Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T07:31:55.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive reflection predicts the acceptance of unfair ultimatum game offers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Dustin P. Calvillo*
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, California State University San Marcos, 333 S. Twin Oaks Valley Road, San Marcos, CA 92096, USA
Jessica N. Burgeno*
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, California State University San Marcos
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

In the ultimatum game, one player proposes a split of money between him- or herself and another player, who can accept the offer (and both players keep the allocated money) or reject the offer (and both players get nothing). The present study examined predictors of accepting unfair ultimatum offers. In Study 1, 184 participants responded to an unfair ultimatum offer, completed a measure of cognitive reflection, and completed a self-report measure of rational and experiential thinking. Slightly more than half of the participants (54.3%) accepted the unfair offer, and cognitive reflection was positively correlated with accepting unfair offers. The rational and experiential thinking scales were not significantly correlated with ultimatum decisions. In Study 2, 306 participants responded to 20 ultimatum offers that varied in fairness and completed an expanded measure of cognitive reflection. Performance on the cognitive reflection measure predicted the number of ultimatum offers accepted. These results suggest that rejecting ultimatum offers is related to intuitive, heuristic-based thinking, whereas accepting offers is related to deliberate, analytic-based thinking.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2015] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

The ultimatum game is commonly used to examine economic decision making (Reference Güth and KocherGüth & Kocher, 2013). In the ultimatum game, a proposer is given a sum of money to divide between him- or herself and a responder. The responder then has the option to accept or reject the allocation. If the responder accepts, both players receive the amount that has been allocated to them; if the responder rejects, both players get nothing. Most proposers split the money relatively evenly and most responders accept these fair offers (e.g., Camerer, 2003). The goal of the present study was to examine predictors of responders’ decisions when they have received an unfair offer.

Unfair offers occur when proposers have divided the money so that they receive more than responders. For example, if a proposer is given $10 to allocate, the proposer may divide it so that he or she receives $8 and the responder receives $2. Game theory suggests that responders, acting in their own self-interest, will accept this offer (or any offer greater than $0) in a single round of the ultimatum game. Studies have consistently demonstrated, however, that unfair offers are often rejected (e.g., Reference Cooper and DutcherCooper & Dutcher, 2011). The rejection of unfair offers may be viewed as irrational because the responder is choosing $0 rather than some value greater than $0.Footnote 1 This behavior is an example of negative reciprocity because the responder is punishing the proposer for being unfair (e.g., Halali, Bereby-Meyer & Meiran, 2014). Negative reciprocity has been studied in a variety of economic situations (e.g., Brandts & Solà, 2001).

Dual process theories of cognition have been employed to explain ultimatum decisions (e.g., Reference Sanfey and ChangSanfey & Chang, 2008). These theories claim that humans have two separate modes of information processing. Although the specifics of the two systems vary among theories (see Evans, 2008, for review), one system (System 1) is regarded as processing information in fast, automatic, heuristic-based ways, whereas the other system (System 2) is slower, more deliberate, and analytic. Furthermore, System 1 processes are believed to operate independently of general intelligence and working memory capacity, whereas System 2 processes are linked to individual differences in general intelligence and working memory capacity (Reference EvansEvans, 2008). One System 1 process, individuals’ affective responses to alternatives, has been shown to influence decisions in a variety of situations (e.g., Slovic, Finucane, Peters & MacGregor, 2002). When applied to unfair ultimatum offers, responders’ negative affective responses to an unfair offer leads them to want to reject the offer, whereas their understanding that accepting any offer greater than zero is beneficial leads them to want to accept the offer (Reference Sanfey and ChangSanfey & Chang, 2008). The explanation that System 1 processes lead to the rejection of unfair offers, whereas analytical System 2 processes lead to the acceptance of these offers has been termed the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis (Reference Halali, Bereby-Meyer and MeiranHalali et al., 2014). In support of this hypothesis, emotions such as anger have been found to be crucial in rejecting unfair offers (Reference Pillutla and MurnighanPillutla & Murnighan, 1996; Reference Srivastava, Espinoza and FedorikhinSrivastava, Espinoza & Fedorikhin, 2009).Footnote 2, Footnote 3

To establish that accepting an unfair offer is related to System 2 processes, several experimental studies have demonstrated that reducing System 2 processing increases the rejection of unfair ultimatum offers. When responders must make their decisions under time pressure they are more likely to reject unfair offers than when there is no deadline (Reference Cappelletti, Güth and PlonerCappelletti, Güth & Ploner, 2011). Similarly, unfair offers are more likely to be accepted after a 10-minute delay than when a decision is made immediately (Reference Grimm and MengelGrimm & Mengel, 2011). Also consistent with the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis, when individuals are deprived of sleep (Reference Anderson and DickinsonAnderson & Dickinson, 2010), when their executive resources are diminished with an ego depletion manipulation (Reference Halali, Bereby-Meyer and MeiranHalali et al., 2014), and when they are alcohol intoxicated (Reference Morewedge, Krishnamurti and ArielyMorewedge, Krishnamurti & Ariely, 2014), they show increased rejection rates compared to control conditions. Furthermore, individuals who accept the most unfair offers show greater cognitive control than those who accept the fewest unfair offers (Reference DeDe Neys, Novitskiy, Geeraerts, Ramautar & Wagemans, 2011).

Despite the abundance of support for the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis, some findings are more equivocal. Cognitive abilities (intelligence, attention, working memory, and executive functioning), which are linked to System 2 processes, do not significantly differ among participants who accept and those who reject unfair offers (Reference Nguyen, Koenigs, Yamada, Teo and CavanaughNguyen et al., 2011). Others have claimed that System 2 processes, rather than System 1 processes, lead to rejecting unfair offers, an approach termed the deliberate negative reciprocity hypothesis by Halali et al. (2014). Correlational studies have shown that individuals with greater heart-rate variability, a marker for inhibitory control and emotion regulation, are more likely to reject unfair offers (Reference Sütterlin, Herbert, Schmitt, Kübler and VögeleSütterlin, Herbert, Schmitt, Kübler & Vögele, 2011; but see Reference Dunn, Evans, Makarova, White and ClarkDunn, Evans, Makarova, White & Clark, 2012). Furthermore, individuals’ rejection of unfair offers is correlated with performance on a motor response inhibition task: those with greater System 2 abilities rejected more offers (Reference Sütterlin, Herbert, Schmitt, Kübler and VögeleSütterlin et al., 2011). Rejection of unfair offers is positively related to reaction time to make a decision and to participants’ self-rated need for cognition (Reference Mussel, Göritz and HewigMussel, Göritz & Hewig, 2013a). Finally, using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), Knoch, Pascual-Leone, Meyer, Treyer, and Fehr (2006) found that disrupting the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, an area believed to be involved in executive processes necessary for inhibiting selfish impulses to accept unfair offers, increases the acceptance of unfair offers. Thus, there is some correlational and TMS evidence that System 2 processes lead to the rejection of unfair offers.Footnote 4

Examining differences in the ability to engage in System 2 processing among those who accept and those who reject unfair offers tests contradictory predictions derived from the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis and from the deliberate negative reciprocity hypothesis. There are several ways to assess System 2 processes, including measures of working memory capacity and general intelligence, and performance on these measures has been associated with performance on a variety of reasoning and judgment and decision tasks that require System 2 processes (see Evans, 2003, for review). The Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005) was created to assess the ability to overcome intuitive but incorrect responses arising from System 1 processes in favor of more effortful correct responses arising from System 2 processes. Indeed, performance on the Frederick’s (2005) three-item CRT is predictive of resisting several cognitive biases (Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli & Labollita, 2010; Reference Cokely and KelleyCokely & Kelley, 2009; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference Oechssler, Roider and SchmitzOechssler, Roider & Schmitz, 2009; Reference Toplak, West and StanovichToplak, West & Stanovich, 2011), and an expanded seven-item CRT has been developed that is a better predictor than the original three-item version for several rational thinking tasks (Reference Toplak, West and StanovichToplak, West & Stanovich, 2014). Regarding ultimatum decisions, if participants who accept unfair offers outperform those who reject them on the CRT, this finding would support the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis. Alternatively, finding that participants who reject unfair offers outperform those who accept them would support the deliberate negative reciprocity hypothesis.

Only one study, to our knowledge, has examined the relationship between performance on the CRT and ultimatum decisions. De Neys et al. (2011) found that performance on the three-item CRT was positively correlated with acceptance of unfair ultimatum offers. Given the mixed support for the automatic- and the deliberate negative reciprocity hypotheses, the present study sought to replicate this relationship between cognitive reflection and acceptance of unfair ultimatum offers.

2 Study 1

In Study 1, participants responded to an unfair ultimatum game offer, completed the seven-item Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT7; Reference Toplak, West and StanovichToplak et al., 2014), and completed a self-reported measure of rational and experiential thinking styles: the 40-item Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI-40; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999). The REI-40 assesses individuals’ self-reported tendencies to engage in automatic, intuitive thinking (experiential thought, which is analogous to System 1 processing) and effortful, analytic thinking (rational thought, which is analogous to System 2 processing). The automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis leads to the predictions that accepting the unfair ultimatum offer would be positively correlated with performance on the CRT7 and the rational thought scales of the REI-40 and negatively correlated with the experiential scales of the REI-40. The deliberate negative reciprocity hypothesis, conversely, leads to the predictions that accepting the unfair offer would be negatively correlated with performance on the CRT7 and the rational thought scales of the REI-40 and positively correlated with the experiential scales of the REI-40.

2.1 Method

2.1.1 Participants

Undergraduate students ( N = 184) enrolled in lower division psychology courses at California State University San Marcos participated in exchange for credit toward the completion of a research requirement. The sample contained 165 (89.7%) women and 19 (10.3%) men, and participants ranged in age from 18 to 27 ( M = 19.93, SD = 1.69) years (21 participants did not provide their age). The racial/ethnic composition of the sample was 77 (41.8%) Hispanic individuals, 57 (31.0%) Caucasian individuals, 24 (13.0%) Asian/Pacific Islander individuals, 5 (2.7%) Black/African American individuals, 1 (0.5%) Native American individual, 14 (7.6%) other/multi-ethnic individuals, and 6 (3.3%) participants declined to provide their ethnicity. An a priori power analyses based on a small to medium point-biserial correlation between ultimatum decisions and continuous predictors ( r = .20), with α = .05 (two-tailed), determined that this sample would be sufficient to detect these relationships with power greater than .75.

2.1.2 Materials and procedure

Participants completed a one-shot ultimatum game decision, a measure of cognitive reflection, and a self-reported measure of rational and experiential abilities and engagement.

Participants first received instructions on the ultimatum game. After reading a description of the game, they were told that they would be randomly assigned to either the role of proposer or responder to play with another participant from the same subject pool who was randomly assigned to the other role. All participants were then informed that they had been assigned to the role of responder, and that another participant assigned to the role of proposer, labeled only as “Participant #1057,” had made an $8/$2 offer. Because proposers’ characteristics such as gender and attractiveness (e.g., Reference Solnick and SchweitzerSolnick & Schweitzer, 1999), ethnicity (Reference Kubota, Li, Bar-David, Banaji and PhelpsKubota, Li, Bar-David, Banaji & Phelps, 2013), and facial expression (Reference Mussel, Göritz and HewigMussel, Göritz & Hewig, 2013b) can influence responders’ behavior, participants were not presented with photos of the proposer. After participants saw the offer, they selected one of two options: “I accept this offer (Participant #1057 gets $8 and I get $2)” or “I reject this offer (Participant #1057 and I get $0).” To ensure that participants took the decision seriously, they were informed that a subset of participants, chosen at random, would be paid according to the decisions they made.

After completing the ultimatum decision, participants provided answers to the 7-item Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT7; Toplak et al., 2014—the first 7 “problems” in the supplement at http://journal.sjdm.org/14/14715/stimuli.pdf). The CRT7 contains the original three items from Frederick’s (2005) test and four additional items. All items have an intuitive response that is incorrect and a rational, correct response. For example, the first item is, “A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs a dollar more than the ball. How much does the ball cost? ____ cents.” The intuitive, incorrect response is 10 cents and the more effortful, correct response is 5 cents.

Table 1: Descriptive statistics and correlations of CRT7 scores and REI-40 scales with ultimatum decision (0 = reject, 1 = accept).

* p < .05,

** p < .01,

*** p < .001.

Participants then completed a 40-item Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI-40; Reference Pacini and EpsteinPacini & Epstein, 1999).Footnote 5 The REI-40 contains four scales: rational ability, rational engagement, experiential ability, and experiential engagement. The rationality scales contain items that assess analytic processing styles, similar to what is assessed in need for cognition measures, whereas the experiential scales contain items that assess intuitive processing styles. Participants rated their agreement with 40 statements on a scale of 1 ( definitely not true) to 5 ( definitely true). An example of a rational ability item is, “I have a logical mind,” an example of a rational engagement item is, “I prefer complex problems to simple problems,” an example of an experiential ability item is, “I believe in trusting my hunches,” and an example of an experiential engagement item is, “I like to rely on my intuitive impressions.”

All of the tasks were completed online. The first screen contained the elements of informed consent and the last screen contained debriefing information. Most participants took about 15 min to complete the study.

2.2 Results and discussion

Of the 184 participants, 100 (54.3%) of them accepted the unfair offer and 84 (45.7%) rejected it. The CRT7 had questionable internal consistency (α = .60); the internal consistencies of the REI scales were generally acceptable (rational ability: α = .75, rational engagement: α = .83, experiential ability: α = .75, experiential engagement: α = .67). We compared CRT7 performance and the four scales of the REI-40 between those who accepted and those who rejected the unfair ultimatum offer. Means and standard deviations of the CRT7 and REI-40 scales for the overall sample, and their Pearson product moment correlations with each other and point-biserial correlations with ultimatum decisions are presented in Table 1. Means and standard deviations of the CRT7 and REI-40 scales separated by ultimatum game decisions are presented in Table 2. Participants who accepted the unfair offer performed significantly better on the CRT7 than those who rejected the offer, t(182) = 2.09, p = .038, d = 0.31, and those who accepted had non-significantly lower experiential engagement scores than those who rejected, t(182) = 1.79, p = .076, d = 0.27. No other comparisons approached significance.

Table 2: CRT7 scores and REI-40 scales based on whether participants accepted or rejected the unfair ultimatum offer.

The results of Study 1 with the CRT7 suggest that rejecting an unfair offer is related to intuitive (System 1) thinking, whereas accepting it is related to rational (System 2) thought. These findings are consistent with the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis, which suggests that the negative emotions associated with an unfair ultimatum offer occur automatically and the acceptance of an unfair offer requires controlled executive processes to inhibit the automatic desire to reject the offer (Reference Halali, Bereby-Meyer and MeiranHalali et al., 2014). Participants’ scores on the four scales of the REI-40 were not significant predictors of their ultimatum game decisions. All four scales (rational ability, rational engagement, experiential ability, and experiential engagement) showed small, nonsignificant, negative correlations with accepting unfair ultimatum offers. Taken together, the results of Study 1 suggest that performance measures of cognitive abilities may be better predictors of ultimatum decisions than self-report measures of thinking dispositions.

3 Study 2

Study 2 contained several improvements based on limitations of Study 1. One limitation of Study 1 was the insensitivity of the ultimatum measure: participants responded to a single unfair offer. In Study 2, participants responded to 20 offers that varied in their level of fairness. Another limitation in Study 1 was that the internal consistency of the CRT7 was low. A recent paper has demonstrated that an expanded version of the CRT shows greater internal consistency than the original three-item version (Reference Baron, Scott, Fincher and MetzBaron, Scott, Fincher & Metz, 2015). Study 2 included items from this expanded measure, along with the original three items from Frederick (2005) and the four items added by Toplak et al. (2014) in hopes of creating a more reliable measure of cognitive reflection. Based on the findings from Study 1, we hypothesized that the cognitive reflection scores would be positively correlated with the acceptance rates of unfair ultimatum offers, but unrelated to the acceptance of fair offers.

3.1 Method

3.1.1 Participants

Undergraduate students ( N = 306) from the same participant pool as Study 1 (none of whom participated in Study 1) participated in Study 2 in exchange for credit toward the completion of a research requirement. The sample contained 251 (82.0%) women and 55 (18.0%) men, and participants ranged in age from 18 to 43 ( M = 20.19, SD = 3.24) years (98 participants did not provide their age). The racial/ethnic composition of the sample was 124 (40.5%) Hispanic individuals, 108 (35.3%) Caucasian individuals, 31 (10.1%) Asian/Pacific Islander individuals, 11 (3.6%) Black/African American individuals, 2 (0.7%) Native American individuals, 28 (9.1%) other/multi-ethnic individuals, and 2 (0.7%) participants declined to provide their ethnicity. An a priori power analyses based on the effect reported in Study 1 ( r = .15), with α = .05 (two-tailed), determined that this sample would be sufficient to detect this relationships with power of .75.

3.1.2 Materials and procedure

Participants completed 20 ultimatum game decisions and an expanded measure of cognitive reflection (all shown in http://journal.sjdm.org/14/14715/stimuli.pdf. After reading instructions of the ultimatum game, all participants were informed that they were randomly assigned to the role of responder and that they would see 20 offers made from 20 previous participants who had been assigned to the role of proposer. Participants saw four offers of five different values: $5, $4, $3, $2, and $1 of a $10 pot. The same random order of these 20 offers was used for all participants. Each offer was presented with a unique participant number for the proposer; no images or names were provided. After the presentation of each offer, participants indicated whether they accepted or rejected that offer. To increase participants’ willingness to consider each decision, they were informed that a randomly selected subset of participant would be paid according to their decisions.

After completing the ultimatum decisions, participants provided answers to a 17-item Cognitive Reflection Test (henceforth, CRT17). The CRT17 contained the original three items from Frederick’s (2005), four items from Toplak et al. (2014) and 10 additional items from Baron et al. (2015).Footnote 6

All of the tasks were completed online with most participants taking about 20 min to complete the study.

3.2 Results and discussion

Consistent with prior ultimatum game studies, acceptance rates were dependent on the offer size, F(4, 1220) = 460.60, p < .001, η 2 p = .60. All pairwise comparisons (with Bonferroni corrections) were significant. The $5 offers had the highest acceptance rate ( M = .96, SD = .13), followed by $4 offers ( M = .79, SD = .36), $3 offers ( M = .45, SD = .42), $2 offers ( M = .27, SD = .40), and $1 offers ( M = .20, SD = .38). The internal consistency for the set of ultimatum decisions was high (α = .92). The internal consistency for the CRT17 was acceptable (α = .81) and showed an improvement over that of the three-item version (α = .63) and the seven-item version (α = .71) in the present study. Participants averaged 4.19 ( SD = 3.52) correct answers on the CRT17.

Figure 1: The acceptance rates of offers across offer values based on CRT17 performance.

As predicted, performance on the CRT17 was significantly positively correlated with the overall acceptance rates of offers, r(304) = .14, p = .013. To examine whether the relationship between CRT17 and the acceptance rates of offers was dependent on the offer value, we calculated the sensitivity to offer fairness for each participant, the slope of the relation between offer size and acceptance: sensitivity = 2 x acceptance-rate-for-$5-offers + acceptance-rate-of-$4-offers − acceptance-rate-of-$2-offers − 2 x acceptance-rate-of-$1-offers. The correlation between sensitivity and CRT17 performance was not significant ( r(304) = .08, p = .164), suggesting that the relationship between CRT17 and acceptance rates was not dependent on the offer value. We illustrate this relationship in Figure 1 by creating three groups of participants based on CRT17 performance. Those classified as low ( n = 117) correctly answered 2 or fewer CRT17 questions, those classified in the middle group ( n = 93) correctly answered 3 or 4 CRT17 items, and those in the high group ( n = 96) correctly answered at least 5 CRT17 questions. Figure 1 shows the similarity in the slope of offer acceptance across offer values for these three groups. Participants who performed well on the CRT17 tended to accept all offer types more frequently than participants who performed poorly on the CRT17.

Study 2 employed a more sensitive ultimatum game measure and an extended, more reliable, cognitive reflection measure, and demonstrated that cognitive reflection was again positively related to accepting ultimatum offers. We discuss these results further in the General Discussion.

4 General discussion

In two studies, cognitive reflection predicted the acceptance of unfair ultimatum offers: those who performed well on the CRT measures were more likely to accept unfair offers. The CRT measures were designed to assess participants’ ability to override intuitive responses in favor of more rational thought (Reference Baron, Scott, Fincher and MetzBaron et al., 2015; Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005; Reference Toplak, West and StanovichToplak et al., 2014). These findings replicate and extend those of De Neys et al. (2011) and are consistent with the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis, which claims the intuitive response to an unfair ultimatum offer is to want to reject it, whereas the more rational, deliberate response is to accept these offers. Participants’ ability to inhibit incorrect intuitive responses in the CRT measures predicted the acceptance of offers. Thus, the findings of the present study add to other studies that can be interpreted as supporting the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis (Reference Anderson and DickinsonAnderson & Dickinson, 2010; Reference Cappelletti, Güth and PlonerCappelletti et al., 2011; Crockett et al. 2010; Reference Grimm and MengelGrimm & Mengel, 2011; Reference Halali, Bereby-Meyer and MeiranHalali et al., 2014; Reference Morewedge, Krishnamurti and ArielyMorewedge et al., 2014).

The results of Study 2, however, suggest that the relationship between CRT performance and accepting offers was not dependent on offer size. If unfair offers elicit an automatic desire to punish the proposer, then the relationship between CRT and accepting offers should be greater for unfair offers than fair offers, and this relationship might become increasingly large as the fairness of the offer decreases. This was not the case in Study 2. Those who performed well on the CRT tended to accept all offer values more than those who performed poorly. Perhaps individuals high in cognitive reflection better understand the ultimatum game and accept all offers more frequently than individuals low in cognitive reflection. Similarly, those who reject unfair offers may not do it because they are unable to inhibit an affective response to the offers. They may simply lack understanding of the task.

The observed relationship between cognitive performance and ultimatum decisions appear to contradict the findings of a previous study. Nguyen et al. (2011) found no differences in cognitive abilities (intelligence, working memory, psychomotor speed, language, visuospatial ability, and executive functioning) between participants who accepted and those who rejected unfair offers. We speculate that these seemingly discrepant results occur because cognitive reflection is different from other cognitive performance measures. Studies have demonstrated that performance on the CRT explains variance in decision making tasks beyond that which is explained by cognitive abilities and thinking dispositions (Toplak et al., 2011, 2014), and the CRT is a stronger predictor of decisions than other cognitive abilities or thinking disposition measures (Reference FrederickFrederick, 2005). This may explain why the CRT7 was a significant predictor of ultimatum decisions in the present study, but other cognitive performance measures were not in a previous study (Reference Nguyen, Koenigs, Yamada, Teo and CavanaughNguyen et al., 2011).

Despite the consistent relationship between cognitive reflection and acceptance of unfair ultimatum offers in the present study, cognitive reflection explained only a small portion of variance in ultimatum decisions (around 2% in Study 1 and Study 2). Thus, there is a large amount of variance in ultimatum decisions that result from something other than cognitive reflection. Future research could examine other sources of variance. Studies have shown that positive and negative affect (Reference Dunn, Makaraova, Evans and ClarkDunn, Makaraova, Evans & Clark, 2010; Reference Nguyen, Koenigs, Yamada, Teo and CavanaughNguyen et al., 2011), personality factors (Reference Brandstätter and KonigsteinBrandstätter & Konigstein, 2001; Reference Nguyen, Koenigs, Yamada, Teo and CavanaughNguyen et al., 2011), and justice sensitivity (Reference Fetchenhauer and HuangFetchenhauer & Huang, 2004; Reference Hoffman and BaumertHoffman & Baumert, 2010) are related to ultimatum responses. A study that includes all of these predictors would allow for a more complete understanding of the sources of different responses to unfair ultimatum offers.

One limitation of the present study was the sample. The sample was fairly ethnically/racially diverse, but the gender and age composition were not: over 80% of the participants were women in both studies and all participants were 43 years of age or younger. Because gender (Reference Hack and LammersHack & Lammers, 2009) and age (Reference Bailey, Ruffman and RendellBailey, Ruffman & Rendell, 2012) can affect ultimatum decisions, the findings of the present study should be examined in more gender-equal and age-diverse samples. Additionally, only a subset of the participants in the present study were paid based on their decisions. Thus, participants may have viewed the offers as hypothetical or of small stakes because the expected value of their decisions was reduced based on the small probability of being selected for payment. Hypothetical offers are rejected more frequently than real money offers, and small stakes offers are rejected more frequently than larger stakes offers (Reference CameronCameron, 1999). Furthermore, holding cash in their hands reduces participants’ rejections of unfair offers (Reference Shen and TakahashiShen & Takahashi, 2013). The findings of the present study should be extended to real money offers, particularly when responders handle cash.

The results of the present study are seemingly at odds with findings using other tasks. For example, some studies have shown that cooperation is automatic and self-interested defection requires deliberation. Reference Cone and RandCone and Rand (2014), for example, found that time pressure increases cooperation in competitive games. Similarly, Kieslich and Hilbig (2014) found that defecting in a social dilemma led to more cognitive conflict than cooperating. They measured cognitive conflict by analyzing the computer mouse’s trajectory; this trajectory was more curved when defecting than cooperating. Whereas the present study’s results suggest that negative reciprocity (i.e., punishing unfair partners) is automatic and responding in self-interested ways is deliberate, these studies using other tasks showed that cooperation may be automatic. Responding to unfair ultimatum offers, however, may be considerably different than the behavior examined in other tasks. Yamagishi et al. (2012) found that rejecting unfair ultimatum game offers was not related to behavior in the prisoner’s dilemma, trust, and dictator games.

In conclusion, the present study showed that cognitive reflection predicted decisions to unfair ultimatum offers: participants who scored higher on measures of cognitive reflection were more likely to accept unfair offers. These findings are consistent with the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis and suggest that affective responses to unfair offers occur automatically and participants with the ability to inhibit the automatic desire to punish unfair proposers and cognitively reflect on their options are more likely to accept unfair offers. Alternatively, participants high in cognitive reflection may better understand that accepting any ultimatum offer is in their best interest.

Footnotes

1 Others have claimed that rejecting unfair offers, as a form of altruistic punishment, is necessary for human cooperation (see Civai, Corradi-Dell’Acque, Gamer & Rumiati, 2009, for review).

2 Correlational studies have demonstrated that the rejection of unfair offers is related to greater skin conductance responses (Reference van’t Wout and Kahnvan’t Wout, Kahn, Sanfey & Aleman, 2006), greater heart rate deceleration (Reference Osumi and OhiraOsumi & Ohira, 2009), and greater anterior insula activation (Reference Sanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom and CohenSanfey, Rilling, Aronson, Nystrom & Cohen, 2003), right ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Reference Tabibnia, Satpute and LiebermanTabibnia, Satpute & Lieberman, 2008), and amygdala activation (Reference Gospic, Mohlin, Fransson, Petrovic, Johannesson and IngvarGospic et al., 2011), all of which are implicated in emotional arousal, monitoring, and regulation. Furthermore, negative mood and affect (Reference Andrade and ArielyAndrade & Ariely, 2009; Reference Forgas and TanForgas & Tan, 2013; Reference Nguyen, Koenigs, Yamada, Teo and CavanaughNguyen et al., 2011) predict the rejection of unfair offers, and having the goal to stay calm increases acceptance of unfair offers (Reference Kirk, Gollwitzer and CarnevaleKirk, Gollwitzer & Carnevale, 2011). Experimentally, induced sadness leads to greater rejection of unfair ultimatum offers (Reference Harléy and SanfeyHarléy & Sanfey, 2007). Furthermore, individuals with training to regulate emotions (Buddhist meditators; Reference Kirk, Downar and MontagueKirk, Downar & Montague, 2011), those instructed to reappraise their emotions (Reference van’t Wout, Chang and Sanfeyvan’t Wout, Chang & Sanfey, 2010), and those given a benzodiazepine (Reference Gospic, Mohlin, Fransson, Petrovic, Johannesson and IngvarGospic et al., 2011) accept unfair offers more often than control participants. Thus, it appears that affective responses, which are a component of System 1, are related to the rejection of unfair offers.

3 Other studies have examined ultimatum game responses in populations that have previously demonstrated deficits in decision making abilities. As these deficits reflect System 2 processing limitations, evidence that these populations reject more unfair offers supports the automatic negative reciprocity hypothesis. Children and adolescent participants report higher minimal acceptable offers than college students when participants know how much money is being divided (Reference Murnighan and SaxonMurnighan & Saxon, 1998). Depressed individuals reject more offers of all types (fair, unfair, and hyperfair) than controls (Reference Radke, Schäfer, Müller and de BruijnRadke, Schäfer, Müller & de Bruijn, 2013), particularly slightly unfair offers (Reference Scheele, Mihov, Schwederski, Maier and HurlemannScheele, Mihov, Schwederski, Maier & Hurlemann, 2013), and platelet serotonin levels are greater among participants who accept unfair offers than among those who reject them, suggesting serotonin dysfunction is related to rejecting unfair offers (Reference Emanuele, Brondino, Bertona, Re and GeroldiEmanuele, Brondino, Bertona, Re & Geroldi, 2008). Similarly, depleting serotonin results in more rejection of unfair offers (Reference Crockett, Clark, Tabibnia, Lieberman and RobbinsCrockett et al., 2008). Furthermore, individuals with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (Reference Koenigs and TranelKoenigs & Tranel, 2007), individuals with frontotemporal dementia (Reference Cappa, Dodich, Cerami, Canessa, Crespi and MarconeCappa et al., 2014), men with Klinefelter syndrome (Reference Van and S.van Rijn, Swaab, Aleman & Kahn, 2006), and incarcerated individuals with symptoms of primary psychopathy (Reference Koenigs, Kruepke and NewmanKoenigs, Kruepke, & Newman, 2010) reject more unfair offers than control participants.

4 There is also evidence from populations that demonstrate limited decision making abilities. As these deficits reflect System 2 processing limitations, evidence that these populations accept more unfair offers supports the deliberate negative reciprocity hypothesis. Individuals with schizophrenia (Reference Csukly, Polgár, Tombor, Réthelyi and KériCsukly, Polgár, Tombor, Réthelyi & Kéri, 2011; but see Agay, Kron, Carmel, Mendlovic & Levkozitz, 2008), individuals with anxiety disorders (Reference Grecucci, Giorgetta, Brambilla, Zuanon and PeriniGrecucci et al., 2013), and individuals with borderline personality disorder (Polgár, Fogd, Unoka, Sirály & Czujky, 2014) accept unfair offers more frequently than healthy controls.

5 The REI-40 items are unavialable because of copyright restrictions.

6 The items taken from Baron et al. were coded in their Table 1 as Bl1, Bl2, Bl3, Bl4, Bl5, S2, S3, Al1, Al2, and Al3.

References

Agay, N., , Kron, S., S., Carmel, Z., , Mendlovic, S., , & Levkovitz, Y., (2008). Ultimatum bargaining behavior of people affected by schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 157, 3946. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2006.03.026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, C., & Dickinson, D. L. (2010). Bargaining and trust: The effects of 36-h total sleep deprivation on socially interactive decision. Journal of Sleep Research, 19 5463. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00767.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrade, E. B., & Ariely, D. (2009). The enduring impact of transient emotions on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 109 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2009.02.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, P. E., Ruffman, T., & Rendell, P. G. (2012). Age-related differences in social economic decision making: The ultimatum game. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 68B, 356363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs073.Google ScholarPubMed
Baron, J., Scott, S., Fincher, K., & Metz, S. E. (2015) Why does the Cognitive Reflection Test (sometimes) predict utilitarian moral judgment (and other things)?Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. Article in Press.Google Scholar
Brandts, J. & Solà, C. (2001). Reference points and negative reciprocity in simple sequential games. Games and Economic Behavior, 36 138157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/game.2000.0818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandstätter, H., & Konigstein, M. (2001). Personality influences on ultimatum bargaining decisions. European Journal of Personality, 15 S53-S70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/per.424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003) Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press: Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. A. (1999). Raising the stakes in the ultimatum game: Experimental evidence from Indonesia. Economic Inquiry, 37 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Labollita, M. (2010). Correlations of cognitive reflection with judgments and choices. Judgment and Decision Making, 5 182191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappa, S., Dodich, A., Cerami, C., Canessa, N., Crespi, C., & Marcone, A. (2014). Strategic choice in frontotemporal dementia: Evidence from the ultimatum game. Neurology, 82, supplement p3.218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappelletti, D., Güth, W., & Ploner, M. (2011). Being of two minds: Ultimatum offers under cognitive constraints. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32 940950. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Civai, C., Corradi-Dell’Acque, C., Gamer, M., & Rumiati, R. I. (2009). Are irrational reactions to unfairness truly emotionally-driven? Dissociated behavioural and emotional responses in the Ultimatum Game task. Cognition, 114 8995. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.001.Google ScholarPubMed
Cokely, E. T., & Kelley, C. M. (2009). Cognitive abilities and superior decision making under risk: A protocol analysis and process model evaluation. Judgment and Decision Making, 4 2033.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cone, J., & Rand, D. G. (2014). Time pressure increases cooperation in competitive social delimmas. PLoS ONE, 9 e115756. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, D. J., & Dutcher, E. G., (2011). The dynamics of responder behavior in ultimatum games: A meta-study. Experimental Economics, 14 519546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9280-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockett, M. J., Clark, L., Tabibnia, G., Lieberman, M. D., & Robbins, T. W. (2008). Serotonin modulates behavioral reactions to unfairness. Science, 320, 1739 . http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1155577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csukly, G., Polgár, P., Tombor, L., Réthelyi, F., & Kéri, S. (2011). Are patients with schizophrenia rational maximizers? Evidence from an ultimatum game study. Psychiatry Research, 187 1117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De, Neys,W.,Novitskiy,N.,Geeraerts,L.,Ramautar,J., &Wagemans,J.  (2011). Cognitive control and individual differences in economic ultimatum decision-making. PLoS ONE, 6 e27107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0027107.Google Scholar
Dunn, B. D., Evans, D., Makarova, D., White, J., & Clark, L. (2012). Gut feelings and the reaction to perceived inequity: The interplay between bodily responses, regulation, and perception shapes the rejection of unfair offers on the ultimatum game. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 12 419429. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13415-012-0092-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, B. D., Makaraova, D., Evans, D., & Clark, L. (2010). “I’m worth more than that”: Trait positivity predicts increased rejection of unfair financial offers. PLoSONE, 5 e15095. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0015095.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emanuele, E., Brondino, N., Bertona, M., Re, S., & Geroldi, D. (2008). Relationship between platelet serotonin content and rejections of unfair offers in the ultimatum game. Neuroscience Letters, 437 158161. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.10.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2003). In two minds: Dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 454459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59 255278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fetchenhauer, D., & Huang, X. (2004). Justice sensitivity and distributive decisions in experimental games. Personality and Individual Differences, 36 10151029. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00197-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P., & Tan, H. B. (2013). Mood effects on selfishness versus fairness: Affective influences on social decisions in the ultimatum game. Social Cognition, 31 504517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19 2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grecucci, A., Giorgetta, C., Brambilla, P., Zuanon, S., Perini, L.,…, Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Anxious ultimatums: How anxiety disorders affect socioeconomic behaviour. Cognition & Emotion, 27 230244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.698982.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gospic, K., Mohlin, E., Fransson, P., Petrovic, P., Johannesson, M., & Ingvar, M. (2011). Limbic justice: Amygdala involvement in immediate rejection in the ultimatum game. PLOS Biology, 9 e1001054. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, V. & Mengel, F. (2011). Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in ultimatum games. Economic Letters, 111 113-115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2011.01.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Güth, W., & Kocher, M. G. (2013). More than thirty years of ultimatum bargaining experiments: Motives, variations, and a survey of the recent literature. Jena Economic Research Papers ,No. 2013 -035. Google Scholar
Hack, A., & Lammers, F. (2009). Gender as a moderator of the fair process effect. Social Psychology, 40 202211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335.40.4.202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halali, E., Bereby-Meyer, Y., & Meiran, N. (2014). Between self-interest and reciprocity: The social bright side of self-control failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143 745-754. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harléy, K. M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2007). Incidental sadness biases social economic decisions in the ultimatum game. Emotion, 7 876-881. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, W., & Baumert, A. (2010). Immediate affect as a basis for intuitive moral judgement: An adaptation of the affect misattribution procedure. Cognition & Emotion, 24 522535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699930902847193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kieslich, P. J., & Hilbig, B. E. (2014). Cognitive conflict in social dilemmas: An analysis of response dynamics. Judgment and Decision Making, 9 510522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, D., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Carnevale, P. J. (2011). Self-regulation in ultimatum bargaining: Goals and plans help accepting unfair but profitable offers. Social Cognition, 29 528546. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.5.528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, U., Downar, J., & Montague, P. R. (2011). Interoception drives increased rational decision-making in mediators playing the ultimatum game. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5 111. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2001.00049.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoch, D., Pascual-Leone, A., Meyer, K., Treyer, V., & Fehr, E. (2006). Diminishing reciprocal fairness by disrupting the right prefrontal cortex. Science, 314 829832. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1129156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., & Newman, J. P. (2010). Economic decision-making in psychopathy: A comparison with ventromedial prefrontal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia, 48 21982204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. Journal of Neuroscience, 27 951955. http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4606-06.2007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kubota, J. T., Li, J., Bar-David, E., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2013). The price of racial bias: Intergroup negotiations in the ultimatum game. Psychological Science, 24 24982504. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613496435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morewedge, C. K., Krishnamurti, T., & Ariely, D. (2014). Focused on fairness: Alcohol intoxication increases the costly rejection of inequitable rewards. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50 1520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.08.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murnighan, J. K., & Saxon, M. S. (1998). Ultimatum bargaining by children and adults. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19 415445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mussel, P., Göritz, A. S., & Hewig, J. (2013a). Which choice is the rational one? An investigation of need for cognition in the ultimatum game. Journal of Research in Personality, 47 588591. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mussel, P., Göritz, A. S., & Hewig, J. (2013b). The value of a smile: Facial expression affects ultimatum-game responses. Judgment and Decision Making, 8 381385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nguyen, C. M., Koenigs, M., Yamada, T. H., Teo, S. H., Cavanaugh, J. E.,…, Denburg, N. L. (2011). Trustworthiness and negative affect predict economic decision making. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23 748759. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2011.575773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oechssler, J., Roider, A., & Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72 147-152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2009.04.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osumi, T., & Ohira, H. (2009). Cardiac responses predict decisions: An investigation of the relation between orienting response and decisions in the ultimatum game. Journal of Psychophysiology, 74 7479. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.07.007.Google ScholarPubMed
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of relational and experiential processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 972987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillutla, M. M., & Murnighan, J. K. (1996). Unfairness, anger, and spite: Emotional rejections of ultimatum offers. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68 208224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polgár, P., Fogd, D., Unoka, Z., Sirály, E., Csukly, G. (2014). Altered social decision making in borderline personality disorder: An ultimatum game study. Journal of Personality Disorders. 28 841852. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radke, S., Schäfer, I. C., Müller, B. W., & de Bruijn, E. R. A. (2013). Do different fairness contexts and facial emotions motivate ‘irrational’ social decision-making in major depression? An exploratory patient study. Psychiatry Research, 210 438443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.07.017.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanfey, A. G., & Chang, L. J. (2008). Multiple systems in decision making. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1128 53-62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1196/annals.1399.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the ultimatum game. Science, 300 17551758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scheele, D., Mihov, Y., Schwederski, O., Maier, W., & Hurlemann, R. (2013). A negative emotional and economic judgment bias in major depression. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 263 675683. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0392-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shen, J., & Takahashi, H. (2013). A cash effect in ultimatum game experiments. Journal of Socio-Economics, 47 94102. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2013.09.005.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. G. (2002). Rational actors or rational fools: Implications of the affect heuristic for behavioral economics. Journal of Socio-Economics, 31 329342.Google Scholar
Solnick, S. J., & Schweitzer, M. E. (1999). The influence of physical attractiveness and gender on ultimatum game decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79 199215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Srivastava, J., Espinoza, F., & Fedorikhin, A. (2009). Coupling and decoupling of unfairness and anger in ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 22 475489. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bdm.631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sütterlin, S., Herbert, C., Schmitt, M., Kübler, A., & Vögele, C. (2011). Overcoming selfishness: Reciprocity, inhibition, and cardiac-autonomic control in the ultimatum game. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 173. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008) Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19 339-347. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The Cognitive Reflection Test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39 12751289. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s1342-011-0104-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2014). Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test. Thinking & Reasoning, 20 147168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2013.844729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van’t Wout, M., , Chang, L. J., , & Sanfey, A. G., (2010). The influence of emotion regulation on social interactive decision-making. Emotion, 10 815821. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10020069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van’t Wout, M., Kahn, R. S., , Sanfey, A. G., & Aleman, A. (2006). Affective state and decision-making in the ultimatum game. Experimental Brain Research, 169 564568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00221-006-0346-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van, Rijn,S.,Swaab,H.,Aleman,A., &Kahn, S., R. (2006) X chromosomal effects on social cognitive processing and emotion regulation: A study with Klinefelter men (47, XXY). Schizophrenia Research, 84 194203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2006.02.020.Google Scholar
Yamagishi, T., Horita, Y., Mifune, N., Hashimoto, H., Li, Y.,…, Simunovic, D. (2012). Rejection of unfair offers in the ultimatum game is no evidence of strong reciprocity. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 109 2036420368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1212126109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1: Descriptive statistics and correlations of CRT7 scores and REI-40 scales with ultimatum decision (0 = reject, 1 = accept).

Figure 1

Table 2: CRT7 scores and REI-40 scales based on whether participants accepted or rejected the unfair ultimatum offer.

Figure 2

Figure 1: The acceptance rates of offers across offer values based on CRT17 performance.

Supplementary material: File

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material 1
Download Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material(File)
File 25.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material 2
Download Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material(File)
File 35.9 KB
Supplementary material: File

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material

Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material 3
Download Calvillo and Burgeno supplementary material(File)
File 62.1 KB