. In Payne, Doris L., Pacchiarotti, Sara & Bosire, Mokaya (eds.) Diversity in African languages. Berlin: Language Science Press. 522.Google Scholar
Mahanta, Shakuntala & Gope, Amalesh (2018). Tonal polarity in Sylheti in the context of noun faithfulness. Language Sciences 69. 8097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyase, Savio M. (2014). Four versus five: the number of tones in Tenyidie. In Carlos Gussenhoven, Yiya Chen & Dan Dediu (eds.) Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Tonal Aspects of Languages. 19–22. Available (February 2021) at http://www.isca-speech.org/archive/tal_2014.Google Scholar
Meyase, Savio M. (2016). A morphological investigation of the tonal phonology of Tenyidie. PhD dissertation, English and Foreign Languages University, Hyderabad.Google Scholar
Newman, Paul (1995). Hausa tonology: complexities in an ‘easy’ tone language. In Goldsmith, John A. (ed.) The handbook of phonological theory. Cambridge, MA & Oxford: Blackwell. 762781.Google Scholar
Olawsky, Knut J. (1996). An introduction to Dagbani phonology. Düsseldorf: Arbeiten des SFB.Google Scholar
Olson, Kenneth S. (2001). The phonology and morphology of Mono. PhD dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Orgun, C. Orhan (1996). Sign-based morphology and phonology with special attention to Optimality Theory. PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Ozerov, Pavel (2018). Tone assignment and grammatical tone in Anal (Tibeto-Burman). Studies in Language 42. 708733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pater, Joe (2007). The locus of exceptionality: morpheme-specific phonology as constraint indexation. In Bateman, Leah, O'Keefe, Michael, Reilly, Ehren & Werle, Adam (eds.) Papers in Optimality Theory III. Amherst: GLSA. 259296.Google Scholar
Picanço, Gessiane (2002). Tone polarity as phonologically conditioned allomorphy in Mundurukú. BLS 28. 237248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1983). Extratonality and polarity. WCCFL 2. 204216.Google Scholar
Pulleyblank, Douglas (1986). Tone in Lexical Phonology. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Schuh, Russell G. (1971). Verb forms and verb aspects in Ngizim. Journal of African Languages 10. 4760.Google Scholar
Shih, Stephanie S. & Inkelas, Sharon (2018). Autosegmental aims in surface-optimizing phonology. LI 50. 137196.Google Scholar
Snider, Keith L. (1999). The geometry and features of tone. Arlington: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Trommer, Jochen (2005). Polar tone in Kanuri. Ms. Available (February 2021) at https://home.uni-leipzig.de/jtrommer/papers/papers.html.Google Scholar
Trommer, Jochen (2007). Voicing polarity in Luo. Ms. Available (February 2021) at https://home.uni-leipzig.de/jtrommer/papers/papers.html.Google Scholar
Watkins, Justin (2013). A first look at tone in Myebon Sumtu Chin. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics 16. 79104.Google Scholar
Welmers, Beatrice F. & Welmers, William E. (1968). Igbo: a learner's dictionary. African Studies Center, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira (1980). The tonal phonology of Chinese. PhD dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table I Tonal decomposition (Yip 1980).