Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T13:57:36.994Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gastrointestinal nematode infection during pregnancy and lactation enhances spatial reference memory and reduces indicators of anxiety-like behaviour in uninfected adult female mouse offspring

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 May 2024

Sophia Noel
Affiliation:
Institute of Parasitology, McGill University (Macdonald Campus), Quebec H9X 3V9, Canada
Ryan LaFrancois
Affiliation:
Institute of Parasitology, McGill University (Macdonald Campus), Quebec H9X 3V9, Canada
Marilyn E. Scott*
Affiliation:
Institute of Parasitology, McGill University (Macdonald Campus), Quebec H9X 3V9, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Marilyn E. Scott; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Maternal bacterial and viral infections that induce neuroinflammation in the developing brain are associated with impaired cognitive function and increased anxiety in the offspring. In contrast, maternal infection with the immunoregulatory murine gastrointestinal (GI) nematode, Heligmosomoides bakeri, appears to benefit neurodevelopment as juvenile 2- and 3-week-old male and female offspring had enhanced spatial memory, which may be due to a Th2/Treg biased neuroimmune environment. Here, the impact of maternal H. bakeri infection during pregnancy and lactation on the spatial and anxiety-like behaviours of adult, 3-month-old uninfected male and female offspring was explored for the first time. It was observed that adult female offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams had enhanced spatial reference memory and reduced anxiety-like behaviour compared to females of uninfected dams. These effects were not observed in adult male offspring. Thus, the positive influence of a maternal GI nematode infection on spatial memory of juvenile offspring persists in adult female offspring.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Brain development is a highly plastic process that, in rodents, starts in utero and continues postnatally until 3 months of age when brain maturation is completed (Hammelrath et al., Reference Hammelrath, Škokić, Khmelinskii, Hess, Van Der Knaap, Staring, Lelieveldt, Wiedermann and Hoehn2016). During this vulnerable period, environmental stimuli such as maternal physical exercise favour brain development (Robinson and Bucci, Reference Robinson and Bucci2014; Gomes da Silva et al., Reference Gomes da Silva, De Almeida, Fernandes, Lopim, Cabral, Scerni, De Oliveira-Pinto, Lent and Arida2016), whereas exposure to bacterial and viral infections or toxins during pregnancy impairs neurodevelopment (Boksa, Reference Boksa2010; Wilhelm and Guizzetti, Reference Wilhelm and Guizzetti2015; Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019; Beversdorf et al., Reference Beversdorf, Stevens, Margolis and Van De Water2019). There has been a large effort over the past 2 decades to understand the link between maternal exposure to these pathogens and the risk of neurological disorders in offspring with a developmental origin, including autism spectrum disorder and schizophrenia (Boksa, Reference Boksa2010; Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019). With the help of rodent models, it is now understood that the maternal immune response, not a specific pathogen, is a risk factor for neurodevelopmental disorders (Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019). Viral or bacterial pathogens or their mimics [polyinosinic–polycytidylic acid (Poly I:C) or lipopolysaccharide (LPS) respectively] induce a strong pro-inflammatory immune response in the mother, which extends to the offspring, resulting in an altered immune profile in the developing brain which ultimately leads to irreversible neurodevelopmental defects and the emergence of behavioural abnormalities and cognitive impairments (Boksa, Reference Boksa2010; Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019). Interestingly, however, it was observed that maternal exposure to the immunoregulatory gastrointestinal (GI) nematode, Heligmosomoides bakeri (also referred to as Heligmosomoides polygyrus and previously known as Nematospiroides dubius), may actually benefit, not harm, at least some aspects of brain development of the offspring (Haque et al., Reference Haque, Koski and Scott2019; El Ahdab et al., Reference El Ahdab, Haque, Madogwe, Koski and Scott2021; Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022, Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024). In contrast to the type 1 pro-inflammatory immune responses [e.g. interferon-γ, tumour necrosis factor-α, CD4 + T helper type 1 (Th1) cells] triggered by bacterial or viral infections, similar to most GI nematodes, infection with H. bakeri elicits a type 2 host resistance and tolerizing immune response [e.g. interleukin (IL)-4, IL-5, IL-13, Th2 cells] (Maizels et al., Reference Maizels, Hewitson, Murray, Harcus, Dayer, Filbey, Grainger, Mcsorley, Reynolds and Smith2012; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Filbey and Maizels2012; Chen et al., Reference Chen, Cao, Liu, Diamond and Jin2023). Heligmosomoides bakeri also induces an immunoregulatory network that aids long-term survival in its host, involving proliferation of Foxp3 + CD4 + regulatory T (Tregs) cells and the potent immunoregulatory cytokines IL-10 and transforming growth factor (TGF-β) (Maizels et al., Reference Maizels, Hewitson, Murray, Harcus, Dayer, Filbey, Grainger, Mcsorley, Reynolds and Smith2012; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Filbey and Maizels2012). This also allows the nematode to dampen inflammatory and pathologic processes, and prevent or ameliorate a number of hyper-immune/inflammatory diseases (Elliott et al., Reference Elliott, Setiawan, Metwali, Blum, Urban and Weinstock2004; Wilson and Maizels, Reference Wilson and Maizels2006; Saunders et al., Reference Saunders, Raine, Cooke and Lawrence2007; Smallwood et al., Reference Smallwood, Giacomin, Loukas, Mulvenna, Clark and Miles2017; White et al., Reference White, Johnston, Grainger, Konkel, O'connor, Anderton and Maizels2020).

Brain gene expression in 7-day-old neonatal male offspring born to H. bakeri-infected CD-1 outbred dams revealed upregulation of 5 key interacting pathways associated with long-term potentiation (LTP) (Haque et al., Reference Haque, Koski and Scott2019), the cellular mechanism of learning and memory (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Han, Li, Bai, Wang, Tu, Peng, Zhou, He, Wu, Tan, Liu, Wu, Zhou, Jin, Zhang, Sacktor, Li, Song and Wang2015). This was consistent with gene expression and electrophysiological data indicating enhanced hippocampal LTP in 3-week-old uninfected offspring (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024). The hippocampus is involved in cognitive functions, and also plays an important role in the regulation of emotional behaviours, particularly anxiety (Ghasemi et al., Reference Ghasemi, Navidhamidi, Rezaei, Azizikia and Mehranfard2022). Spatial learning and memory in rodents is critically dependent on hippocampal synaptic plasticity (Clark and Martin, Reference Clark and Martin2005; Dringenberg, Reference Dringenberg2020); thus, enhanced hippocampal LTP was consistent with the enhanced spatial memory that was observed in 2- and 3-week-old uninfected male and female offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022). Furthermore, maternal H. bakeri infection resulted in a Th2/Treg biased neuroimmune environment in the uninfected offspring. Gene expression analysis of the brains of neonates of infected dams revealed upregulated Th2/Treg pathways, including the genes for the potent immunoregulatory cytokines IL-4 and TGF-β, and downregulated Th1/Th17 pathways (Haque et al., Reference Haque, Koski and Scott2019; El Ahdab et al., Reference El Ahdab, Haque, Madogwe, Koski and Scott2021), mimicking the immune response of the infected mother (Odiere et al., Reference Odiere, Scott, Leroux, Dzierszinski and Koski2013; Su et al., Reference Su, Chen, Mao, Chen, Steudel, Jiao, Lan, Fasano, Walker and Shi2023). This altered neuroimmune environment was also seen in the hippocampus of male and female 3-week-old juvenile offspring where the immunoregulatory TGF-β signalling pathway was upregulated, and where a greater number of 2 immune sensitive cells, microglia and astrocytes, were observed, as well as a higher percentage of CD206-positive microglia (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024) which are typically increased in response to the Th2 cytokine, IL-4 (Francos-Quijorna et al., Reference Francos-Quijorna, Amo-Aparicio, Martinez-Muriana and López-Vales2016; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Liu, Zhao, Zhang, Cai, Cai, Ji, Leak, Gao, Chen and Hu2016; Jurga et al., Reference Jurga, Paleczna and Kuter2020; Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Rong, Zhang, He, Zhou, Fan, Mo, Zhao, Han, Li, Wang, Yan, Chen and You2021). These findings reveal a potential mechanism behind the enhanced LTP and spatial memory of uninfected offspring, as both TGF-β and IL-4 are known to have measurable downstream effects on LTP and spatial memory (Nolan et al., Reference Nolan, Maher, Martin, Clarke, Brady, Bolton, Mills and Lynch2005; Fukushima et al., Reference Fukushima, Liu and Byrne2007; Derecki et al., Reference Derecki, Cardani, Yang, Quinnies, Crihfield, Lynch and Kipnis2010; Gadani et al., Reference Gadani, Cronk, Norris and Kipnis2012; Caraci et al., Reference Caraci, Gulisano, Guida, Impellizzeri, Drago, Puzzo and Palmeri2015). Whether the improved spatial memory extends to adult offspring is, however, unknown.

The Barnes maze test (BMT) has been previously used to assess spatial learning and memory of 3-week-old offspring in response to maternal H. bakeri infection (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022). This test assesses hippocampus-dependent spatial reference memories formed over repeated trials in an unchanging environment (Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Rakoczy and Brown-Borg2010) by measuring the ability of rodents to learn and recall the location of a goal box which is located under 1 of 20 holes around the perimeter of a platform (Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, Rakoczy and Brown-Borg2010). It involves a habituation trial (day 0), a training phase (days 1–4) to test spatial learning, both of which contain the goal box, and probe trials 1 (day 5) and 2 (day 12), with no goal box, to test short- and long-term spatial reference memory, respectively. Of note, although the BMT is primarily designed to assess spatial learning and memory, it can also be used to explore 2 indicators of anxiety-like behaviour. First, when rodents are initially placed in an environment that is distinctly different from any environment they have previously encountered, they become afraid or anxious, and defecate (Denenberg, Reference Denenberg1969; Bailey and Crawley, Reference Bailey, Crawley and Buccafusco2011). Thus, quantifying the number of fecal pellets during the 5 min habituation trial when mice are first introduced to the BMT can be used as an indicator of their anxiety/fear level, with a greater number of fecal pellets indicating a higher level of anxiety/fear (Denenberg, Reference Denenberg1969; Bailey and Crawley, Reference Bailey, Crawley and Buccafusco2011). Second, mice tend to favour darker, more enclosed spaces and thus avoid exploring open areas, especially when they are brightly lit (Bailey and Crawley, Reference Bailey, Crawley and Buccafusco2011). Thus, during the BMT training trials, mice are motivated to seek shelter from the brightly lit, exposed maze, by entering the dark enclosed goal box. It can therefore be interpreted that mice that find the goal box, but choose instead to continue exploring the maze, may be less fearful/anxious than mice who immediately enter the goal box upon finding it.

Using the BMT, the goal of this study was to determine if the positive impact of maternal H. bakeri infection during pregnancy and lactation on spatial memory of juvenile offspring was retained in adult offspring. Indicators of anxiety-like behaviour in these offspring were also explored. This study presents the first evidence that enhanced spatial memory previously observed in juvenile offspring in response to a maternal GI nematode infection is retained in uninfected adult female, but not male, offspring, and that these adult females also have reduced anxiety-like behaviour compared to female offspring of uninfected dams.

Materials and methods

Experimental design

A 2 × 2 factorial design was employed using H. bakeri infected vs uninfected dams, and their male vs female offspring. All procedures were approved by the McGill University Animal Care Committee according to the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care.

Mice and parasites

Of the 40 primiparous 8-week-old timed pregnant outbred CD-1 mice that were received from Charles River Laboratories (Quebec, Canada) on gestation day (GD) 4, 31 were pregnant (78% pregnancy rate). Each dam with her litter was housed individually in a Nalgene cage (Fisher Scientific, Montreal, Canada) at 21–23°C, 40–60% relative humidity and a 12 h light and dark cycle. Mice had ad libitum access to a 2920X Teklad rodent diet (18% crude protein, 5% crude fat, 5% crude fibre). Within each of the 6 staggered groups of dams received over 3 months, dams were randomized into uninfected and infected groups, providing a total of 15–16 dams per group were used for this study. This provided an acceptable sample size based on a minimum of at least 6 dams per treatment condition (Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Feldon and Fatemi2009). Using standard H. bakeri protocols (Johnston et al., Reference Johnston, Robertson, Harcus, Grainger, Coakley, Smyth, Mcsorley and Maizels2015), infective L3 were obtained by fecal culture of stock parasites maintained in outbred CD-1 mice. In our previous studies on the impact of maternal H. bakeri infection on uninfected offspring (Haque et al., Reference Haque, Koski and Scott2019; Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022, Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), a trickle infection protocol was used to maximize antigenic stimulation during pregnancy and lactation and to simulate ongoing natural infection that occurs in wild mice (Brailsford and Behnke, Reference Brailsford and Behnke1992). This same protocol was used here. Dams in the infected group were intubated using an oral gavage needle with 100 ± 3 L3 suspended in 0.1 mL distilled water on GD 7, 12, 17, and postnatal day (PD) 3, 8, and 13 (Fig. 1). Uninfected dams were intubated at the same frequency with 0.1 mL distilled water, to control for any stress due to handling. Given that H. bakeri eggs released into the environment develop into infective larvae after 7 days, all cages were cleaned every 5 days to ensure offspring could not ingest infective larvae. Dams were weighed on GD 7, 12, and 17. Following weaning (PD 20), dams were euthanized and necropsied to confirm successful infection of dams based on the presence of adult worms in the small intestine.

Figure 1. Schematic representation of experimental design and protocol. Of the 40 timed-pregnant dams received on gestation day (GD) 4, only 31 delivered litters. On postnatal day (PD) 20, 1 pup per sex per litter was selected to perform the Barnes maze test. Of the pups selected for behavioural analysis, their size, specifically crown-rump length and weight, were recorded on PD 20 and 69 (see Supplementary Fig. 2).

Pups were born on GD 19, litter size was recorded on PD 3, 8, 13, and 20, and body mass and length from the top of the head to the base of the tail were recorded on PD 20 and 69. At PD 20, pups were weaned, sexed, and given a unique identifier with a permanent marker, and 1 male and 1 female pup per litter were randomly selected for the BMT (Fig. 1). Pups selected for the adult BMT were housed with 2 littermates of the same sex until testing was performed 2 months later. After the BMT, experimental pups were necropsied and intestines were examined for adult H. bakeri to confirm that they had not accidentally become infected. Remaining pups were used for a separate experiment (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024).

Experimental room

The BMT was conducted in a quiet room (340 cm × 260 cm) which was brightly lit (a floor lamp in each corner and an over-head light) to provide a mild negative reinforcement. Trials were recorded using an overhead monochromatic video camera (Basler Ace monochrome) connected to a computer located in the back corner of the room behind a curtain. The experimenter remained behind the curtain during all recordings. Data were extracted from the videos using the Ethovision XT software (version 17). All equipment remained in the same location in the room, providing visual spatial cues.

Barnes maze test

The BMT procedure followed a protocol (Sunyer et al., Reference Sunyer, Patil, Hoger and Lubec2007) that was previously used to successfully test spatial learning and short- and long-term reference memory in juvenile CD-1 mice born to uninfected or H. bakeri-infected dams (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022), as well as adult CD-1 mice born to uninfected dams (Patil et al., Reference Patil, Sunyer, Hoger and Lubec2009). As previously described (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022), the Barnes Maze (Maze engineers, 412 Wilmette Ave, Glenview, IL 60025, USA) is an opaque circular platform (diameter: 92 cm, height: 70 cm) with 20 equally spaced holes (diameter: 5 cm) located 2 cm from the edge. In a brightly lit environment, mice naturally seek the dark enclosed area provided by the black goal box (20 × 10 × 4 cm) located under the same escape hole throughout all trials. From the surface of the maze, the escape hole, containing the goal box, looks identical to the other 19 holes. Mice learn the location of the goal box based on spatial cues in the room.

The BMT was conducted when offspring were 3 months old. The BMT involved a habituation trial (day 0), a training phase (days 1–4) to test spatial learning and probe trials 1 (day 5) and 2 (day 12) to test short- and long-term spatial reference memory, respectively. Home cages were moved into the experimental room for 15–20 min acclimation prior to trials and all equipment was cleaned with 70% ethanol between trials.

The habituation trial was used to introduce the mouse to the apparatus and reduce anxiety during the test. The mouse was placed in an opaque starting cylinder (diameter: 10.5 cm, height: 8 cm) at the centre of the platform. After 10 s, the cylinder was removed, and the animal was allowed to freely explore the apparatus for 5 min. After 5 min, the mouse was guided to the goal box and remained there for 2 min. During the habituation trial, the number of fecal droppings was counted, as an indicator of anxiety (Denenberg, Reference Denenberg1969).

Training involved four 3 min trials per day for 4 training days. Each of the 16 training trials began by placing the mouse in the starting cylinder at the centre of the platform. After 10 s, the cylinder was removed, recording began and the animal was allowed to freely explore the apparatus for 3 min. Once the animal entered the goal box, it was allowed to remain there for 1 min. Mice that failed to enter the goal box within 3 min were gently guided to its location and placed inside. After each of the four 3 min training trials per day, mice were returned to their home cage for 20 min. During the training trials, the following variables were recorded: (1) total latency (s), defined as time taken until the mouse enters the goal box; (2) total distance (cm), defined as distance travelled until the mouse enters the goal box; (3) total errors, defined as number of times the mouse visited non-escape holes (noted as nose pokes into holes), before entering the goal box; and (4) mean velocity (cm s−1) used to determine if performance differences reflected motor ability. If a mouse did not enter the goal box during a 3 min training trial, 180 s was entered as their total latency, and their number of errors and distance travelled during the 3 min trial were entered as total errors and total distance, respectively. As previous studies have observed that mice may find the goal box but choose to continue exploring the maze (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Reiserer, Tomarken and Mcdonald2006; Patil et al., Reference Patil, Sunyer, Hoger and Lubec2009), it is recommended to also record latency, distance and number of errors to the first encounter (nose poke) of the escape hole, called primary latency, primary distance and primary errors, respectively, to assess spatial learning. The number of trials where mice did not enter the goal box was also recorded.

Prior to probe trials 1 and 2, the goal box was removed from the escape hole and mice explored the maze for 90 s. No training occurred between the 2 probe trials. Primary latency, primary distance and primary errors were recorded during the probe trials.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed in R statistical software 4.2.3 (R Core Team, 2020), and figures were produced using GraphPad Prism V9. For comparisons over time, where there were repeated measures (i.e. pup size and spatial learning), models were built with maternal treatment condition (H. bakeri infected vs uninfected), offspring sex (male vs female) and timepoint/trial included as a fixed factors and the identity of the mouse as a random factor (Lazic, Reference Lazic2010). A similar model was built for dam weight. Litter size was included as a covariate when assessing mouse size (pup and dam). Further, as body weight has been shown to negatively influence spatial learning (Cordner and Tamashiro, Reference Cordner and Tamashiro2015), and we observed this in our study, offspring weight was included as a covariate in our models for spatial learning. No association was found between body weight and spatial memory performance, thus body weight was not included as a covariate in these models. Non-significant interactions between fixed effects were excluded from models (Zuur et al., Reference Zuur, Ieno, Walker, Saveliev and Smith2009). As trends in the anxiety-like behaviour and spatial memory data were observed, where differences were evident between females born to H. bakeri infected vs uninfected dams but not between males, data from male and female offspring were analysed separately. This approach is often taken in the literature (Lante et al., Reference Lante, Meunier, Guiramand, Maurice, Cavalier, De Jesus Ferreira, Aimar, Cohen-Solal, Vignes and Barbanel2007; Batinić et al., Reference Batinić, Santrač, Divović, Timić, Stanković, Obradović, Joksimović and Savić2016); thus, models were built with maternal treatment condition (H. bakeri infected vs uninfected) as the fixed factor.

Extreme outliers were identified in Prism using the ROUT method (a method combining Robust regression and Outlier removal). The strictest cut off of Q = 0.1% was selected to reduce the chance of falsely detecting outliers, meaning only extreme outliers were identified and removed. Extreme outliers occurred only in probe trial 1 where 2 male treatment mice were outliers for primary latency, distance and errors. Analyses for probe 1 were performed with these mice included and excluded and exclusion of these outliers did not influence the results.

Using the fisher.test function in R, Fisher's exact tests were used to analyse the number of trials where mice did or did not enter the goal box during the training phase; male and female offspring were analysed separately. For remaining variables, linear models (LM), negative binomial generalized linear models (NB.GLM), linear mixed models (LMMs) or generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) were built using the lm, glm.nb, nlme or glmer function, respectively [MASS package (Venables and Ripley, Reference Venables and Ripley2002), nlme package (Pinheiro, Reference Pinheiro2023) and lme4 package (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Steve2015)]. When necessary, post hoc pairwise comparisons were performed using the emmeans function [emmeans package (Lenth, Reference Lenth2020)] with a Tukey correction. Normality, independence and homogeneity of variances of mixed models were assessed using fitted residuals from the plotresid function [RVAideMemoire package (Hervé, Reference Hervé2020)], and in the case of GLMMs, also using the DHARMa package (Hartig, Reference Hartig2020). Unless otherwise stated, values are presented as means ± s.e.m. The significance level was set at 0.05.

As no pup mortality occurred, the influence of the maternal infection status on litter size was analysed on PD 20 using a LM. For dam weight and offspring weight and length, measured over time, LMMs were used with log transformations for weight and length.

Data on number of fecal droppings were discrete and overdispersed and were analysed using NB.GLMs. Variables from the training and probe trials were positively skewed, and in some instances, heteroscedastic. For the training phase, LMMs with log transformations were used for total latency, total distance and mean velocity and Gamma GLMMs, with log link function, were used for primary latency and primary distance. Both total and primary errors were discrete and overdispersed, and negative binomial GLMMs, with log link function, were used. For the probe trials, LMs with log transformations were used for primary latency and primary distance and a NB.GLM was used for primary error.

Results

This study assessed the influence of maternal H. bakeri infection on the spatial learning and memory and anxiety-like behaviour of uninfected male and female adult offspring in the BMT. Outbred CD-1 mice were infected repeatedly or sham-infected during pregnancy and lactation and their 3-month-old adult offspring from 15 uninfected and 16 H. bakeri-infected dams were used (1 male and 1 female pup per dam). Mortality was consistently zero in this infection model, as expected (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022).

Impact of maternal infection on dam mass, litter size, and pup size

Maternal infection did not influence dam mass at GD 7, 12 or 17 (all P values >0.05) or litter size (uninfected: 12.2 ± 0.4; infected: 11.8 ± 0.3; P = 0.44) (Supplementary Fig. 1). As reported in the literature (Kristan, Reference Kristan2002; Odiere et al., Reference Odiere, Scott, Weiler and Koski2010; Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022), male and female pups born to infected dams had lower mass and shorter length than pups of uninfected dams at PD 20 (all P values <0.001, Supplementary Fig. 2). For the first time, it was observed that this impaired growth persisted in adult offspring at PD 69 (all P values <0.001, Supplementary Fig. 2). As expected, mass and length were greater in males than females (Kristan, Reference Kristan2002; Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022) at both PD 20 and PD 69 (all P values <0.01, Supplementary Fig. 2).

Impact of maternal infection on offspring anxiety-like behaviour and spatial learning and memory in the Barnes maze test

Anxiety-like behaviour during habituation trial

Defecation is a sign of fear or anxiety in rodents, which is often observed when they are placed in a novel environment (Denenberg, Reference Denenberg1969; Bailey and Crawley, Reference Bailey, Crawley and Buccafusco2011). Thus, the number of fecal pellets was counted during the 5 min habituation trial when mice were introduced to the maze, as a measure of anxiety-like behaviour. Female offspring of H. bakeri-infected mothers had significantly fewer fecal pellets than female offspring of uninfected mothers (P = 0.02; Fig. 2), suggesting that maternal infection during pregnancy and lactation reduced anxiety in their uninfected adult female offspring. This difference was not observed in the males.

Figure 2. Maternal H. bakeri infection reduced female offspring fecal count during the 5 min habituation trial of the Barnes maze test. Values are means±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group (*P < 0.05; ns = not significant).

Spatial learning and anxiety-like behaviour in the training phase

As mice may continue to explore the maze after finding the goal box, 2 sets of spatial learning variables were explored, those related to first arriving at the escape hole which contained the goal box (‘primary’ variables), and those related to entering the goal box (‘total’ variables). Primary variables were a better indication of spatial learning whereas total variables were used to assess spatial exploration as an indicator of anxiety-like behaviour.

Spatial learning: Regardless of maternal infection or offspring sex, adult offspring learned the location of the escape hole, based on the first nose poke into the escape hole, on the first training day as indicated by a decrease in the average primary latency (P < 0.0001; Fig. 3a), primary distance (P < 0.0001; Fig. 3b) and primary errors (P < 0.0001; Fig. 3c) between training days 1 and 2. Thereafter, values remained low. Neither maternal infection nor offspring sex influenced mean velocity (Fig. 3d), indicating no differences in motor ability. Of note, independent of maternal infection, primary latency and primary distance were negatively associated with offspring weight (all P values < 0.05, data not shown).

Figure 3. Neither maternal H. bakeri infection nor adult offspring sex influenced primary variables of spatial learning in the Barnes maze test over 4 training days. All mice performed significantly better after the first training day. Values are LSmeans±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group. (a) Primary latency, (b) primary distance and (c) number of primary errors to reach the escape hole, and (d) mean velocity during the trial. Different letters show the effect of training day, P < 0.05.

Anxiety-like behaviour: Adult female offspring of infected mothers had higher total latency (P = 0.044; Fig. 4a), total distance (P = 0.058; Fig. 4b) and total errors (P = 0.043; Fig. 4c) compared to female offspring of uninfected mothers. No differences were observed between adult male offspring of uninfected and infected mothers in total latency, total distance and total errors (Supplementary Fig. 3). Furthermore, although all mice found the goal box during each training trial, a significantly higher percentage of females of infected dams did not enter the goal box (17.2%) compared with females of uninfected dams (4.6%) (P < 0.0001). There was no difference in the percentage of training trials where males did not enter the goal box (males of infected mothers: 6.6%; males of uninfected mothers: 4.6%; P = 0.34). Taken together, these data indicate that females of infected mothers were less anxious as they were less motivated to seek shelter in the goal box during training trials, and more inclined to explore the maze after finding the goal box.

Figure 4. Maternal H. bakeri infection influenced female offspring exploration during the 4-day training phase of the Barnes maze test. Total parameters were used as an indication of exploration to provide an understanding of fear/anxiety levels. Values are LSmeans±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group. (a) Total latency, (b) total distance and (c) number of total errors to enter the goal box.

Probe trials of spatial memory

During probe trial 1, which assessed short-term spatial reference memory, maternal infection did not influence the time taken (primary latency) or distance travelled (primary distance) to find the escape hole for adult female offspring (Fig. 5a–b). However, adult female offspring of infected mothers made half as many primary errors before first finding the escape hole compared to offspring of uninfected mothers (P = 0.043; Fig. 5c) indicating better short-term spatial reference memory in response to maternal H. bakeri infection. These differences were not detected in the adult male offspring (Fig. 5a–c).

Figure 5. Maternal H. bakeri infection influenced adult female offspring short-term (probe trial 1) and long-term (probe trial 2) spatial reference memory in the Barnes maze test but not male offspring spatial reference memory. Probe trial 1 was conducted 24 h after the last training day and probe trial 2 was conducted 1-week later. Values are means±s.e.m., n = 14–16 offspring per group (ns = not significant). (a) Primary latency, (b) primary distance and (c) number of primary errors to reach the escape hole.

During probe trial 2, which assessed long-term spatial reference memory, females of infected mothers travelled a shorter primary distance (P = 0.049; Fig. 5b) and although not significant, they appeared to make fewer primary errors (P = 0.073; Fig. 5c) in finding the escape hole compared to females of uninfected mothers. These results provide evidence of better long-term spatial reference memory in adult female offspring of infected dams. There were no differences in long-term spatial reference memory between males of uninfected or infected mothers (all P values in probe 1 and 2 > 0.05; Fig. 5a–c). The fewer primary errors in probe trial 1 and shorter primary distance in probe trial 2 provide evidence that maternal infection may enhance spatial memory in adult female mice.

Discussion

Using a GI nematode parasite that remains in the maternal intestine, the main goal of this study was to assess if the positive influence of maternal infection during pregnancy and lactation on spatial memory of juvenile male and female offspring was retained in the uninfected adult offspring. Some indicators of anxiety-like behaviour in adult offspring were also explored. It was reported for the first time that female adult offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams retained enhanced spatial reference memory and also exhibited signs of reduced anxiety-like behaviour compared to females of uninfected dams. No differences were observed in the behaviour of adult male offspring of infected vs uninfected dams. It is hypothesized that sex hormones may at least in part explain the sex-specific differences in behavioural responses to maternal infection of adult offspring.

Previous findings showed that maternal H. bakeri infection enhanced spatial memory in both juvenile male and female offspring, where PD 17 pups of infected dams retained object location memories for 3 h in the object location test but offspring of uninfected mothers did not, and where PD 34 juveniles of infected mothers retained their ability to find an escape location in the BMT for 1 week but offspring from uninfected mothers did not (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022). These findings were consistent with the enhanced hippocampal LTP in uninfected 3-week-old offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams as evidenced by both gene expression and electrophysiological data, and the upregulated neurogenesis pathway (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), all of which are strongly associated with spatial memory (Abrous and Wojtowicz, Reference Abrous and Wojtowicz2015; Lieberwirth et al., Reference Lieberwirth, Pan, Liu, Zhang and Wang2016). Further, maternal H. bakeri infection also upregulated the expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) in the whole brain of neonates (unpublished data from our lab; P = 6.8 × 10−5), and hippocampus of juveniles (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024). BDNF is a key molecule for learning and memory as it is involved in neurogenesis and synaptic plasticity (Lu et al., Reference Lu, Christian and Lu2008; Miranda et al., Reference Miranda, Morici, Zanoni and Bekinschtein2019), and it also plays an important role in reducing behaviours associated with anxiety (Tatiana Marins et al., Reference Tatiana Marins, Rebeca Ataíde, Danton Ferraz, João Vitor Costa, Ana Carolina Tavares, Silvia Fernanda Lima De Moura, Kaneez Fatima and Kamil Hakan2020; Yin et al., Reference Yin, Ma, Liu, Wang, Du and Yang2022). It has been suggested that these differences may be due to the Th2/Treg biased neuroimmune environment observed in the hippocampus of uninfected male and female juvenile offspring of H. bakeri-infected mothers (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), a neuroimmune environment that mimics the systemic immune response of the infected mother (Odiere et al., Reference Odiere, Scott, Leroux, Dzierszinski and Koski2013; Su et al., Reference Su, Chen, Mao, Chen, Steudel, Jiao, Lan, Fasano, Walker and Shi2023).

The present study has shown that the enhanced spatial reference memory observed in juvenile offspring in response to maternal H. bakeri infection (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022) was retained in adult female offspring. This was evidenced by adult female offspring of infected mothers making half the number of errors before finding the escape hole in both probe trials, and having a more direct path to find the escape hole in probe trial 2 in comparison to adult females of uninfected mothers. These differences were not observed between male offspring of infected vs uninfected mothers. The present study also provides evidence of decreased anxiety-like behaviour in adult female offspring of H. bakeri-infected mothers, as they produced fewer fecal pellets in the 5 min habituation trial, compared with the adult female offspring of uninfected mothers. In addition, during the training phase, despite finding the goal box which offered protection from the brightly lit and exposed maze, adult female offspring of infected dams chose to continue exploring instead of seeking shelter and thus did not enter the goal box as soon as females of uninfected mothers. Again, these differences were not observed between male offspring of infected vs uninfected mothers. Previous studies have found that maternal physical exercise enhances offspring spatial memory and decreases anxiety-like behaviour in 3-week-old juveniles and 4-month-old adult rats of both sexes (Aksu et al., Reference Aksu, Baykara, Ozbal, Cetin, Sisman, Dayi, Gencoglu, Tas, Büyük, Gonenc-Arda and Uysal2012; Dayi et al., Reference Dayi, Agilkaya, Ozbal, Cetin, Aksu, Gencoglu, Cingoz, Pekcetin, Tugyan, Kayatekin and Uysal2012) and that this is associated with an increase in hippocampal BDNF and neurogenesis (Bick-Sander et al., Reference Bick-Sander, Steiner, Wolf, Babu and Kempermann2006; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Kim, Lee, Kim, Yang, Chang, Lee, Shin, Lee, Shin, Park, Baek and Kim2006; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Lee, Kim, Yoo and Kim2007; Aksu et al., Reference Aksu, Baykara, Ozbal, Cetin, Sisman, Dayi, Gencoglu, Tas, Büyük, Gonenc-Arda and Uysal2012; Dayi et al., Reference Dayi, Agilkaya, Ozbal, Cetin, Aksu, Gencoglu, Cingoz, Pekcetin, Tugyan, Kayatekin and Uysal2012; Robinson and Bucci, Reference Robinson and Bucci2012; Akhavan et al., Reference Akhavan, Miladi-Gorji, Emami-Abarghoie, Safari, Sadighi-Moghaddam, Vafaei and Rashidy-Pour2013; Gomes da Silva et al., Reference Gomes da Silva, De Almeida, Fernandes, Lopim, Cabral, Scerni, De Oliveira-Pinto, Lent and Arida2016). Considering that juvenile male and female offspring of infected mothers have enhanced hippocampal LTP and upregulated genes associated with BDNF and neurogenesis (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), it is hypothesized that heightened hippocampal BDNF, neurogenesis and LTP may persist in adult females of H. bakeri-infected dams, driving at least in part the enhanced spatial memory and decreased anxiety-like behaviour that was observed.

It is of considerable interest that the enhanced spatial memory in response to maternal H. bakeri infection persisted into adulthood in female but not male offspring. Previous maternal infection models, which have primarily used bacterial or viral infections, or their mimics, have observed sex-dependent effects of maternal infection on the behaviour of mouse offspring (Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019). Unlike an H. bakeri infection which regulates the host immune system, limits pathology and can be considered ‘anti-inflammatory’ in nature (Maizels et al., Reference Maizels, Hewitson, Murray, Harcus, Dayer, Filbey, Grainger, Mcsorley, Reynolds and Smith2012; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Filbey and Maizels2012), bacterial and viral infections are associated with inflammation and harmful pathology in their host (Britton and Saunders, Reference Britton, Saunders, Kaufmann, Rouse and Sacks2010; Rouse and Sehrawat, Reference Rouse and Sehrawat2010). In these maternal bacterial and viral models, the strong proinflammatory Th1 immune response in the infected mother is mimicked in the offspring brain and is associated with impaired hippocampal LTP and spatial memory (Boksa, Reference Boksa2010; Bergdolt and Dunaevsky, Reference Bergdolt and Dunaevsky2019). In response to maternal LPS or PolyI:C, male offspring display significant spatial learning and memory impairments, whereas females do not (Lante et al., Reference Lante, Meunier, Guiramand, Maurice, Cavalier, De Jesus Ferreira, Aimar, Cohen-Solal, Vignes and Barbanel2007; Howland et al., Reference Howland, Cazakoff and Zhang2012; Wischhof et al., Reference Wischhof, Irrsack, Osorio and Koch2015; Batinić et al., Reference Batinić, Santrač, Divović, Timić, Stanković, Obradović, Joksimović and Savić2016; Gogos et al., Reference Gogos, Sbisa, Witkamp and Van Den Buuse2020). The ability of the female sex hormones, oestrogen and progesterone, to dampen Th1 immunity (Roved et al., Reference Roved, Westerdahl and Hasselquist2017) may offer protection to female offspring in these models, by ameliorating the damaging neuroinflammatory responses. In contrast to LPS and Polyl:C models, maternal H. bakeri infection induces a Th2/Treg, not a Th1, immune response in the mother (Odiere et al., Reference Odiere, Scott, Leroux, Dzierszinski and Koski2013; Su et al., Reference Su, Chen, Mao, Chen, Steudel, Jiao, Lan, Fasano, Walker and Shi2023), which is mimicked in the brain of neonatal and juvenile offspring and is suggested to promote hippocampal BDNF, neurogenesis, LTP and spatial memory (Haque et al., Reference Haque, Koski and Scott2019; El Ahdab et al., Reference El Ahdab, Haque, Madogwe, Koski and Scott2021; Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024). Given that oestrogen and progesterone enhance Th2 immune responses (Roved et al., Reference Roved, Westerdahl and Hasselquist2017), it is possible that the influence of these sex hormones on the immune system may allow the Th2/Treg biased neuroimmune environment to persist in adult female offspring of H. bakeri dams, explaining the positive influence on female behaviour at adulthood.

In contrast to oestrogen and progesterone, the male sex hormone, testosterone, dampens Th2 immune responses (Hepworth et al., Reference Hepworth, Hardman and Grencis2010; Klein and Flanagan, Reference Klein and Flanagan2016; Roved et al., Reference Roved, Westerdahl and Hasselquist2017; Taneja, Reference Taneja2018). Evidence of this is seen in mice infected with H. bakeri (Dobson, Reference Dobson1961; Van Zandt et al., Reference Van Zandt, Cypess and Zidian1973; Prowse et al., Reference Prowse, Mitchell, Ey and Jenkin1979; Maizels et al., Reference Maizels, Hewitson, Murray, Harcus, Dayer, Filbey, Grainger, Mcsorley, Reynolds and Smith2012; Rynkiewicz et al., Reference Rynkiewicz, Clerc, Babayan and Pedersen2019) or the GI nematode Trichuris muris (Hepworth et al., Reference Hepworth, Hardman and Grencis2010), where male mice are less resistant and harbour more worms than female mice (Dobson, Reference Dobson1961; Van Zandt et al., Reference Van Zandt, Cypess and Zidian1973; Prowse et al., Reference Prowse, Mitchell, Ey and Jenkin1979; Maizels et al., Reference Maizels, Hewitson, Murray, Harcus, Dayer, Filbey, Grainger, Mcsorley, Reynolds and Smith2012; Rynkiewicz et al., Reference Rynkiewicz, Clerc, Babayan and Pedersen2019), due to the suppression of protective Th2 immunity by testosterone in males (Hepworth et al., Reference Hepworth, Hardman and Grencis2010; Roved et al., Reference Roved, Westerdahl and Hasselquist2017). It is thus possible that the Th2/Treg biased neuroimmune environment observed in juvenile male and female offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), which was hypothesized to promote spatial memory (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Fortin-Hamel, Haque and Scott2022, Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024), may be retained in adult female offspring but dampened by testosterone in adult males, explaining why enhanced spatial memory in adult male offspring was no longer observed. Given that circulating sex hormones are at low levels in prepubescent mice (Bell, Reference Bell2018), they would not have been expected to have a strong influence on the immune response in juvenile offspring.

The ecological effects of a GI nematode infection that enhances spatial memory and reduces anxiety-like behaviour of female offspring are unknown. Spatial memory in mice is necessary for mate location, foraging, predator avoidance and territorial defence, and it is therefore an essential aspect of survival (Vorhees and Williams, Reference Vorhees and Williams2014). Enhancement of spatial memory might thus be beneficial. Additionally, the reduced anxiety-like behaviour might complement the enhanced spatial memory, as adult female offspring of H. bakeri-infected dams would be less anxious and more inclined to explore, increasing opportunities for foraging. Of note, however, reduced anxiety-like behaviour could be a disadvantage with respect to safety and predator avoidance. Lastly, given that reproductive effort and parental investment are more costly for female mice than for males (Parmigiani et al., Reference Parmigiani, Palanza, Mainardi, Brain, Parmigiani and vom Saal1994), there could be evolutionary advantages if females have better spatial memory and lower anxiety.

The following limitations are acknowledged. First, the oestrous cycle of adult female offspring was not controlled for. Although oestrous cycle is recognized as a strong determinant in emotionality and cognitive capacity of female rodents (Gawel et al., Reference Gawel, Gibula, Marszalek-Grabska, Filarowska and Kotlinska2019), the large sample size, and use of the BMT which spanned 13 days [i.e. 3 oestrous cycles/female mouse (Byers et al., Reference Byers, Wiles, Dunn and Taft2012)], makes it likely that all the phases were represented. Second, a test designed to specifically assess anxiety behaviour in mice (e.g. elevated plus maze) would have provided more conclusive data on the influence of maternal H. bakeri infection on anxiety behaviour in offspring. Third, we were unable to determine whether the neuroimmune environment of adult female offspring of infected dams was altered in response to maternal H. bakeri infection, as it had been in the juvenile offspring of infected dams (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Madranges, Gothie, Ewald, Milnerwood, Kennedy and Scott2024).

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to assess the impact of a maternal GI helminth infection on the spatial memory of adult offspring and to determine if the positive influence observed in juvenile male and female offspring persisted into adulthood. It was observed that maternal GI nematode infection during pregnancy and lactation enhanced spatial memory and may also reduce anxiety-like behaviour in adult female, but not male, offspring. It will be of great interest to determine whether sex hormones are the driving factor behind these observations, if this maternal infection influences other aspects of offspring behaviour, and if other nematode infections, such as Nippostrongylus brasiliensis, also alter offspring behaviour.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0031182024000696.

Data availability statement

The datasets generated and analysed for this study are available via a link to the Borealis Dataverse [https://doi.org/10.5683/SP3/HVCHM4], a public data repository.

Author contributions

S. N. conceived and designed the study, conducted experimental work, analysed the data, interpreted the results and drafted the manuscript. R. L. assisted with experimental work and extraction of data from the behaviour videos using the Ethovision XT software. M. E. S. provided input on the study design and data interpretation, as well as critical suggestions that have been incorporated into the manuscript, and obtained funding for the research.

Financial support

The work was supported by a Natural Science and Engineering Council of Canada (NSERC) grant to M. E. S. and the behaviour equipment was purchased with The Canadian Foundation for Innovation Fund # 36144 (2017). S. N. acknowledges Fonds de recherche du Québec sur la nature et les technologies (FRQNT) for a Doctoral Research Scholarship. R. L. acknowledges McGill for a Bieler Undergraduate Research Award. Funding agencies had no role in the study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of data or writing of the manuscript.

Competing interests

None.

Ethical standards

This study (protocol #2000– 4601) was approved by the McGill University Animal Care Committee according to the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and the study was carried out in compliance with ARRIVE guidelines (https://arriveguidelines.org).

References

Abrous, DN and Wojtowicz, JM (2015) Interaction between neurogenesis and hippocampal memory system: new vistas. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7, a018952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akhavan, M, Miladi-Gorji, H, Emami-Abarghoie, M, Safari, M, Sadighi-Moghaddam, B, Vafaei, A and Rashidy-Pour, A (2013) Maternal voluntary exercise during pregnancy enhances the spatial learning acquisition but not the retention of memory in rat pups via a TrkB-mediated mechanism: the role of hippocampal BDNF expression. Iranian Journal of Basic Medical Sciences 16, 955961.Google Scholar
Aksu, I, Baykara, B, Ozbal, S, Cetin, F, Sisman, AR, Dayi, A, Gencoglu, C, Tas, A, Büyük, E, Gonenc-Arda, S and Uysal, N (2012) Maternal treadmill exercise during pregnancy decreases anxiety and increases prefrontal cortex VEGF and BDNF levels of rat pups in early and late periods of life. Neuroscience Letters 516, 221225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, KR and Crawley, JN (2011) Anxiety-related behaviors in mice. In Buccafusco, JJ (ed.), Methods of Behavior Analysis in Neuroscience. 2nd Edn. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, pp. 77101.Google Scholar
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Steve, W (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batinić, B, Santrač, A, Divović, B, Timić, T, Stanković, T, Obradović, AL, Joksimović, S and Savić, MM (2016) Lipopolysaccharide exposure during late embryogenesis results in diminished locomotor activity and amphetamine response in females and spatial cognition impairment in males in adult, but not adolescent rat offspring. Behavioural Brain Research 299, 7280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, MR (2018) Comparing postnatal development of gonadal hormones and associated social behaviors in rats, mice, and humans. Endocrinology 159, 25962613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bergdolt, L and Dunaevsky, A (2019) Brain changes in a maternal immune activation model of neurodevelopmental brain disorders. Progress in Neurobiology 175, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beversdorf, DQ, Stevens, HE, Margolis, KG and Van De Water, J (2019) Prenatal stress and maternal immune dysregulation in autism spectrum disorders: potential points for intervention. Current Pharmaceutical Design 25, 43314343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bick-Sander, A, Steiner, B, Wolf, SA, Babu, H and Kempermann, G (2006) Running in pregnancy transiently increases postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis in the offspring. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103, 38523857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boksa, P (2010) Effects of prenatal infection on brain development and behavior: a review of findings from animal models. Brain Behaviour and Immunity 24, 881897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brailsford, TJ and Behnke, JM (1992) The dynamics of trickle infections with Heligmosomoides polygyrus in syngeneic strains of mice. International Journal of Parasitology 22, 351359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Britton, WJ and Saunders, BM (2010) Pathology and pathogenesis of bacterial infections. In Kaufmann, SHE, Rouse, BT and Sacks, DL (eds), The Immune Response to Infection. Washington, DC: ASM Press, pp. 325336.Google Scholar
Byers, SL, Wiles, MV, Dunn, SL and Taft, RA (2012) Mouse estrous cycle identification tool and images. PLoS ONE 7, e35538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caraci, F, Gulisano, W, Guida, CA, Impellizzeri, AAR, Drago, F, Puzzo, D and Palmeri, A (2015) A key role for TGF-β1 in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory. Scientific Reports 5, 11252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H, Cao, Z, Liu, M, Diamond, MS and Jin, X (2023) The impact of helminth-induced immunity on infection with bacteria or viruses. Veterinary Research 54, 87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, RE and Martin, SJ (2005) Interrogating rodents regarding their object and spatial memory. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15, 593598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cordner, ZA and Tamashiro, KL (2015) Effects of high-fat diet exposure on learning & memory. Physiology and Behaviour 152, 363371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dayi, A, Agilkaya, S, Ozbal, S, Cetin, F, Aksu, I, Gencoglu, C, Cingoz, S, Pekcetin, C, Tugyan, K, Kayatekin, BM and Uysal, N (2012) Maternal aerobic exercise during pregnancy can increase spatial learning by affecting leptin expression on offspring's early and late period in life depending on gender. The Scientific World Journal 2012, 429803.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denenberg, VH (1969) Open-field behavior in the rat: what does it mean?*. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 159, 852859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derecki, NC, Cardani, AN, Yang, CH, Quinnies, KM, Crihfield, A, Lynch, KR and Kipnis, J (2010) Regulation of learning and memory by meningeal immunity: a key role for IL-4. Journal of Experimental Medicine 207, 10671080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, C (1961) Certain aspects of the host-parasite relationship of Nematospiroides dubius (Baylis). I. Resistance of male and female mice to experimental infections. Parasitology 51, 173179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, Z, Han, H, Li, H, Bai, Y, Wang, W, Tu, M, Peng, Y, Zhou, L, He, W, Wu, X, Tan, T, Liu, M, Wu, X, Zhou, W, Jin, W, Zhang, S, Sacktor, TC, Li, T, Song, W and Wang, YT (2015) Long-term potentiation decay and memory loss are mediated by AMPAR endocytosis. Journal of Clinical Investigation 125, 234247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dringenberg, HC (2020) The history of long-term potentiation as a memory mechanism: controversies, confirmation, and some lessons to remember. Hippocampus 30, 9871012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
El Ahdab, N, Haque, M, Madogwe, E, Koski, KG and Scott, ME (2021) Maternal nematode infection upregulates expression of Th2/Treg and diapedesis related genes in the neonatal brain. Scientific Reports 11, 22082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, DE, Setiawan, T, Metwali, A, Blum, A, Urban, JF Jr. and Weinstock, JV 2004. Heligmosomoides polygyrus inhibits established colitis in IL-10-deficient mice. European Journal of Immunology 34, 26902698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francos-Quijorna, I, Amo-Aparicio, J, Martinez-Muriana, A and López-Vales, R (2016) IL-4 drives microglia and macrophages toward a phenotype conducive for tissue repair and functional recovery after spinal cord injury. Glia 64, 20792092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fukushima, T, Liu, RY and Byrne, JH (2007) Transforming growth factor-beta2 modulates synaptic efficacy and plasticity and induces phosphorylation of CREB in hippocampal neurons. Hippocampus 17, 59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gadani, SP, Cronk, JC, Norris, GT and Kipnis, J (2012) IL-4 in the brain: a cytokine to remember. The Journal of Immunology 189, 42134219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawel, K, Gibula, E, Marszalek-Grabska, M, Filarowska, J and Kotlinska, JH (2019) Assessment of spatial learning and memory in the Barnes maze task in rodents-methodological consideration. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Archives of Pharmacology 392, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghasemi, M, Navidhamidi, M, Rezaei, F, Azizikia, A and Mehranfard, N (2022) Anxiety and hippocampal neuronal activity: relationship and potential mechanisms. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 22, 431449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogos, A, Sbisa, A, Witkamp, D and Van Den Buuse, M (2020) Sex differences in the effect of maternal immune activation on cognitive and psychosis-like behaviour in Long Evans rats. European Journal of Neuroscience 52, 26142626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes da Silva, S, De Almeida, AA, Fernandes, J, Lopim, GM, Cabral, FR, Scerni, DA, De Oliveira-Pinto, AV, Lent, R and Arida, RM (2016) Maternal exercise during pregnancy increases BDNF levels and cell numbers in the hippocampal formation but not in the cerebral cortex of adult rat offspring. PLoS ONE 11, e0147200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammelrath, L, Škokić, S, Khmelinskii, A, Hess, A, Van Der Knaap, N, Staring, M, Lelieveldt, BPF, Wiedermann, D and Hoehn, M (2016) Morphological maturation of the mouse brain: an in vivo MRI and histology investigation. NeuroImage 125, 144152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haque, M, Koski, KG and Scott, ME (2019) Maternal gastrointestinal nematode infection up-regulates expression of genes associated with long-term potentiation in perinatal brains of uninfected developing pups. Scientific Reports 9, 4165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, FE, Reiserer, RS, Tomarken, AJ and Mcdonald, MP (2006) Spatial and nonspatial escape strategies in the Barnes maze. Learning and Memory 13, 809819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartig, F (2020) DHARMa: residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level/mixed) regression models. 0.3.3.0 ed. R package. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/DHARMa/vignettes/DHARMa.htmlGoogle Scholar
Hepworth, MR, Hardman, MJ and Grencis, RK (2010) The role of sex hormones in the development of Th2 immunity in a gender-biased model of Trichuris muris infection. European Journal of Immunology 40, 406416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hervé, M (2020) RVAideMemoire: testing and plotting procedures for biostatistics. 0.9-78 ed. R package. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/RVAideMemoire/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Howland, JG, Cazakoff, BN and Zhang, Y (2012) Altered object-in-place recognition memory, prepulse inhibition, and locomotor activity in the offspring of rats exposed to a viral mimetic during pregnancy. Neuroscience 201, 184198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnston, CJC, Robertson, E, Harcus, Y, Grainger, JR, Coakley, G, Smyth, DJ, Mcsorley, HJ and Maizels, R (2015) Cultivation of Heligmosomoides polygyrus: an immunomodulatory nematode parasite and its secreted products. Journal of Visualized Experiments: JoVE (98), e52412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurga, AM, Paleczna, M and Kuter, KZ (2020) Overview of general and discriminating markers of differential microglia phenotypes. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 14, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H, Lee, SH, Kim, SS, Yoo, JH and Kim, CJ (2007) The influence of maternal treadmill running during pregnancy on short-term memory and hippocampal cell survival in rat pups. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience 25, 243249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, SL and Flanagan, KL (2016) Sex differences in immune responses. Nature Reviews Immunology 16, 626638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kristan, DM (2002) Maternal and direct effects of the intestinal nematode Heligmosomoides polygyrus on offspring growth and susceptibility to infection. Journal of Experimental Biology 205, 39673977.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lante, F, Meunier, J, Guiramand, J, Maurice, T, Cavalier, M, De Jesus Ferreira, MC, Aimar, R, Cohen-Solal, C, Vignes, M and Barbanel, G (2007) Neurodevelopmental damage after prenatal infection: role of oxidative stress in the fetal brain. Free Radical Biology and Medicine 42, 12311245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazic, SE (2010) The problem of pseudoreplication in neuroscientific studies: is it affecting your analysis? BMC Neuroscience 11, 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, HH, Kim, H, Lee, JW, Kim, YS, Yang, HY, Chang, HK, Lee, TH, Shin, MC, Lee, MH, Shin, MS, Park, S, Baek, S and Kim, CJ (2006) Maternal swimming during pregnancy enhances short-term memory and neurogenesis in the hippocampus of rat pups. Brain Development 28, 147154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenth, R (2020) emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. 1.4.8 ed. R package. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Lieberwirth, C, Pan, Y, Liu, Y, Zhang, Z and Wang, Z (2016) Hippocampal adult neurogenesis: its regulation and potential role in spatial learning and memory. Brain Research 1644, 127140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, X, Liu, J, Zhao, S, Zhang, H, Cai, W, Cai, M, Ji, X, Leak, RK, Gao, Y, Chen, J and Hu, X (2016) Interleukin-4 Is essential for microglia/macrophage M2 polarization and long-term recovery after cerebral ischemia. Stroke 47, 498504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, Y, Christian, K and Lu, B (2008) BDNF: a key regulator for protein synthesis-dependent LTP and long-term memory? Neurobiology Learning and Memory 89, 312323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maizels, RM, Hewitson, JP, Murray, J, Harcus, YM, Dayer, B, Filbey, KJ, Grainger, JR, Mcsorley, HJ, Reynolds, LA and Smith, KA (2012) Immune modulation and modulators in Heligmosomoides polygyrus infection. Experimental Parasitology 132, 7689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, U, Feldon, J and Fatemi, SH (2009) In-vivo rodent models for the experimental investigation of prenatal immune activation effects in neurodevelopmental brain disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 33, 10611079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miranda, M, Morici, JF, Zanoni, MB and Bekinschtein, P (2019) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: a key molecule for memory in the healthy and the pathological brain. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 13, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noel, SC, Fortin-Hamel, L, Haque, M and Scott, ME (2022) Maternal gastrointestinal nematode infection enhances spatial memory of uninfected juvenile mouse pups. Scientific Reports 12, 9796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noel, SC, Madranges, JF, Gothie, JM, Ewald, J, Milnerwood, AJ, Kennedy, TE and Scott, ME (2024) Maternal gastrointestinal nematode infection alters hippocampal neuroimmunity, promotes synaptic plasticity, and improves resistance to direct infection in offspring. Scientific Reports 14, 10773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolan, Y, Maher, FO, Martin, DS, Clarke, RM, Brady, MT, Bolton, AE, Mills, KH and Lynch, MA (2005) Role of interleukin-4 in regulation of age-related inflammatory changes in the hippocampus. Journal of Biological Chemistry 280, 93549362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odiere, MR, Scott, ME, Weiler, HA and Koski, KG (2010) Protein deficiency and nematode infection during pregnancy and lactation reduce maternal bone mineralization and neonatal linear growth in mice. Journal of Nutrition 140, 16381645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Odiere, MR, Scott, ME, Leroux, LP, Dzierszinski, FS and Koski, KG (2013) Maternal protein deficiency during a gastrointestinal nematode infection alters developmental profile of lymphocyte populations and selected cytokines in neonatal mice. Journal of Nutrition 143, 100107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parmigiani, S, Palanza, P, Mainardi, D and Brain, PF (1994) Infanticide and protection of young in house mice (Mus domesticus): female and male strategies. In Parmigiani, S and vom Saal, F (eds), Infanticide and Parental Care. Chur, Switzerland: Harwood, pp. 341363.Google Scholar
Patil, SS, Sunyer, B, Hoger, H and Lubec, G (2009) Evaluation of spatial memory of C57BL/6J and CD1 mice in the Barnes maze, the Multiple T-maze and in the Morris water maze. Behavioural Brain Research 198, 5868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinheiro, BBD (2023) nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R package version 3.1-162. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlmeGoogle Scholar
Prowse, SJ, Mitchell, GF, Ey, PL and Jenkin, CR (1979) The development of resistance in different inbred strains of mice to infection with Nematospiroides dubius. Parasite Immunology 1, 277288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2020) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reynolds, LA, Filbey, KJ and Maizels, RM (2012) Immunity to the model intestinal helminth parasite Heligmosomoides polygyrus. Seminars in Immunopathology 34, 829846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, AM and Bucci, DJ (2012) Maternal exercise and cognitive functions of the offspring. Cognitive Science (Hauppauge) 7, 187205.Google ScholarPubMed
Robinson, AM and Bucci, DJ (2014) Physical exercise during pregnancy improves object recognition memory in adult offspring. Neuroscience 256, 5360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouse, BT and Sehrawat, S (2010) Immunity and immunopathology to viruses: what decides the outcome? Nature Reviews Immunology 10, 514526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roved, J, Westerdahl, H and Hasselquist, D (2017) Sex differences in immune responses: hormonal effects, antagonistic selection, and evolutionary consequences. Hormones and Behaviour 88, 95105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rynkiewicz, EC, Clerc, M, Babayan, SA and Pedersen, AB (2019) Variation in local and systemic pro-inflammatory immune markers of wild wood mice after anthelmintic treatment. Integrative and Comparative Biology 59, 11901202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, KA, Raine, T, Cooke, A and Lawrence, CE (2007) Inhibition of autoimmune type 1 diabetes by gastrointestinal helminth infection. Infection and Immunity 75, 397407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharma, S, Rakoczy, S and Brown-Borg, H (2010) Assessment of spatial memory in mice. Life Sciences 87, 521536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smallwood, TB, Giacomin, PR, Loukas, A, Mulvenna, JP, Clark, RJ and Miles, JJ (2017) Helminth immunomodulation in autoimmune disease. Frontiers in Immunology 8, 453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, CW, Chen, CY, Mao, T, Chen, N, Steudel, N, Jiao, L, Lan, J, Fasano, A, Walker, WA and Shi, HN (2023) Maternal helminth infection protects offspring from high-fat-diet-induced obesity through altered microbiota and SCFAs. Cellular and Molecular Immunology 20, 389403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sunyer, B, Patil, S, Hoger, H and Lubec, G (2007) Barnes maze, a useful task to assess spatial reference memory in the mice. Nature Protocols, 112.Google Scholar
Taneja, V (2018) Sex hormones determine immune response. Frontiers in Immunology 9, 1931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tatiana Marins, F, Rebeca Ataíde, C, Danton Ferraz, S, João Vitor Costa, F, Ana Carolina Tavares, L and Silvia Fernanda Lima De Moura, C (2020) BDNF protein and anxiety disorders. In Kaneez Fatima, S and Kamil Hakan, D (eds.), Neurological and Mental Disorders. Rijeka: IntechOpen, pp. 116124.Google Scholar
Van Zandt, PD, Cypess, RH and Zidian, JL (1973) Development of age and sex resistance to Nematospiroides dubius in the mouse following single and multiple infections. Journal of Parasitology 59, 977979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Venables, WN and Ripley, BD (2002) Modern Applied Statistics with S. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vorhees, CV and Williams, MT (2014) Assessing spatial learning and memory in rodents. ILAR Journal 55, 310332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, MPJ, Johnston, CJC, Grainger, JR, Konkel, JE, O'connor, RA, Anderton, SM and Maizels, RM (2020) The helminth parasite Heligmosomoides polygyrus attenuates EAE in an IL-4Rα-dependent manner. Frontiers in Immunology 11, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilhelm, CJ and Guizzetti, M (2015) Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: an overview from the glia perspective. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 9, 65.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, MS and Maizels, RM (2006) Regulatory T cells induced by parasites and the modulation of allergic responses. Chemical Immunology and Allergy 90, 176195.Google ScholarPubMed
Wischhof, L, Irrsack, E, Osorio, C and Koch, M (2015) Prenatal LPS-exposure – a neurodevelopmental rat model of schizophrenia – differentially affects cognitive functions, myelination and parvalbumin expression in male and female offspring. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 57, 1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yin, XL, Ma, YY, Liu, YL, Wang, LX, Du, N and Yang, L (2022) Changes of brain-derived neurotrophic factors in rats with generalized anxiety disorder before and after treatment. European Review for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences 26, 15001507.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhang, J, Rong, P, Zhang, L, He, H, Zhou, T, Fan, Y, Mo, L, Zhao, Q, Han, Y, Li, S, Wang, Y, Yan, W, Chen, H and You, Z (2021) IL4-driven microglia modulate stress resilience through BDNF-dependent neurogenesis. Science Advances 7, eabb9888.Google ScholarPubMed
Zuur, A, Ieno, EN, Walker, N, Saveliev, A and Smith, GM (2009) Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Schematic representation of experimental design and protocol. Of the 40 timed-pregnant dams received on gestation day (GD) 4, only 31 delivered litters. On postnatal day (PD) 20, 1 pup per sex per litter was selected to perform the Barnes maze test. Of the pups selected for behavioural analysis, their size, specifically crown-rump length and weight, were recorded on PD 20 and 69 (see Supplementary Fig. 2).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Maternal H. bakeri infection reduced female offspring fecal count during the 5 min habituation trial of the Barnes maze test. Values are means±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group (*P < 0.05; ns = not significant).

Figure 2

Figure 3. Neither maternal H. bakeri infection nor adult offspring sex influenced primary variables of spatial learning in the Barnes maze test over 4 training days. All mice performed significantly better after the first training day. Values are LSmeans±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group. (a) Primary latency, (b) primary distance and (c) number of primary errors to reach the escape hole, and (d) mean velocity during the trial. Different letters show the effect of training day, P < 0.05.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Maternal H. bakeri infection influenced female offspring exploration during the 4-day training phase of the Barnes maze test. Total parameters were used as an indication of exploration to provide an understanding of fear/anxiety levels. Values are LSmeans±s.e.m., n = 15–16 offspring per group. (a) Total latency, (b) total distance and (c) number of total errors to enter the goal box.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Maternal H. bakeri infection influenced adult female offspring short-term (probe trial 1) and long-term (probe trial 2) spatial reference memory in the Barnes maze test but not male offspring spatial reference memory. Probe trial 1 was conducted 24 h after the last training day and probe trial 2 was conducted 1-week later. Values are means±s.e.m., n = 14–16 offspring per group (ns = not significant). (a) Primary latency, (b) primary distance and (c) number of primary errors to reach the escape hole.

Supplementary material: File

Noel et al. supplementary material

Noel et al. supplementary material
Download Noel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 557.3 KB