Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T06:13:01.413Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Style and stability in mother conversational behaviour: a study of individual differences*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Lynda Olsen-Fulero
Affiliation:
Antioch College

Abstract

This paper explores differences in conversational style among mothers. Although previous research suggests that many aspects of mother speech to the child covary in a way which serves the underlying intention of the mother, it was unclear whether such clusters of conversational behaviour would prove useful in characterizing stylistic differences among mothers across time. An analysis of the functionally-coded speech of 11 mothers showed statistically significant variability among mothers, especially in those behaviours most closely associated with intention. Further, mothers showed stability in these behaviour patterns across two sessions. A typology of mother style, based on the intentions of mothers to direct or converse with their children, is suggested and illustrated by individual cases.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

The portion of this paper concerning ratings of mother warmth is derived from an Honours thesis in progress by Ms Jill Conforti under the supervision of the author, and from a paper in progress by Conforti & Olsen-Fulero. I should like to acknowledge the assistance of Dr Solomon Olsen-Fulero and Dr Lawrence Kurdek. The author was formerly known as Lynda McDonald. Address for correspondence: Psychology, Antioch College, Yellow Springs, Ohio 45387.

References

REFERENCES

Baumrind, D. (1975). Early socialization and the discipline controversy. University Programs Modular Studies. Morristown, N.J.: General Learning Press.Google Scholar
Becker, W. O. (1964). Consequences of different kinds of parental discipline. In Hoffman, M. L. & Hoffman, L. W. (eds) Review of child development research, Vol. 1. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1979). Changes in the explicitness of mothers' directives as children age. JChLang 6. 443–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Bellinger, D. (1980). Consistency in the pattern of change in mothers' speech: some discriminant analyses. JChLang 7. 469–88.Google ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. CogPsych 8. 521–52.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke-Stewart, A. (1973). Interactions between mothers and their young children: characteristics and consequences. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38. Nos. 6–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Cross, T. (1978). Mothers' speech and its association with rate of linguistic development in young children. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dore, J. (1973). A developmental theory of speech act production. TN Y ASci 35. 623–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Dore, J. (1978). Variation in preschool children's conversational performances. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Children's language, Vol. 1. New York: Gardner.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1976). Is Sybil there? The structures of some American English directives. LangSoc 5. 2566.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1977). Wait for me, roller skate! In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Folger, J. & Chapman, R. (1978). A pragmatic analysis of spontaneous imitations. JChLang 5. 2538.Google Scholar
Furrow, D., Nelson, K. & Benedict, H. (1979). Mothers' speech to children and syntactic development: some simple relationships. JChLang 6. 423–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Garvey, C. (1975). Requests and responses in children's speech. JChLang 2. 4160.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaye, K. & Charney, R. (1981). Conversational asymmetry between mothers and children. JChLang 8. 3550.Google ScholarPubMed
Lieven, E. V. M. (1978). Conversations between mothers and young children: individual differences and their possible implication for the study of language learning. In Waterson, N. & Snow, C. (eds), The development of communication. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Martin, B. (1975). Parent-child relations. In Horowitz, F. (ed.), Review of child development research 4. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McDonald, L. & Pien, D. (1982). Mother conversational behaviour as a function of interactional intent. JChLang 9. 337–58.Google ScholarPubMed
Mitchell-Kernan, C. & Kernan, K. (1977). Pragmatics of directive choice among children. In Ervin-Tripp, S. & Mitchell-Kernan, C. (eds), Child discourse. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mosteller, F. & Rourke, R. (1973). Sturdy statistics: nonparametrics and order statistics. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monogr. Soc. Res. Ch. Devel. 38. Nos. 1–2.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: implications for development and language. DevPsych 17. 170–87.Google Scholar
Newport, E. (1976). Motherese: the speech of mothers to young children. In Castellan, N., Pisoni, D. & Potts, G. (eds), Cognitive theory 2. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Phillips, J. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. ChDev 44. 182–5.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. & Jefferson, G. (1978). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn taking for conversation. In Schenkein, J. (ed.), Studies in the organization of conversational interaction. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech arts: an essay in the philosophy of language. New York: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1975). Indirect speech acts. In Cole, P. & Morgan, J. (eds), Syntax and semantics 3. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J. & Coulthard, M. (1975). Towards an analysis of discourse: the English used by teachers and pupils. Oxford: O.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1972). Mothers' speech to children learning language. ChDev 43. 549–65.Google Scholar
Waters, E. (1978). The reliability and stability of individual differences in infant-mother attachment. ChDev 49. 483–94.Google Scholar
Yarrow, M. & Waxier, C. (1979). Observing interaction: a confrontation with methodology. In Cairns, R. (ed.), The analysis of social interactions. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar