Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-fcbfl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-30T20:30:56.274Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altruism, Ethnic Identity, and the Limits of Shared Hardship

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 March 2025

Ana Bracic*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI, USA

Abstract

Are people more inclined to help strangers when they’ve experienced similar hardships? People who have experienced displacement could be tremendous allies to the newly displaced, but they are relatively understudied. This study explores how people who have experienced wartime displacement respond to refugees fleeing new violence. I prime Serbs who experienced wartime displacement with either (1) their experience of displacement or (2) their ethnic identity. I then measure their altruism toward Syrian refugees traveling the Balkan route. Compared to participants who were reminded of their ethnic identity, participants who were reminded of their displacement were no more generous toward displaced Syrians. In fact, participants who experienced displacement, as well as wartime violence, were more generous toward the refugees when they were reminded of their ethnic identity. These results suggest that shared hardship alone may not necessarily enhance refugee inclusion. The results further suggest that interventions may benefit from calling out the differences between hosts and refugees—in this case, on the dimension of ethnicity. These findings caution humanitarians to construct their interventions with care.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Race, Ethnicity, and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

Traumatic events affect how people behave. Exposure to wartime violence can hamper political participation (Lyall Reference Lyall2009) or increase it (Bateson Reference Bateson2012; Bellows and Miguel Reference Bellows and Miguel2009; Blattman Reference Blattman2009). Experiencing violence can lead to apathy (Wood Reference Wood2006), but also to backlash mobilization (Francisco Reference Francisco2004) and stronger social cohesion (Gilligan et al. Reference Gilligan, Pasquale and Samii2014). These diverging findings suggest that the relationship between traumatic events and behavior is meaningful, but far from simple. In this article, I explore how people who experienced wartime displacement behave toward distant strangers who experienced the same.

In particular, I explore whether drawing attention to someone’s identity as a displaced person affects their altruism toward displaced strangers who are members of an ethnic outgroup. The idea of “altruism born of suffering” (Staub Reference Staub2003, Vollhardt Reference Vollhardt2009) posits that people who have experienced hardship may be more likely to help others. Whether such helping behaviors are limited to ingroup members is less clear; some studies suggest that this is the case (Gilligan et al. Reference Gilligan, Pasquale and Samii2014, Voors et al. Reference Voors, Nillesen, Verwimp, Bulte, Lensink and Van Soest2012), while others do not (Bauer et al. Reference Bauer, Cassar, Chytilová and Henrich2013, Whitt and Wilson Reference Whitt and Wilson2007). I explore the flexibility of ingroup salience by recategorizing strangers ordinarily seen as outgroup members into ingroup members, where the new ingroup is defined by a superordinate identity—an identity that is shared and includes both groups (Gaertner et al. Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio, Bachman and Rust1993, Transue Reference Transue2007). I then examine whether such recategorization results in more altruism exhibited toward the new ingroup.

To explore this relationship between shared experience of hardship and altruism, I study formerly displaced ethnic Serbs’ reactions to Syrian refugees traveling along the Balkan route. Following the Yugoslav wars of the 1990s and early 2000s, almost a quarter million refugees and internally displaced people remain in Serbia. Since 2015, hundreds of thousands of other refugees, mostly from Syria and Afghanistan, have traveled through Serbia on their way to northern Europe. Formerly displaced ethnic Serbs provide a valuable opportunity to examine the role of shared hardship because they do not share characteristics that might otherwise be used to elicit a sense of commonality with the new refugees, like common religion or ethnicity. Rather, this context provides a hard test of recategorization as Serbs displaced during the Yugoslav wars may harbor anti-Muslim sentiment, which was relevant in the ethno-religious context of those wars and which may also matter in the context of refugees from Syria and Afghanistan, many of whom are perceived to be Muslim.

Serbian public discourse discussed Serbia’s role in the so-called refugee crisis in two ways (Šelo Šabić Reference Šelo Šabić2017). First, it emphasized empathy that Serbian citizens might feel given their shared wartime displacement experiences. Second, it highlighted Serbian exceptionalism in its humane response to the refugees, particularly when compared to other Balkan route countries. These two narratives present an opportunity to examine how ingroup salience affects altruism: does a reminder of displacement (superordinate group) better motivate altruism than a reminder of Serbian (ingroup) identity?

To explore this question, I recruited participants who experienced wartime displacement and conducted an online survey experiment. Only individuals who experienced displacement due to conflicts in Croatia (1991–1995), Bosnia (1992–1995), or Kosovo (1998–1999) were included. Half of eligible participants received the superordinate “displacement” prime, while the other half received an ingroup “Serbian” prime; the primes reflect the Serbian discourse on the crisis and follow scholarship that compares two contextually salient primes (as opposed to a control and a treatment) (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; McClendon and Riedl Reference McClendon and Riedl2015). Participants then played the dictator game in which they had the option of sharing their participation earnings with an anonymous Syrian refugee family; this is a widely used behavioral measure of altruism (Hoffman et al. Reference Hoffman, McCabe, Shachat and Smith1994).

The priming increased the salience of the superordinate identity among treated participants, but had no effect on their altruism. In fact, participants who experienced wartime violence in addition to displacement were more generous in response to a treatment that primed the opposite—their Serbian identity. This challenges a number of findings on superordinate identity salience and altruism (Charnysh et al. Reference Charnysh, Lucas and Singh2015; Gaertner et al. Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio, Bachman and Rust1993; Levine and Thompson Reference Levine and Thompson2004; Riek et al. Reference Riek, Mania, Gaertner, McDonald and Lamoreaux2010) and suggests that simply increasing the salience of a superordinate displaced identity does not necessarily increase altruism toward displaced strangers. The findings suggest that host society interventions may even benefit from capitalizing on differences between hosts and refugees (Brewer Reference Brewer1991). More broadly, these findings speak to scholarship on identity salience (Klar Reference Klar2013; Diamond Reference Diamond2020), outgroup derogation (Hewstone et al. Reference Hewstone, Rubin and Willis2002), host society attitudes toward refugees (Bansak et al. Reference Bansak, Hainmueller and Hangartner2016), and immigrant inclusion (Gorodzeisky and Semyonov Reference Gorodzeisky and Semyonov2020; Schenk Reference Schenk2021; Tyrberg Reference Tyrberg2024).

Background

Reactions to Refugees

Host society reactions to refugees and asylum seekers vary, often in response to who the refugees are or how they are perceived. For example, survey participants across 15 European countries express significantly greater support for asylum applicants with high employability, severe vulnerabilities, and consistent asylum testimonies (Bansak et al. Reference Bansak, Hainmueller and Hangartner2016). In contrast, Muslim asylum applicants receive far less support (Bansak et al. Reference Bansak, Hainmueller and Hangartner2016) and often experience discrimination in the destination country (Paz and Kook Reference Paz and Kook2021). Contact and exposure are likewise important, if inconsistent, determinants of host society attitudes toward refugees (Altındağ and Kaushal Reference Altındağ and Kaushal2021, Getmansky et al. Reference Getmansky, Sınmazdemir and Zeitzoff2018, Zorlu Reference Zorlu2017).

Recent studies examine the effectiveness of several different interventions aimed at improving host society attitudes. First, perspective taking—asking the participant to imagine they were a refugee—results in supportive attitudes and behaviors among Americans with no personal experience of displacement (Adida et al. Reference Adida, Lo and Platas2018; Williamson et al. Reference Williamson, Adida, Lo, Platas, Prather and Werfel2021), among Greeks whose ancestors experienced displacement after the Greco-Turkish War of 1919–1922, and among Germans whose ancestors were expelled from the former Eastern territories of the German Reich after the end of World War II (Dinas et al. Reference Dinas, Fouka and Schläpfer2021). Second, emphasizing shared religion between Syrian refugees and Turkish citizens improves certain Turkish attitudes toward refugees and increases respondent donations to a refugee charity (Lazarev and Sharma Reference Lazarev and Sharma2017).

These recent studies reveal that while perspective taking and emphasizing shared religion appear to be effective in fostering refugee inclusion, some interventions fail and may even lead to backlash. For example, providing facts about countries’ commitments to accept refugees does not change attitudes among some and results in negative reactions among others (Adida et al. Reference Adida, Lo and Platas2018; Getmansky et al. Reference Getmansky, Sınmazdemir and Zeitzoff2018). Similarly, a positive reminder that an open-door policy saves innocent lives can reduce support for refugees (Getmansky et al. Reference Getmansky, Sınmazdemir and Zeitzoff2018). Thus, more research is needed not only concerning effective strategies but potentially counterproductive ones as well. This is especially true for understudied communities such as those that have experienced wartime displacement themselves (but see Hall Reference Hall2016, Reference Hall2018 and Corstange and York Reference Corstange and York2018). In addition, more work is needed to explore the role of ethnicity in these dynamics (Bracic Reference Bracic2022).

With this goal in mind, I examine an intervention proven successful in several contexts: recategorization based on a shared superordinate identity (Gaertner et al. Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio, Bachman and Rust1993; Transue Reference Transue2007). Like Lazarev and Sharma (Reference Lazarev and Sharma2017), I exploit a common identity between hosts and refugees, but unlike them, I choose an identity acquired by experiencing displacement. Like Dinas et al. (Reference Dinas, Fouka and Schläpfer2021), I leverage a personal experience of the same sort of hardship—displacement—but unlike them, I examine participants with first-hand experience of displacement. I also diverge from Dinas et al. (Reference Dinas, Fouka and Schläpfer2021) in examining the effect of a shared identity alone, without a perspective-taking component that explicitly draws parallels between participants’ personal experiences and a current refugee crisis. In doing so, I conduct a hard test by comparing the effects of a shared acquired identity to those of a highly salient identity dimension that constitutes a difference between hosts and refugees: ethnicity.Footnote 1 Finally, I examine host society reactions to refugees in a relatively underexamined context: Serbia.

Serbia

Serbia, along with other Western Balkan states, often appears in popular imagination as part of a volatile region shaped by deeply rooted ethno-nationalist sentiment. This view is, at best, simplistic (Todorova Reference Todorova1997). The wars that accompanied Yugoslavia’s dissolution in the 1990s and the 2000s were indeed fought along ethno-religious lines and resulted in the partition of a multi-ethnic Yugoslavian federation into states that were far less ethnically diverse (Subotić Reference Subotić2015). The causes of the wars, however, were complex and not rooted in ancient ethnic hatreds (Gagnon Reference Gagnon2004, Jović Reference Jović2001). Ethnic hatred was created anew—by cultural, political, and intellectual elites who engaged in manipulation of the public (Jović Reference Jović2001, Klanjšek and Flere Reference Klanjšek and Flere2011, Sekulić et al. Reference Sekulić, Massey and Hodson2006).

Since before the wars and continuing to present day, Serbian identity has been variously constructed, narrated, and contested. In Yugoslavia, under Tito’s leadership, identity narratives selectively highlighted aspects of unity among the Southern Slavic ethnic communities (Malešević Reference Malešević2002) to facilitate the construction of the supranational Yugoslavian identity (Kalemaj and Lleshi Reference Kalemaj, Lleshi, Meka and Bianchini2020). In the late 1980s, Serbia saw a shift in elite discourse. Serbian national identity became its focus, with discussions eventually coalescing around two opposing but mutually reinforcing constructions: First Serbia and Other Serbia (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016).

The First Serbia discourse was “all-penetrating and dominant” (Russell-Omaljev 86, Reference Russell-Omaljev2016) and placed Serbian ethnicity at its core. After gaining power in the League of Communists of Serbia in 1987, Milošević mobilized supporters through extreme ethno-nationalist rhetoric that relied on a set of myths no longer suppressed for the purpose of unity: the Kosovo myth, the myth of Saint Sava, the myth of victimization in Jasenovac, and myths of Serbian military victories and the sacrifices of its heroes (Malešević Reference Malešević2002). These myths collectively ground Serbian identity in narratives of sacrifice, victimhood, and heroism while presenting Serbian culture as largely unchanged since the 8th century. Their content is “solely ethnic (Serb) and occasionally religious (Orthodox Serb)” (Malešević Reference Malešević2002, 177). Serbian political and cultural elites invoked these myths in the period before and during the Yugoslav wars, strengthening the exclusionary identity by portraying Croats, Kosovar Albanians, the United States, and the European Union as enemies (Malešević Reference Malešević2002, Subotić Reference Subotić2011).

Other Serbia emerged in 1992 from the civic opposition to the Yugoslav wars and the Milošević regime. Witnessing atrocities committed in the name of ethnic superiority, Other Serbia struggled with the idea of a Serbian national identity and embraced a pacifist, civic collective identity grounded in moral and ethical principles rather than ethnicity (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016). Supported by a minority of citizens, Other Serbia explicitly rejected the dominant First Serbia discourse (Dawson Reference Dawson2014) and Serbian ethno-nationalism (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016). It openly fought against war crimes and ethnic cleansing.

In the post-2000 period, Other Serbia splintered when the anti-war stance no longer unified the liberal intelligentsia (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016). First Serbia discourse refocused on Europe in response to Europe’s view that Serbia was responsible for the breakup of Yugoslavia and the war atrocities (Subotić Reference Subotić2011). While ethnic nationalism became less central as a new divide emerged between moderate and fundamentalist nationalists (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016), the loss of Kosovo—which First Serbia considered inherently Serbian—reinforced ethno-nationalist sentiment (Subotić Reference Subotić2011). Though Kosovo’s loss remains a central issue today, narratives of the war appear less frequently in contemporary conversations about identity (Wygnańska Reference Wygnańska2021).

The Balkan Route

During the European refugee crisis between 2014 and 2017, over 3 million people applied for asylum in several European states (Eurostat 2018). Syrians and Afghans comprised the two largest asylum applicant groups; the remainder were predominantly from Iraq, Pakistan, Albania, Eritrea, Kosovo, Somalia, Nigeria, and Bangladesh (Dustmann et al. Reference Dustmann, Fasani, Frattini, Minale and Schönberg2016). Depending on their country of origin, most refugees reached the EU through Mediterranean routes, the route along the border of the easternmost EU member states, routes through the Canary Islands and the Iberian peninsula, or the western Balkan route (Frontex European Border & Coast Guard Agency 2018).

With its history of conflict and its prominent position on the Balkan route, Serbia presents an opportunity to explore the link between experiences of displacement and altruism. Serbia is home to many people experiencing displacement, either as refugees or as persons internally displaced during the Yugoslav wars. In total, Serbia has hosted 537,937 refugees, mostly from conflicts in Croatia (1991–1995) and Bosnia and Herzegovina (1992–1995), and 209,021 internally displaced persons, mostly from the conflict in Kosovo (1998–1999) (KIRS 2016). In June 2014, 43,763 refugees still lived in Serbia, as did 204,049 internally displaced people (KIRS 2016), comprising a total of 3.5% of the Serbian population (KIRS 2016, RZS 2016); the actual proportion may have been even higher as only those formally registered as displaced were included in the official count.

In recent years, Serbs have witnessed tens of thousands of refugees traveling the Balkan route. From October 2015 to February 2016, between 1,000 and 10,000 refugees entered Serbia daily, most continuing north toward Germany and beyond (UNHCR 2016). In early March 2016, Austrian border restrictions aimed at closing the Balkan route triggered a wave of similar policies in the countries to the south. Following these border closures, the number of refugees traveling the Balkan route was dramatically reduced, although substantial numbers still passed through, legally and illegally (Vasovic Reference Vasovic2016). In March 2017, the UNHCR reported that 7,900 refugees entered Serbia legally; 707 of them formally registered their intent to seek asylum there (UNHCR 2017).

Serbian public discourse surrounding the crisis differed substantially from discourse in many other European countries. While some press coverage in Hungary and France persistently promoted hostility and hate speech, and while the media in Czechia, France, and the United Kingdom wrote more about national security than about caring for the newly displaced, the Serbian press focused on helping refugees (Georgiou and Zaborowski Reference Georgiou and Zaborowski2017). Early narratives concentrated on the criminality of smuggling networks, but as the numbers passing through rose, humanitarian narratives prevailed, emphasizing the human dimension of the crisis (Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018; Šelo Šabić Reference Šelo Šabić2017).

One common narrative favorably compared Serbia’s response to the crisis with those of its neighbors (Galijaš Reference Galijaš2019, Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018). Serbia was thus “saving Europe’s soul” (Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018, 223) and “teaching Europe a lesson” (223) while “Bulgaria [set] tanks on refugees” (224), “Croats [forced] women and children into a frozen river” (224), and Hungary’s Victor Orbán, the “fascist from the heart of Europe” (224), built a wall. This framing presented Serbian “hospitality and kindness toward refugees” (Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018, 221) as a stark departure from how EU members and other states along the Balkan route approached the crisis.

Another common narrative connected Serbs and the refugees, highlighting Serbian empathy stemming from the experience of displacement and wartime violence during the Yugoslav conflicts (Šelo Šabić Reference Šelo Šabić2017). For example, in response to a photograph of a smiling Serbian policeman holding a Syrian child, some Serbs reported experiencing “the moment of salvation, because they themselves have been through the war” (Blic, 12 September 2015, 4 in Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018, 223).

Scholars link these narratives to a combination of politically salient factors. The first is Serbia’s need to rebuild its image in the aftermath of the atrocities during the Yugoslav wars and its stigma as a “rogue state” (Petrović Trifunović and Poleti Ćosić Reference Petrović Trifunović, Poleti Ćosić, Pudar Draško, Pavlović and Meka2018, 212). The second is Serbia’s status as an EU candidate. The refugee crisis allowed Serbia to advance both agendas by linking the policy of welcoming to memories of Serbia’s own wartime plight and by showcasing the country as a competent and responsible champion of European values (Šelo Šabić Reference Šelo Šabić2017). These efforts were recognized in December 2015 with the opening of the first accession negotiation chapters. Finally, scholars have also noted that Serbian discourse reflects its status as a transit country and not a destination (Galijaš Reference Galijaš2019). Indeed, as countries along the Balkan route progressively closed their borders, the discourse in Serbia became more security-driven (Sardelić Reference Sardelić2017; Šelo Šabić Reference Šelo Šabić2017).

In short, Serbia offers a compelling opportunity to explore how previously displaced people behave toward displaced strangers in a real-life context, given its (1) residents who experienced displacement, (2) prominent position on the Balkan route, and (3) crisis-response narratives of ingroup exceptionalism and shared hardship.

Theoretical Expectations

Humans possess and can activate multiple social identities. Which social identity is activated depends on the environment, as individuals self-categorize based on contextually salient social categories (Turner et al. Reference Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher and Wetherell1987). These social categories then classify the ingroup, to which the individual belongs, and outgroups (Tajfel and Turner Reference Tajfel and Turner1979). Once people are categorized into groups, perceived differences between members of the same group are minimized (Tajfel Reference Tajfel1969), while perceived differences between ingroup and outgroup members are enhanced. When an individual activates a social identity, she begins to behave as a member of that ingroup (Turner and Onorato Reference Turner, Onorato, Tom, Roderick and Oliver1999).

People tend to systematically favor their own group members over those of other groups (Billing and Tajfel Reference Billing and Tajfel1973; Dasgupta Reference Dasgupta2004). This bias can manifest itself in ingroup favoritism, outgroup derogation, or both (Hewstone, Rubin, and Willis Reference Hewstone, Rubin and Willis2002). Prosocial behaviors that benefit others are more commonly exhibited toward an ingroup: people are more likely to help, trust, or share resources with members of their own group (Penner et al. Reference Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin and Schroeder2005). Correspondingly, people are less likely to be prosocial toward outsiders. For example, Greeks donate more money to feed ethnic Greek children than Roma children (Linos et al. Reference Linos, Jakli and Carlson2021), and Germans are more likely to help a German native than a Muslim immigrant after a mishap (Choi et al. Reference Choi, Poertner and Sambanis2019). More generally, people who strongly identify with their national ingroup tend to express more support for exclusionary immigration policies, though this depends on the way people think about national identity (Bracic et al. Reference Bracic, Israel-Trummel and Shortle2023; Breidahl et al. Reference Breidahl, Holtug and Kongshøj2018; Goodman and Alarian Reference Goodman and Alarian2021; Rapp Reference Rapp2022; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2011). When the national ingroup is defined in civic terms—a commitment to the country’s laws and institutions—people are more open to refugees, migrants, or asylum seekers. When the ingroup is instead defined in ethnic terms, commitment to the national ingroup results in exclusionary attitudes (Esses et al. Reference Esses, Hamilton and Gaucher2017).

After Tito’s Yugoslavia, views of Serbian national identity have been shaped by the dominant First Serbia discourse, which grounded the identity in ethnicity, and its much smaller opposite, Other Serbia, which grounded it in civic terms (Russell-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016). While the two counterparts have since divided into multiple visions of identity that fill out the space between them, ethnocultural narratives of Kosovo belonging to Serbia persist and are quite common (Wygnańska Reference Wygnańska2021). For example, 78 percent of the participants in this study strongly agree that Kosovo and Metohija belong to Serbia.Footnote 2 And Serbs who believe that “every country belongs primarily to its first inhabitants” are more willing to engage in an anti-refugee demonstration (Hasbún López et al. Reference Hasbún López, Martinović, Bobowik, Chryssochoou, Cichocka, Ernst-Vintila, Franc, Fülöp, Ghilani, Kochar, Lamberty, Leone, Licata and Žeželj2019). It is therefore reasonable to expect that ethnic identity remains salient in Serbia and that it’s linked to exclusionary attitudes.

Group categorization is often spontaneous, but it is not unalterable. The characteristics on the basis of which categorization occurs can be modified by manipulating the perceiver’s expectations, goals, motives, past experiences, as well as situational factors (Dovidio and Gaertner Reference Dovidio and Gaertner2000). Ingroups form based on random assignment and clothing color (Wright et al. Reference Wright, Aron, McLaughlin-Volpe and Ropp1997), but also on the basis of ethnicity (Montoya and Pittinsky Reference Montoya and Pittinsky2016), disability (Darling Reference Darling2013), and environmental disaster experience (Vezzali et al. Reference Vezzali, Cadamuro, Versari, Giovannini and Trifiletti2015). Personal experience of displacement should therefore also enable people to form ingroups.

Recategorization, as proposed by the Common Ingroup Identity Model (Gaertner et al. Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio, Bachman and Rust1993), seeks to reshape group boundaries by creating an inclusive ingroup in place of two or more outgroups. While recategorization processes don’t do away with the smaller groups, they increase the salience of the superordinate identity, emphasizing the shared bond between the smaller groups. As the salience of a superordinate identity common to two outgroups increases, behaviors between members should improve following the cognitive processes that motivate ingroup favoritism (Gaertner at al. Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Nier, Banker, Ward, Houlette, Loux, Capozza and Brown2000). Interventions that emphasize superordinate identities, ranging from dress similarity (Dovidio et al. Reference Dovidio, Gaertner, Isen and Lowrance1995) to party identification (Riek et al. Reference Riek, Mania, Gaertner, McDonald and Lamoreaux2010), can reduce intergroup bias and lead to increased helping behaviors (Dovidio et al. Reference Dovidio, Gaertner, Validzic, Matoka, Johnson and Frazier1997; Levine et al. Reference Levine, Prosser, Evans and Reicher2005). For example, British citizens whose identity as Europeans was made salient indicate a substantially higher willingness to donate money to help victims of an environmental disaster in Europe than participants whose British identity was salient instead (Levine and Thompson Reference Levine and Thompson2004). Among Australians, those identifying strongly as humans are substantially more welcoming toward asylum seekers, while those strongly identifying as Australians are not (Nickerson and Louis Reference Nickerson and Louis2008). Indian Hindus whose salience of their Indian national identity was increased express more altruism toward Indian Muslims (Charnysh et al. Reference Charnysh, Lucas and Singh2015).

The relationship between shared hardship and altruism grounded in the common ingroup identity model could result from several mechanisms. People with a universal orientation find it easier to see similarities with others (Phillips and Ziller Reference Phillips and Ziller1997), while people with higher levels of empathy might be more likely to draw parallels between their own experiences and someone else’s (Zaki and Ochsner Reference Zaki and Ochsner2012). Empathy and universal orientation are mechanisms that could lead a person who experienced displacement to form inclusive victim consciousness—a perception that the suffering of their ingroup is similar to the suffering of other groups (Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021). Inclusive victim consciousness may then promote altruism toward others who experienced displacement.

An alternative mechanism—perceived ingroup superiority—is a perception that fosters a competitive posture toward other groups (Roccas et al. Reference Roccas, Sagiv, Schwartz, Halevy and Eidelson2008) and positions the ingroup as morally superior (Noor et al. Reference Noor, Shnabel, Halabi and Nadler2012). People who see their ingroup as superior may develop exclusive victim consciousness where they perceive their own experiences of suffering as unique (Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Nair and Tropp2016). This likely fosters negative attitudes toward other groups who also suffered (Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021). Finally, intergroup contact could also shape the relationship between shared hardship and altruism, but the direction of the effect may vary (Homola and Tavits Reference Homola and Tavits2018, Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021, Zorlu Reference Zorlu2017). These mechanisms illuminate the potential relationship between shared hardship and altruism. This study, however, is not designed to distinguish between them.

Serbian public discourse on the refugee crisis appealed to both ingroup and superordinate identities. The first narrative highlights Serbian exceptionalism and could evoke feelings of ingroup favoritism. The second narrative focuses on shared hardship, evoking memories of Serbia’s own wartime plight. Among Serbs who experienced wartime displacement, such a narrative might increase the salience of a superordinate displacement identity. During the refugee crisis, Serbs were likely exposed to these narratives. Given the contextual salience of these narratives, I therefore compare the effectiveness of an ingroup and a superordinate prime, foregoing a control condition (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; McClendon and Riedl Reference McClendon and Riedl2015). Following social identity theory, I expect participants receiving the ingroup ethnic identity prime to express less altruism toward refugees traveling the Balkan route. By contrast, I expect that recategorization will lead to higher altruism toward the refugees:

H1: Participants whose displaced identity is primed will, on average, engage in significantly higher levels of altruism than participants whose ingroup identity is primed.

While all those displaced in war suffer, such experiences will differ. A meaningful source of variation is exposure to violence, which can affect attitudes and behaviors, political or otherwise (Bateson Reference Bateson2012, Lyall Reference Lyall2009, Wood Reference Wood2006). In Burundi, for example, conflict victimization at the individual level is positively associated with altruistic behavior (Voors et al. Reference Voors, Nillesen, Verwimp, Bulte, Lensink and Van Soest2012). In Nepal, violence-affected communities also exhibit higher levels of altruism and cooperation (Gilligan et al. Reference Gilligan, Pasquale and Samii2014).

The intensity of violence also matters. In Sierra Leone, people from households that experienced more direct civil war victimization—such as murder or arson—are more likely to vote, join local political and community groups, and attend community meetings (Bellows and Miguel Reference Bellows and Miguel2009). Ex-combatants from northern Uganda, who were victims of rebel abduction and conscription, have substantially higher rates of voting and becoming community leaders (Blattman Reference Blattman2009). Among them, witnessing violence accounts for a substantial portion of this relationship. Still, while numerous studies demonstrate higher altruism among people who experienced wartime violence, this relationship is not universal. In the Republic of Georgia and Sierra Leone, increased altruism is only expressed toward ingroup members (Bauer et al. Reference Bauer, Cassar, Chytilová and Henrich2013), while in Kyrgyzstan, people in victimized neighborhoods express less prosociality toward ingroup and outgroup members alike (Hager et al. Reference Hager, Krakowski and Schaub2019).

Nevertheless, as most research shows that exposure to wartime violence increases prosociality, my second hypothesis is:

H2: Participants who experienced war violence in addition to displacement will, on average, engage in significantly higher levels of altruism than participants who did not.

Empirical Strategy

I conducted an online survey experiment. A local research firm, Ninamedia, administered the survey between June 1 and August 2, 2017. Participants were recruited using targeted Facebook advertisements, a method that has recently been used to recruit immigrant respondents (Tyrberg Reference Tyrberg2024). I chose this method of recruitment after exhausting several other recruiting options, none of which garnered a sufficient number of participants. I discuss the unsuccessful strategies in the Online Appendix. While not ideal, this recruitment strategy is better than snowball sampling, which is often used to recruit hard-to-reach populations but results in participants who are interconnected and often similar to one another (Shaghaghi et al. Reference Shaghaghi, Bhopal and Sheikh2011). In fact, Facebook-recruited convenience samples can provide useful evidence for experimental treatment effects, even though they have lower generalizability (Krupnikov et al. Reference Krupnikov, Nam, Style, James and Donald2021).

Given the sensitive nature of my online survey experiment, recruitment materials and the consenting procedure explicitly identified it as a study of wartime displacement and reactions to the European refugee crisis. A separate statement in the survey itself warned participants about upcoming questions about their wartime experience; if they wished, they could skip these questions without reading them while still completing the survey.

Potential participants first answered four screening questions. They were eligible to participate if they experienced displacement either as refugees or as internally displaced persons during the conflicts in (1) Croatia (1991–1995); (2) Bosnia (1992–1995); or (3) Kosovo (1998–1999); and (4) were at least seven years old when the conflict occurred (Bauer et al. Reference Bauer, Cassar, Chytilová and Henrich2013).Footnote 3 The sample consists of 384 ethnic Serbs who experienced displacement.Footnote 4

Eligible participants randomly received either the ingroup Serbian prime or the superordinate displaced prime, reflecting the common narratives discussed earlier (Chong and Druckman Reference Chong and Druckman2007; McClendon and Riedl Reference McClendon and Riedl2015). To increase the salience of either the ingroup ethnic identity or the superordinate identity, I used a questions-as-treatments framework and asked participants a series of questions listed in the Appendix (Bloom et al. Reference Bloom, Arikan and Courtemanche2015, Transue Reference Transue2007, Tyrberg Reference Tyrberg2024). The final question in both primes was open-ended, asking for a short answer to “What does it mean to be Serbian?” or “What does it mean to be displaced?” The number and selection of priming questions were determined in consultation with local experts.Footnote 5 While the experimental treatments reflect the narratives in Serbian discourse on the crisis, the treatments do not directly mention the refugee crisis. The ingroup identity prime therefore primes only the Serbian ethnic ingroup, without reference to the refugee crisis or Serbia’s role in it. The superordinate prime likewise primes the displaced identity by referring to past experiences of displacement in the Yugoslav wars without referencing the refugee crisis.Footnote 6

I captured altruism by asking participants to play the dictator game (Hoffman et al. Reference Hoffman, McCabe, Shachat and Smith1994):Footnote 7

To thank you for participating in this study, the University of Oklahoma gives you 800 dinars. If you wish, you have the opportunity to divide these 800 dinars between you and a Syrian refugee family in need that would like to seek asylum in Serbia. The family will be chosen with the aid of the Commissariat for Refugees and Migration of the Republic of Serbia and will remain anonymous. After this study is over, this family will receive the bulk sum of whatever the participants in this study send to them. You don’t have to send anything to this family, but if you want to send something, type the amount in the box below.

The survey continued with several questions capturing attitudes and relevant population covariates such as age (Freund and Blanchard-Fields Reference Freund and Blanchard-Fields2014), income (Chowdhury and Jeon Reference Chowdhury and Jeon2014), gender (Croson and Gneezy Reference Croson and Gneezy2009), education, and exposure to war violence (Grossman et al. Reference Grossman, Manekin and Miodownik2015).

A word on generalizability. The sample is not representative of ethnic Serbs who have experienced displacement. Since recruitment materials were explicit about the purpose of the study, it is likely that people who self-selected into the study were willing to contemplate their own displacement. The sample is overly representative of people who are comfortable with technology, and Facebook in particular, and also disproportionately female. While gender effects on altruism are inconsistent and overall null (Balliet et al. Reference Balliet, Li, Macfarlan and Van Vugt2011), estimating the direction of the other biases in relation to altruism is difficult. The results of the analysis that follows should therefore not be generalized beyond this sample of participants.

Results

This section first discusses the effectiveness of priming and then presents the dictator game results. Briefly, the superordinate priming questions increased the comparative salience of the displaced identity, but the increased salience of this identity resulted in neither higher nor lower average contributions to the Syrian family in the dictator game (no support for H1). Participants who experienced wartime violence donated significantly more to the refugee family (support for H2) but did so in response to the ingroup and not the displacement prime.

The Relative Salience of the Superordinate Identity

The manipulation check following the priming questions asked participants to rate how strongly they felt like a Serbian and how strongly they felt like a displaced individual (5-point scale).Footnote 8 The superordinate treatment group reported a significantly higher average salience of the displaced identity compared to the ingroup prime participants (p < 0.05; t-test). The average salience of Serbian identity, however, was the same for both groups (see Table 1). Further, among ingroup prime participants, the average salience of the displaced identity was significantly lower than the average salience of Serbian identity (p < 0.05; t-test). This was not the case for the superordinate prime participants; they felt just as strongly displaced as they felt Serbian (see Table 2). The manipulation check suggests that the superordinate priming questions achieved their intended result. I next examine whether the priming that successfully raised the salience of the superordinate identity also increased altruism.

Table 1. Salience of displaced and ethnic identities, between groups (t-test)

Table 2. Salience of displaced and ethnic identities, within groups (t-test)

Levels of Altruism Exhibited by the Two Groups

Even though participants receiving the superordinate prime reported a significantly higher salience of their identity as displaced individuals, they were no more generous toward the Syrian refugee family than ingroup prime participants. Figure 1 presents the average amount sent to the Syrian refugee family by those receiving the ethnic ingroup and superordinate primes. The difference is not statistically significant: participants sent 29.5% (ethnic ingroup prime) and 24% (superordinate prime) of their 800 dinars to the family.

Figure 1. This figure presents the average amount of dinars sent to the Syrian refugee family in need along with 95% confidence intervals, by prime. Participants had an endowment of 800 dinars.

To control for covariates possibly influencing decision-making, I use a regression. The first column in Table 3 presents OLS regression results with covariates for age, gender, income, education, and personal experience of wartime violence.

Table 3. OLS regression results (dependent variable: amount sent in the dictator game)

Note: *p < 0.05

Regression results confirm the initial null finding.Footnote 9 As the superordinate prime had no discernible bearing on the participants’ levels of generosity, I have no support for H1, which hypothesized that a more salient superordinate identity would result in more altruism.Footnote 10 This result suggests that recategorization, even when it works, does not always lead to prosociality.

Personal Experience of Conflict

Here I examine whether personal experience of conflict among displaced Serbs moderates the effect of the superordinate prime. This was not a pre-registered line of inquiry and is therefore exploratory, but it offers insight into the null finding. To take a closer look at the role of exposure to violence, I divide the sample into two subgroups: (1) participants who experienced displacement and also lost someone close to them in the war and (2) participants who experienced displacement but didn’t lose anyone.

Participants who did not lose a close person during the war contributed the same amount in the dictator game, on average, regardless of the two primes (left panel of Figure 2). Participants who lost someone close to them in the war, however, responded differently to the two primes. Those who received the ingroup ethnic identity prime contributed significantly more to the refugee family. Figure 2 (right panel) shows that this group was more altruistic than participants who experienced both displacement and violence but who instead received the superordinate prime. Table 3 shows the results of two additional OLS models, now split by subgroup. The findings in column 2 are for the displacement-only subgroup and show the null effect of the displacement prime. The findings in column 3 are for the displacement and war violence subgroup and show a negative effect of the displacement prime.Footnote 11

Figure 2. This figure presents the average amount of dinars sent to the Syrian refugee family in need along with 95% confidence intervals, by prime and by personal experience of violence. Participants had an endowment of 800 dinars.

In H2, I expected that participants who experienced wartime violence would exhibit more generosity. They did so, but only when receiving the ingroup ethnic identity prime. This result presents a challenge to research on altruism that finds that people who have experienced violence favor ingroup members (Bauer et al. Reference Bauer, Cassar, Chytilová and Henrich2013). It also underscores what has been found elsewhere: personal experience of violence is a factor that shapes other-regarding behavior (Gilligan et al. Reference Gilligan, Pasquale and Samii2014, Voors et al. Reference Voors, Nillesen, Verwimp, Bulte, Lensink and Van Soest2012).

Additional Exploratory Analyses

Here, I conduct a few additional analyses. Since this study was not designed to test additional explanations, most of the measures that I use in this section are adequate for exploratory purposes, but not ideal. Several are also subject to post-treatment bias (Montgomery et al. Reference Montgomery, Nyhan and Torres2018). In most of the analyses that follow, I therefore examine the two treatment groups separately. To be clear, these analyses do not offer any definitive answers, only suggestive insight for future work. I examine feelings of closeness toward refugees, generalized anti-Muslim sentiment, contact, empathy, ideology, perceived threat, conceptions of Serbian identity, and positive identity attachment.

There are several factors that potentially shape the relationship between shared hardship and altruism. First, if people develop exclusive victim consciousness, shared hardship may not lead to altruism (Vollhardt and Bilali Reference Vollhardt and Bilali2015). Exclusive victim beliefs are based on perceived distinctiveness of ingroup victimization (Brewer Reference Brewer1991, Vollhardt Reference Vollhardt2013). For example, when the Holocaust is invoked in discussions about other rights abuses, some Jewish people welcome the reference (Schulz Reference Schulz2001, Wayne and Zhukov Reference Wayne and Zhukov2022), while others argue that it erodes the unique character and horror of the Holocaust (Rothberg Reference Rothberg2011). While this survey did not probe for feelings of distinctiveness explicitly, I asked participants how close they felt toward their ethnic ingroup, toward people who experienced wartime displacement in general, and toward Syrian and Afghan refugees. The only feelings of closeness that matter are the specific feelings felt toward Syrians and Afghans. The more closeness participants in both ingroup and superordinate subsamples felt toward these particular refugees, the more generous they were toward the family in need (Appendix Tables 15-17).

Second, when animus exists between two subgroups, recategorization is more challenging (Vollhardt Reference Vollhardt2009). The Yugoslav wars, during which this study’s participants experienced displacement, were fought along ethno-religious lines. Generalized anti-Muslim sentiment might be salient, presenting a barrier to recategorization such that ethnic Serbs may be less open to the idea of shared hardship with refugees who are largely perceived to be Muslim. I find, however, that generalized anti-Muslim sentiment was not statistically significant in either treatment group (Appendix Table 15). Exploratory evidence further shows that Serbian participants feel significantly more warmly toward Syrian refugees than they do toward Kosovar Albanians, which indicates that ethnic Serbs distinguish between wartime enemies and the new refugees—even though both groups are perceived to be Muslim (Appendix Table 18). While participants overwhelmingly subscribe to the ethno-nationalist notion that Kosovo and Metohija belong to Serbia and exhibit proportionate levels of animus toward Kosovar Albanians, these attitudes do not generalize to the refugees.

Finally, contact and empathy may shape altruism in light of shared hardship (Szabo et al. Reference Szabó, Vollhardt and Mészáros2020, Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021). I find that participants who saw refugees traveling the Balkan route were no more and no less altruistic, regardless of the prime they received (Appendix Figure 4). Similarly, I obtained null findings when examining emotion-specific empathy (Olderbak et al. Reference Olderbak, Sassenrath, Keller and Wilhelm2014) as a potential moderator (Appendix Figures 1-3). Altogether, it seems that distinctiveness likely mattered in the context of the superordinate prime, while anti-Muslim sentiment, contact with refugees, and empathy did not.

I next turn to the ingroup prime. As discussed earlier, in the decades following Tito’s death, Serbian identity was mutually constructed and challenged by two opposing viewpoints: a dominant exclusionary narrative, which placed Serbian ethnic identity at its core, and the smaller, civic narrative (Russel-Omaljev Reference Russell-Omaljev2016). Although this stark polarization has softened, the symbolic importance of Kosovo—and with it an exclusionary vision of Serbian identity—remains highly salient (Wygnańska Reference Wygnańska2021). An ingroup identity prime should have led to less altruism (Hasbún López et al. Reference Hasbún López, Martinović, Bobowik, Chryssochoou, Cichocka, Ernst-Vintila, Franc, Fülöp, Ghilani, Kochar, Lamberty, Leone, Licata and Žeželj2019, Penner et al. Reference Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin and Schroeder2005). Why didn’t it?

The ingroup prime did not change how strongly Serbian the participants felt. Still, the way participants think about Serbian identity might matter. Specifically, these findings could be driven by higher altruism among those who favor a civic vision of identity. One of the ingroup treatment questions asked participants “What does it mean to be Serbian?” This question was open-ended and 53% of the ingroup prime participants gave answers that can be categorized as having either an ethnic or a civic understanding of identity. Comparing the levels of altruism between these two groups reveals no statistically significant differences (Appendix Table 22). While participants certainly have divergent conceptions of what it means to be Serbian, these differences are not reflected in their generosity. This is especially puzzling given the two polarizing Serbian identity narratives—one exclusionary (ethnic) and one not (civic).

An alternative explanation stems from the narrative surrounding the refugee crisis in Serbia, which framed Serbs as exceptionally helpful. This narrative might have led participants to exhibit more generosity because they felt good about being Serbian or because they were trying to follow the exemplar of Serbs as helpful toward refugees (Han et al. Reference Han, Workman, May, Scholtens, Dawson, Glenn and Meindl2022). I am unable to examine the effects of this rhetoric directly. Instead, I use the answers to “What does it mean to be Serbian?” to explore this possibility by categorizing them into positive, neutral, and negative attachments to being Serbian. I find no differences in generosity. Serbs who express a positive attachment to their identity are no more and no less altruistic to refugees compared to Serbs who do not (Appendix Table 36). Thus, neither potential explanation for the generosity associated with the ingroup prime bears out.

The literature identifies two other factors that may affect altruism toward refugees, writ large. The first is political ideology (Szabo et al. Reference Szabó, Vollhardt and Mészáros2020, Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021, but see Vollhard et al Reference Vollhardt, Nair and Tropp2016), for which I find no support in this sample. The second is threat perception (Vollhard et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021). I find that displaced Serbs who would feel unsafe if Serbia accepted more Syrian and Afghan refugees send significantly less to the refugee family in need. This is the case for both treatment groups (both factors in Appendix Tables 20-21).

The exploratory analyses find several potential explanations unhelpful. They also highlight two promising avenues for future research, which should be tackled systematically: closeness and perceived threat.

Conclusion

While the findings in this study are unexpected, they are not unique. A recent multi-country study of willingness to help a stranger during the COVID-19 pandemic shows that compared to participants in Germany, England, Ireland, and Sweden, participants in Serbia defy the typical expectation of ingroup favoritism based on an ethno-religious dimension (Carol et al. Reference Carol, David, Malešević and Uzelac2024). Religious Serbian participants were no less likely to help a fictitious Muslim neighbor than non-religious Serbs. In fact, Serbs were more willing to help an outgroup member than participants from any other country included in the study. Together with the findings in this study, this suggests that the typical relationship between ingroup favoritism and altruism may not hold in Serbia. As the case of Serbia is relatively underexplored (Carol et al. Reference Carol, David, Malešević and Uzelac2024), scholars may consider further work in order to gain a better understanding of how Serbian identity shapes altruism.

This study has several limitations. First, the lack of a control condition precludes a baseline to which altruism levels associated with the ingroup and superordinate primes could be compared. Second, due to the convenience sample of self-selected participants, I am unable to generalize these findings to the general population of displaced Serbs. Scholars who study shared hardship often contend with this limitation, as recruiting hard-to-reach populations for participation in research of a sensitive nature requires snowball or convenience sampling (Szabo et al. Reference Szabó, Vollhardt and Mészáros2020, Vollhardt et al. Reference Vollhardt, Cohrs, Szabó, Winiewski, Twali, Hadjiandreou and McNeill2021). Future research might therefore consider fielding similar surveys using larger, representative samples, while establishing clear baselines for comparisons.

Altogether, these results carry some implications for displacement interventions. Perhaps a successful intervention might rely on differences between refugees and hosts, in addition to naming commonalities. People may not necessarily reject a superordinate identity altogether; they might identify with a shared, superordinate identity while preferring an ingroup identity to be acknowledged as distinct (Hornsey and Hogg Reference Hornsey and Hogg2000). Such an intervention would be unlikely to dissuade people who already feel closeness toward the new refugees from altruism, but it might encourage those who feel distinct to be generous. Future work might also consider interventions that aim to reduce perceptions of threat or interventions that state the need for cross-group solidarity explicitly.

The need to understand why humanitarian appeals work and why they fail is acute. As the global number of displaced people rises, so does the chance that members of newfound host societies will have experienced conflict displacement themselves (Fisk Reference Fisk2018). Knowing how to narrate a potential commonality between a host society and a refugee population not only increases the chance that refugees will find a safe haven but likely also affects how displaced members of the host population process their own trauma. In the short term, understanding the interaction between superordinate identity and altruism can help policymakers reduce animosity; in the long term, it can aid interethnic integration and coalition-building efforts.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2025.4

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the author. The data are not publicly available due to the sensitive nature of this research. Participants agreed to the data being made available to approved researchers.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank W. Nicholson Price II, Mackenzie Israel-Trummel, Allyson Shortle, and the participants of the Comparative Politics Workshop at Michigan State University for invaluable feedback on the manuscript. I would like to thank Lucija Dežan for research assistance in coding open-ended responses.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the University of Oklahoma College of Arts & Sciences Funding Assistance Program, which played no role in the design, execution, analysis and interpretation of data, or writing of the study.

Ethical standards

This project was approved by the University of Oklahoma IRB (IRB: 7020) and pre-registered at EGAP (ID: 20160809AB).

Footnotes

1 Following Chandra (Reference Chandra and Chandra2012), ethnic identity is “a category in which descent-based attributes, and only descent-based attributes, are necessary for membership” (11), while ethnicity consists of “any concept related to nominal attributes or categories (ethnic structure)” (12) as well as “any concept related to activated attributes or categories (ethnic practice)” (12). Nationality is the status of belonging to a particular nation, and altruism means sharing with others and helping others (Tomasello Reference Tomasello2009).

2 See Appendix Table 19. The Appendix is online.

3 Eligible participants then read an online consent form and indicated agreement to participate by choosing to continue to the rest of the survey.

4 I performed a power analysis using means and standard deviations from similar work conducted in the region (see Mironova and Whitt Reference Mironova and Whitt2014), and standard values for power (0.80) and type 1 error rate (α = .05). Based on these calculations, this sample size is adequately powered (see the Appendix).

5 I thank members of the Ninamedia team for their contributions in crafting the survey.

6 Table 2 in the Appendix presents basic population covariates, by prime assignment.

7 800 Serbian dinars is approximately $8.34, or 35% of the average daily wage in Serbia at the time of survey administration. This amount is substantial enough to elicit a meaningful decision from the participant. Typically, similar studies in Western societies use $20 in such games, while some studies in non-Western societies use higher amounts (adjusted for local economic standards) (Bracic Reference Bracic2020, Larney et al. Reference Larney, Rotella and Barclay2019).

8 The check assessed the comparative salience of various identities by asking participants to rate how strongly they felt like a (1) man/woman, (2) Serbian, (3) a parent (if applicable), (4) displaced, and (5) Christian (or other appropriate religious identity).

9 The results remain substantively unchanged when using tobit (Appendix Table 10).

10 The questions-as-treatments framework can be responsible for the null results of an experiment as different aspects of the treatment can drive effects in opposite directions, ultimately leading to null results. To explore this possibility, I disaggregate the bundled treatments and examine the effect of each question on altruism. None of the individual questions that make up the two treatments have a statistically significant effect on the dependent variable, which suggests that the nature of the questions-as-treatment framework is not responsible for the null result here (see Appendix Tables 23-54).

11 The dictator game findings replicate for participants who witnessed someone being hurt, but not for participants who had their homes destroyed or who witnessed shelling or grenades being thrown (for them, the superordinate prime did not have a statistically significant effect on altruism; see Appendix Tables 11-13).

References

Adida, CL, Lo, A, and Platas, MR. (2018) Perspective taking can promote short-term inclusionary behavior toward Syrian refugees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115 (38), 95219526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altındağ, O, and Kaushal, N. (2021) Do refugees impact voting behavior in the host country? Evidence from Syrian refugee inflows to Turkey. Public Choice 186 (1), 149178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balliet, D, Li, NP, Macfarlan, SJ, and Van Vugt, M. (2011) Sex differences in cooperation: a meta-analytic review of social dilemmas. Psychological Bulletin 137 (6), 881909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bansak, K, Hainmueller, J, and Hangartner, D. (2016) How economic, humanitarian, and religious concerns shape european attitudes toward asylum seekers. Science 354 (6309), 217222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, R. (2012) Crime victimization and political participation. American Political Science Review 106 (3), 570587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, M, Cassar, A, Chytilová, J, and Henrich, J. (2013) War’s enduring effects on the development of egalitarian motivations and in-group biases. Psychological Science 20 (10), 111.Google Scholar
Bellows, J, and Miguel, E. (2009) War and local collective action in Sierra Leone. Journal of Public Economics 93 (11-12), 11441157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billing, M, and Tajfel, H. (1973) Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology 3 (1), 2752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blattman, C. (2009) From violence to voting: war and political participation in Uganda. American Political Science Review 103 (2), 231247.Google Scholar
Bloom, PB-N, Arikan, G, and Courtemanche, M. (2015) Religious social identity, religious belief, and anti-immigration sentiment. American Political Science Review 109 (2), 203221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracic, A. (2020) Breaking the Exclusion Cycle: How to Promote Cooperation between Majority and Minority Ethnic Groups. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bracic, A. (2022) Ethnicity and social exclusion. Nationalities Papers 50(6), 10451056.Google Scholar
Bracic, A, Israel-Trummel, M, and Shortle, AF. (2023) Ethnocultural or generalized? Nationalism and support for punitive immigration policy. Politics, Groups, and Identities 11 (5), 979996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breidahl, KN, Holtug, N, and Kongshøj, K. (2018) Do shared values promote social cohesion? If so, Which? Evidence from Denmark. European Political Science Review 10 (1), 97118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, MB. (1991) The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 17 (5), 475482.Google Scholar
Carol, S, David, L, Malešević, S, and Uzelac, G. (2024) Pro-social attitudes towards ethno-religious out-groups during the COVID-19 pandemic: a survey experiment in five countries. International Sociology 39 (1), 113137.Google Scholar
Chandra, K. (2012) Introduction. In Chandra, K, (Ed.), Constructivist Theories of Ethnic Politics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Charnysh, V, Lucas, C, and Singh, P. (2015) The ties that bind: national identity salience and pro-social behavior toward the ethnic other. Comparative Political Studies 48 (3), 267300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, DD, Poertner, M, and Sambanis, N. (2019) Parochialism, social norms, and discrimination against immigrants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116 (33), 1627416279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chong, D, and Druckman, JN. (2007) Framing theory. Annual Review of Political Science 10,103126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chowdhury, SM, and Jeon, JY. (2014) Impure altruism or inequality aversion?: An experimental investigation based on income effects. Journal of Public Economics 118,143150.Google Scholar
Corstange, D, and York, EA. (2018) Sectarian framing in the Syrian civil war. American Journal of Political Science 62 (2), 441455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R, and Gneezy, U. (2009) Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature 47 (2), 448474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darling, RB. (2013) Disability and Identity: Negotiating Self in a Changing Society. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasgupta, N. (2004) Implicit ingroup favoritism, outgroup favoritism, and their behavioral manifestations. Social Justice Research 17 (2), 143169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, J. (2014) Cultures of Democracy in Serbia and Bulgaria: How Ideas Shape Publics. London: Ashgate Publishing.Google Scholar
Diamond, EP. (2020) The influence of identity salience on framing effectiveness: an experiment. Political Psychology 41 (6), 11331150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinas, E, Fouka, V, and Schläpfer, A. (2021) Family history and attitudes toward out-groups: evidence from the European Refugee Crisis. The Journal of Politics 83 (2), 647661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, JF, Gaertner, SL, Validzic, A, Matoka, K, Johnson, B, and Frazier, S. (1997) Extending the benefits of recategorization: evaluations, self-disclosure, and helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 33 (4), 401420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, JF, Gaertner, SL, Isen, AM, and Lowrance, R. (1995) Group representations and intergroup bias: positive affect, similarity, and group size. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 21 (8), 856865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J, and Gaertner, S. (2000) Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science 11 (4), 315319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dustmann, C, Fasani, F, Frattini, T, Minale, L, and Schönberg, U. (2016) On the economics and politics of refugee migration. Economic Policy 32 (91), 497550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esses, VM, Hamilton, LK, and Gaucher, D. (2017) The global refugee crisis: empirical evidence and policy implications for improving public attitudes and facilitating refugee resettlement. Social Issues and Policy Review 11 (1), 78123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisk, K. (2018) One-sided violence in refugee-hosting areas. Journal of Conflict Resolution 62 (3), 529556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francisco, R. (2004) After the Massacre: mobilization in the wake of harsh repression. Mobilization: An International Quarterly 9 (2), 107126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freund, AM, and Blanchard-Fields, F. (2014) Age-related differences in altruism across adulthood: making personal financial gain versus contributing to the public good. Developmental Psychology 50 (4), 11251136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frontex European Border and Coast Guard Agency. 2018. Risk Analysis for 2018. Warsaw: Frontex.Google Scholar
Gaertner, SL, Dovidio, JF, Nier, JA, Banker, BS, Ward, CM, Houlette, M, and Loux, S. (2000) The common ingroup identity model for reducing intergroup bias: progress and challenges. In Capozza, D and Brown, R, (eds), Social Identity Processes: Trends in Theory and Research. Los Angeles, CA: Sage, pp. 133148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, SL, Dovidio, JF, Anastasio, PA, Bachman, BA, and Rust, MC. (1993) The common ingroup identity model: recategorization and the reduction of intergroup bias. European Review of Social Psychology 4 (1), 126.Google Scholar
Gagnon, VP. (2004) The Myth of Ethnic War: Serbia and Croatia in the 1990s. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Galijaš, A. (2019) Permanently in transit. Middle Eastern migrants and refugees in Serbia. Comparative Southeast European Studies 67 (1), 75109.Google Scholar
Georgiou, M, and Zaborowski, R. (2017) Media Coverage of the “Refugee Crisis”: A Cross-European Perspective. London, UK: Council of Europe Report DG1(2017)03.Google Scholar
Getmansky, A, Sınmazdemir, T, and Zeitzoff, T. (2018) Refugees, Xenophobia, and domestic conflict: Evidence from a survey experiment in Turkey. Journal of Peace Research 55 (4), 491507.Google Scholar
Gilligan, MJ, Pasquale, BJ, and Samii, C. (2014) Civil war and social cohesion: lab-in-the-field evidence from Nepal. American Journal of Political Science 58 (3), 604619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, SW, and Alarian, HM. (2021) National belonging and public support for multiculturalism. Journal of Race, Ethnicity, and Politics 6 (2), 305333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorodzeisky, A, and Semyonov, M. (2020) Perceptions and misperceptions: actual size, perceived size and opposition to immigration in European Societies. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 46 (3), 612630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossman, G, Manekin, D, and Miodownik, D. (2015) The political legacies of combat: attitudes toward war and peace among Israeli ex-combatants. International Organization 69 (4), 9811009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hager, A, Krakowski, K, and Schaub, M. (2019) Ethnic riots and prosocial behavior: evidence from Kyrgyzstan. American Political Science Review 113 (4), 10291044.Google Scholar
Hall, J. (2016) Are migrants more extreme than locals after war? Evidence from a simultaneous survey of migrants in Sweden and Locals in Bosnia. Journal of Conflict Resolution 60 (1), 89117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, J. (2018) Integration of refugees and support for the ethos of conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution 62 (9), 20402067.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, H, Workman, CI, May, J, Scholtens, P, Dawson, KJ, Glenn, AL, and Meindl, P. (2022) Which moral exemplars inspire prosociality?Philosophical Psychology 35 (7), 943970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasbún López, P, Martinović, B, Bobowik, M, Chryssochoou, X, Cichocka, A, Ernst-Vintila, A, Franc, R, Fülöp, É, Ghilani, D, Kochar, A, Lamberty, P, Leone, G, Licata, L, and Žeželj, I. (2019) “Support for collective action against refugees: the role of National, European, and global identifications, and autochthony beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology 49 (7), 14391455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewstone, M, Rubin, M, and Willis, H. (2002) Intergroup Bias. Annual Review of Psychology 53,575604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, El, McCabe, K, Shachat, K, and Smith, V. (1994) Preferences, property rights, and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior 7,346380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Homola, J, and Tavits, M. (2018) Contact reduces immigration-related fears for leftist but not for rightist voters. Comparative Political Studies 51 (13), 17891820.Google Scholar
Hornsey, MJ, and Hogg, MA. (2000) Subgroup relations: a comparison of mutual intergroup differentiation and common ingroup identity models of prejudice reduction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26 (2), 242256.Google Scholar
Jović, D. (2001) The disintegration of Yugoslavia: a critical review of explanatory Approaches. European Journal of Social Theory 4 (1), 101120.Google Scholar
Kalemaj, I and Lleshi, S. (2020) Sources and continuities of ethno-religious nationalism in the Western Balkans.” In Meka, E and Bianchini, S, (eds), The Challenges of Democratization and Reconciliation in the Post-Yugoslav Space. Baden-Baden: Nomos.Google Scholar
KIRS (Commissariat for Refugees and Migration, Republika Srbija). (2016) “About Us.” Accessed on September 7, 2018. http://www.kirs.gov.rs/articles/aboutus.php?lang=ENG.Google Scholar
Klanjšek, R, and Flere, S. (2011) Exit Yugoslavia: longing for mononational states or entrepreneurial manipulation?Nationalities Papers 39 (5), 791810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klar, S. (2013) The influence of competing identity primes on political preferences. The Journal of Politics 75 (4),11081124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krupnikov, Y, Nam, HH, and Style, H. (2021) Convenience Samples in Political Science Experiments. In James, ND and Donald, PG (eds), Advances in Experimental Political Science, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 165183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larney, A, Rotella, A, and Barclay, P. (2019) Stake size effects in ultimatum game and dictator game offers: a meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 151, 6172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarev, E, and Sharma, K. (2017) Brother or burden: an experiment on reducing prejudice toward Syrian refugees in Turkey. Political Science Research and Methods 5 (2), 201219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, M, and Thompson, K. (2004) Identity, place, and bystander intervention: social categories and helping after natural disasters. The Journal of Social Psychology 144 (3), 229245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, M, Prosser, A, Evans, D, and Reicher, S. (2005) Identity and emergency intervention: how social group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping Behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31 (4), 443453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linos, K, Jakli, L, and Carlson, M. (2021) Fundraising for stigmatized groups: a text message donation experiment. American Political Science Review 115 (1), 1430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyall, J. (2009) Does indiscriminate violence incite insurgent attacks? Evidence from Chechnya. Journal of Conflict Resolution 53 (3), 331362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malešević, S. (2002) Ideology, Legitimacy and the New State: Yugoslavia, Serbia and Croatia. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
McClendon, G, and Riedl, RB. (2015) Religion as a stimulant of political participation: experimental evidence from Nairobi, Kenya. The Journal of Politics 77 (4), 10451057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mironova, V, and Whitt, S. (2014) Ethnicity and altruism after violence: the contact hypothesis in Kosovo. Journal of Experimental Political Science 1 (2), 170180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, JM, Nyhan, B, and Torres, M. (2018) How conditioning on post-treatment variables can ruin your experiment and what to do about it. American Journal of Political Science 62 (3), 760775.Google Scholar
Montoya, MR, and Pittinsky, TL. (2016) Bounded rationality’s account for the influence of group identification on ingroup favoritism: a field investigation using Jewish and Arab populations in Israel. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 38 (3), 127136.Google Scholar
Nickerson, AM, and Louis, WR. (2008) Nationality versus humanity? Personality, identity, and norms in relation to attitudes toward asylum seekers. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 38 (3), 796817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noor, M, Shnabel, N, Halabi, S, and Nadler, A. (2012) When suffering begets suffering: the psychology of competitive victimhood between adversarial groups in violent conflicts. Personality and Social Psychology Review 16 (4), 351374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olderbak, S, Sassenrath, C, Keller, J, and Wilhelm, O. (2014) An Emotion-differentiated perspective on empathy with the emotion specific empathy questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology 5, 653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paz, A, and Kook, R. (2021) ‘It reminds me that I still exist’. Critical thoughts on intersectionality, refugee muslim women in Berlin and the meanings of the Hijab. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 47 (13), 29792996.Google Scholar
Penner, LA, Dovidio, JF, Piliavin, JA, and Schroeder, DA. (2005) Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology 56, 365392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrović Trifunović, T, and Poleti Ćosić, D. (2018) Discourses of hospitality, politics of hostility: instrumentalization of the “Refugee Crisis” in the Serbian press. In Pudar Draško, G, Pavlović, A, and Meka, E, (eds), Politics of Enmity, Institute for Philosophy/Social Theory, University of Belgrade. pp. 211228.Google Scholar
Phillips, ST, and Ziller, RC. (1997) Toward a theory and measure of the nature of nonprejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 (2), 420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rapp, CH. (2022) The bond that binds or drives us apart? An empirical test of the national identity argument in three countries. European Political Science Review 14 (3), 296314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riek, BM, Mania, EW, Gaertner, SL, McDonald, SA, and Lamoreaux, MJ. (2010) Does a common ingroup identity reduce intergroup threat?Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 13 (4), 403423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roccas, S, Sagiv, L, Schwartz, S, Halevy, N, and Eidelson, R. (2008) Toward a unifying model of identification with groups: integrating theoretical perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review 12 (3), 280306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothberg, M. (2011) From Gaza to Warsaw: mapping multidirectional memory. Criticism 53 (4), 523548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell-Omaljev, A. (2016) Divided We Stand: Discourses on identity in ‘First’ and ‘Other’ Serbia. Social Construction of the Self and the Other in a Divided Serbia. Stuttgart: Ibidem-Verlag.Google Scholar
RZS (Statistical Office of the Republic of Serbia). (2016) “Stanovništvo.” RZS, Accessed on September 7, 2018. http://www.stat.gov.rs/WebSite/Default.aspx.Google Scholar
Sardelić, J. (2017) “From Temporary Protection to Transit Migration: Responses to Refugee Crises Along the Western Balkan Route.” Robert Schuman Centre for Advanced Studies Research Paper No. RSCAS 35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schenk, C. (2021) The migrant other: exclusion without nationalism? Nationalities Papers 49 (3), 397408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, D. (2011) Americanism in the Twenty-First Century: Public Opinion in the Age of Immigration. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schulz, DL. (2001) Going South: Jewish Women in the Civil Rights Movement. New York, NY: NYU Press.Google Scholar
Sekulić, D, Massey, G, and Hodson, R. (2006) Ethnic intolerance and ethnic conflict in the dissolution of Yugoslavia. Ethnic and Racial Studies 29 (5), 797827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaghaghi, A, Bhopal, RS, and Sheikh, A. (2011) Approaches to recruiting ‘hard-to-reach’ populations into research: a review of the literature. Health Promotion Perspectives 1 (2), 8694.Google ScholarPubMed
Staub, E. (2003) The Psychology of Good and Evil: Why Children, Adults, and Groups Help and Harm Others. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Subotić, J. (2011) Europe is a state of mind: identity and Europeanization in the Balkans. International Studies Quarterly 55 (2), 309330.Google Scholar
Subotić, J. (2015) Out of Eastern Europe: legacies of violence and the challenge of multiple transitions”. East European Politics and Societies and Cultures 29 (2), 409419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szabó, ZP, Vollhardt, JR, and Mészáros, NZ. (2020) Through the lens of history: the effects of beliefs about historical victimization on responses to refugees. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 74, 94114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Šelo Šabić, S. (2017) The impact of the refugee crisis in the Balkans: a drift towards security. Journal of Regional Security 12 (1), 5174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1969) Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Biosocial Science 1 (1), 173191.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H, and Turner, JC. (1979) An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations 33 (47), 74.Google Scholar
Todorova, M. (1997) Imagining the Balkans. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009) Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Transue, JE. (2007) Identity salience, identity acceptance, and racial policy attitudes: American national identity as a uniting force. American Journal of Political Science 51 (1), 7891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, JC, and Onorato, RS. (1999) Social identity, personality, and the self-concept: a self-categorizing perspective. In Tom, RT, Roderick, MK, and Oliver, PJ. (eds), The Psychology of the Social Self. Applied Social Research, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Turner, JC, Hogg, MA, Oakes, PJ, Reicher, S, and Wetherell, MS. (1987) Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology 399417.Google Scholar
Tyrberg, M. (2024) The impact of discrimination and support on immigrant trust and belonging. European Political Science Review 16 (1), 1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
UNHCR. (2016) “Europe Refugees and Migrants Emergency Response - Daily Estimated Arrivals per Country - Flows through Western Balkans Route 1 October 2015-25 July 2016.” UNHCR, Accessed on March 21, 2018. data. unhcr.org/mediterranean.Google Scholar
UNHCR. (2017) The UN Refugee Agency, Ministry of Interior, and Serbian Commissariat for Refugees and Migration. “SERBIA MONTHLY SNAPSHOT – May 2017.” UNHCR, Accessed on March 21, 2018. data.unhcr.org/mediterranean.Google Scholar
Vasovic, A. (2016) Migrants Test ‘Closed’ Balkan Borders to Chase European Dream.” Reuters, May 20. Accessed on August 3, 2016. http://www.reuters.com/article/us-europe-migrants-serbia-idUSKCN0YB1BX.Google Scholar
Vezzali, L, Cadamuro, A, Versari, A, Giovannini, D, and Trifiletti, E. (2015) Feeling like a group after a natural disaster: common ingroup identity and relations with outgroup victims among majority and minority young children. British Journal of Social Psychology 54 (3), 519538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollhardt, JR. (2009) Altruism born of suffering and prosocial behavior following adverse life events: a review and conceptualization. Social Justice Research 22 (1), 5397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollhardt, JR. (2013) ‘Crime against humanity’ or ‘crime against jews’? Acknowledgment in construals of the holocaust and its importance for intergroup relations. Journal of Social Issues 69 (1), 144161.Google Scholar
Vollhardt, JR, and Bilali, R. (2015) The role of inclusive and exclusive victim consciousness in predicting intergroup attitudes: findings From Rwanda, Burundi, and DRC. Political Psychology 36 (5), 489506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollhardt, JR, Nair, R, and Tropp, LR. (2016) Inclusive victim consciousness predicts minority group members’ support for refugees and immigrants. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 46 (6), 354368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vollhardt, JR, Cohrs, JC, Szabó, ZP, Winiewski, M, Twali, MS, Hadjiandreou, E, and McNeill, A. (2021) The role of comparative victim beliefs in predicting support for hostile versus prosocial intergroup outcomes. European Journal of Social Psychology 51 (3), 505524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voors, MJ, Nillesen, EEM, Verwimp, P, Bulte, EH, Lensink, R, and Van Soest, DP. (2012) Violent conflict and behavior: a field experiment in Burundi. The American Economic Review 102 (2), 941964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wayne, C, and Zhukov, YM. (2022) Never again: the holocaust and political legacies of genocide. World Politics 74 (3), 367404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitt, S, and Wilson, RK. (2007) The dictator game, fairness and ethnicity in postwar Bosnia. American Journal of Political Science 51 (3), 655668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williamson, S, Adida, CL, Lo, A, Platas, MR, Prather, L, and Werfel, SH. (2021) Family matters: how immigrant histories can promote inclusion. American Political Science Review 115 (2), 686693.Google Scholar
Wood, EJ. (2006) Variation in sexual violence during war. Politics and Society 34 (3), 307342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, SC, Aron, A, McLaughlin-Volpe, T, and Ropp, SA. (1997) The extended contact effect: knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73 (1), 7390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wygnańska, J. (2021) Between political myths, dormant resentments, and redefinition of the recent history: a case study of Serbian national identity. Qualitative Sociology Review 17 (2), 3868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaki, J, and Ochsner, KN. (2012) The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience 15 (5), 675680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zorlu, A. (2017) Attitudes toward asylum seekers in small local communities. International Migration 55 (6), 1436.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Salience of displaced and ethnic identities, between groups (t-test)

Figure 1

Table 2. Salience of displaced and ethnic identities, within groups (t-test)

Figure 2

Figure 1. This figure presents the average amount of dinars sent to the Syrian refugee family in need along with 95% confidence intervals, by prime. Participants had an endowment of 800 dinars.

Figure 3

Table 3. OLS regression results (dependent variable: amount sent in the dictator game)

Figure 4

Figure 2. This figure presents the average amount of dinars sent to the Syrian refugee family in need along with 95% confidence intervals, by prime and by personal experience of violence. Participants had an endowment of 800 dinars.

Supplementary material: File

Bracic supplementary material

Bracic supplementary material
Download Bracic supplementary material(File)
File 161.8 KB