Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:00:28.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Role of adult input in young children's category evolution: II an experimental study*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Rae L. Banigan
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Carolyn B. Mervis*
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Amherst
*
Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA.

Abstract

Young children's initial categories often are not identical to the adult category labelled by the same word. Eventually, children's categories must evolve to correspond to the adult standard. The purpose of this study was to consider the relative effectiveness of four input strategies in inducing the child to learn the adult-appropriate label and begin to form a new category. Fifty-six children aged 2;0 were taught new labels for objects that they included in categories labelled by different names. Comprehension and production post-tests were then administered. As expected, the most effective strategy involved labelling an object and providing both a physical demonstration and a verbal description of important attributes that made the object a member of the adult-appropriate category. The label plus physical demonstration strategy was next most effective. Neither the label plus verbal description strategy nor the label only strategy was effective for children of this age. Results also indicated that these 24 month olds did not yet honour the convention of mutual exclusivity of basic level categories.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This paper is based on a doctoral dissertation submitted by the first author to the University of Massachusetts, Amherst. Portions of this paper were presented at the November 1986 convention of the American Speech—Language—Hearing Association, Detroit, Michigan. We would like to thank the parents and children who participated in this study. Albyn Davis, Cindy Mervis, and Harry Seymour provided valuable comments on previous drafts of this paper. This research was supported by the National Science Foundation, grant no. BNS 84–19036.

References

REFERENCES

Banigan, R. L. (1987). The effect of input on young children's acquisition of adult-basic categories. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Banigan, R. L. & Seymour, H. N. (1985). The effect of cognitive variables on lexical acquisition. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American Speech-Language—Hearing Association, Washington D.C.Google Scholar
Barrett, M. D. (1978). Lexical development and overextension in child language. Journal of Child Language 14. 205–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, M. D. (1982). Distinguishing between prototypes: the early acquisition of the meaning of object names. In Kuczaj, S. A. II (ed.), Language development. Vol. 1. Syntax and semantics. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., McNew, S., O'Connell, B., Reznick, S., Shore, C., Snyder, L. & Volterra, V. (1984). Early language inventory. Unpublished manuscript, Dept. of Psychology, University of California, San Diego.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1973). What's in a word? On the Child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1987). How children constrain the possible meanings of words. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Merriman, W. E. (1986). Some reasons for the occurrence and eventual correction of children's naming errors. Child Development 57. 942–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriman, W. E. (1987). Lexical contrast in toddlers: a reanalysis of the diary evidence. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore MD.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (1982). Mother—child interaction and early lexical development. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Minneapolis MN.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (1984). Early lexical development: the contributions of mother and child. In Sophian, C. (ed.), Origins of cognitive skills. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (1986). Operating principles and personal theories: their roles in early lexical development. Paper presented at a meeting of the New England Child Language Association, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. (1987). Child-basic object categories and early lexical development. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorisation. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Greco, C. (1984). Parts and early conceptual development: comment on Tversky and Hemenway. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113. 194–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Long, L. M. (1987). Words refer to whole objects: young children's interpretation of the referent of a novel word. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore MD.Google Scholar
Mervis, C. B. & Mervis, C. A. (1988). Role of adult input in young children's category evolution: I. An observational study. Journal of Child Language 15. 257–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. (1987). From direct perception to conceptual structure. In Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: ecological and intellectual factors in categorization. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M. & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8. 382439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B. & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113. 169–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed