Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T15:46:58.559Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Lexical tone variation and spoken word recognition in preschool children: effects of perceptual salience*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2016

LEHER SINGH*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore
ALOYSIA TAN
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore
THILANGA D. WEWALAARACHCHI
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, National University of Singapore
*
Address for correspondence: Leher Singh, Dept. of Psychology, National University of Singapore, 9 Arts Link, Singapore117570. e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Children undergo gradual progression in their ability to differentiate correct and incorrect pronunciations of words, a process that is crucial to establishing a native vocabulary. For the most part, the development of mature phonological representations has been researched by investigating children's sensitivity to consonant and vowel variation, with a much lesser focus on lexical tones. The current study investigates sensitivity to lexical tones in word recognition with specific attention to role of perceptual salience. Chinese-speaking preschoolers were presented with familiar words that were correctly pronounced, substituted for a subtle tone variant (Tones 2 and 3), or substituted for a salient tone variant (Tones 1 and 4). Results demonstrated that subtle tone variants were mistakenly perceived as correct pronunciations and only salient tone variants were recognized as mispronunciations. Findings suggest that tone integration follows a more complex developmental course that previously concluded.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We are grateful to an Academic Research Fund grant (MOE Tier 1 FY2013-FRC2-009) to support the research reported in this paper.

References

REFERENCES

Bailey, T. M. & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development 17(2), 1265–82.Google Scholar
Blicher, D. L., Diehl, R. L. & Cohen, L. B. (1990). Effects of syllable duration on the perception of the Mandarin Tone 2/Tone 3 distinction: evidence of auditory enhancement. Journal of Phonetics 18(1), 3749.Google Scholar
Burnham, D., Ciocca, V. & Stokes, S. (2001). Auditory-visual perception of lexical tone. Paper presented at EUROSPEECH 2001.Google Scholar
Chen, M. Y. (2000). Tone sandhi: patterns across Chinese dialects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Chen, T. H. & Massaro, D. W. (2008). Seeing pitch: visual information for lexical tones of Mandarin-Chinese. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123(4), 2356–66.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Byers-Heinlein, K. & Werker, J. F. (2011). Bilingual beginnings as a lens for theory development: PRIMIR in focus. Journal of Phonetics 39(4), 492504.Google Scholar
Curtin, S., Fennell, C. & Escudero, P. (2009). Weighting of vowel cues explains patterns of word–object associative learning. Developmental Science 12(5), 725–31.Google Scholar
Edmondson, J. A. & Esling, J. H. (2006). The valves of the throat and their functioning in tone, vocal register and stress: laryngoscopic case studies. Phonology 23(2), 157–91.Google Scholar
Eliot, C. E. (1991). The relationship between the perception and production of Mandarin tones: an exploratory study. University of Hawai'i Working Papers in ESL 10(2), 177204.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., McRoberts, G. W. & Swingley, D. (2001). Infants’ developing competence in recognizing and understanding words in fluent speech. In Weissenborn, J. & Hoehle, B. (eds), Approaches to bootstrapping: phonological, syntactic and neurophysiological aspects of early language acquisition, 97124. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fromkin, V. A. (ed.) (1978). Tone: a linguistic survey. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hao, Y. C. (2012). Second language acquisition of Mandarin tones by tonal and non-tonal language speakers. Journal of Phonetics 40(2), 269–79.Google Scholar
Havy, M., Bertoncini, J. & Nazzi, T. (2011). Word learning and phonetic processing in preschool age children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 108, 2543.Google Scholar
Hua, Z. & Dodd, B. (2000). The phonological acquisition of Putonghua (modern standard Chinese). Journal of child language 27(1), 342.Google Scholar
Jerger, S., Damian, M., Spence, M., Tye-Murray, N. & Abdi, H. (2009). Developmental shifts in children's sensitivity to visual speech: a new multimodal picture word task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 102, 4059.Google Scholar
Kiriloff, C. (1969). On the auditory perception of tones in Mandarin. Phonetica 20(2–4), 63–7.Google Scholar
Leather, J. (1983). Speaker normalization in perception of lexical tone. Journal of Phonetics 11(4), 373–82.Google Scholar
Li, C. N. & Thompson, S. A. (1977). The acquisition of tone in Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 4(2), 185–99.Google Scholar
Liu, S. & Samuel, A. G. (2004). Perception of Mandarin lexical tones when F0 information is neutralized. Language and Speech 47(2), 109–38.Google Scholar
Mani, N., Coleman, J. & Plunkett, K. (2008). Phonological specificity of vowel contrasts at 18-months. Language and Speech 51(1/2), 321.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2007). Phonological specificity of vowels and consonants in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language 57(2), 252–72.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2008). Fourteen-month-olds pay attention to vowels in novel words. Developmental Science 11(1), 53–9.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2010). In the infant's mind's ear: evidence for implicit naming in 18-month-olds. Psychological Science 21(7), 908–13.Google Scholar
Mani, N. & Plunkett, K. (2011). Phonological priming and cohort effects in toddlers. Cognition 121(2), 196206.Google Scholar
Mattock, K. & Burnham, D. (2006). Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy 10(3), 241–65.Google Scholar
Meints, K., Plunkett, K. & Harris, P. L. (1999). When does an ostrich become a bird? The role of typicality in early word comprehension. Developmental Psychology 35(4), 1072–8.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T. (2005). Use of phonetic specificity during the acquisition of new words: differences between consonants and vowels. Cognition 98(1), 1330.Google Scholar
Purves, D., Augustine, G. & Fitzpatrick, D. (2001). Types of eye movements and their functions. In Neuroscience, 2nd ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
Quam, C. & Swingley, D. (2012). Development in children's interpretation of pitch cues to emotions. Child Development 83, 246–50.Google Scholar
Schafer, G. & Plunkett, K. (1998). Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions. Child Development 69(2), 309–20.Google Scholar
Singh, L. & Chee, M. (2016). Rise and fall: effects of tone and intonation on spoken word recognition in early childhood. Journal of Phonetics 55, 109–18.Google Scholar
Singh, L. & Foong, J. (2012). Influences of lexical tone and pitch on word recognition in bilingual infants. Cognition 124(2), 128–42.Google Scholar
Singh, L., Goh, H. H. & Wewalaarachchi, T. D. (2015). Spoken word recognition in early childhood: comparative effects of vowel, consonant and lexical tone variation. Cognition 142, 111.Google Scholar
Singh, L., Hui, T. J., Chan, C. & Golinkoff, R. M. (2014). Influences of vowel and tone variation on emergent word knowledge: a cross-linguistic investigation. Developmental Science 17(1), 94109.Google Scholar
So, L. K. & Dodd, B. J. (1995). The acquisition of phonology by Cantonese-speaking children. Journal of Child Language 22(3), 473–95.Google Scholar
Stager, C. L. & Werker, J. F. (1997). Infants listen for more phonetic detail in speech perception than in word-learning tasks. Nature 388(6640), 381–2.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2000). Spoken word recognition and lexical representation in very young children. Cognition 76(2), 147–66.Google Scholar
Swingley, D. & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science 13(5), 480–4.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. M. (2008). The effect of acoustically similarity of lexical-tone perception of one-year-old Mandarin-learning infants. Chinese Journal of Psychology 50(2), 111–24.Google Scholar
Van der Feest, S. V. (2007). Building a phonological lexicon: the acquisition of the Dutch voicing contrast in perception and production. Prince Productions. Online <http://suzanne.vanderfeest.nl/OutlineDiss.pdf>..>Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M. & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy 3(1), 130.Google Scholar
Wewalaarachchi, D. & Singh, L. (2014). Influences of tone sandhi on word recognition in preschool children. Poster presented at the Fifteenth Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association.Google Scholar
Whalen, D. H. & Xu, Y. (1992). Information for Mandarin tones in the amplitude contour and in brief segments. Phonetica 49(1), 2547.Google Scholar
White, K. S. & Morgan, J. L. (2008). Sub-segmental detail in early lexical representations. Journal of Memory and Language 59(1), 114–32.Google Scholar
Wong, P. (2012). Acoustic characteristics of three-year-olds’ correct and incorrect monosyllabic Mandarin lexical tone productions. Journal of Phonetics 40(1), 141–51.Google Scholar
Wong, P. (2013). Perceptual evidence for protracted development in monosyllabic Mandarin lexical tone production in preschool children in Taiwan. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 133(1), 434–43.Google Scholar
Wong, P. C. & Diehl, R. L. (2003). Perceptual normalization for inter- and intratalker variation in Cantonese level tones. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46(2), 413–21.Google Scholar
Wong, P., Schwartz, R. G. & Jenkins, J. J. (2005). Perception and production of lexical tones by 3-year-old, Mandarin-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48(5), 1065–79.Google Scholar
Yeung, H. H., Chen, K. H. & Werker, J. F. (2013). When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language 68(2), 123–39.Google Scholar
Yip, M. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar