Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T18:39:59.619Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Finding meaning in a noisy world: exploring the effects of referential ambiguity and competition on 2·5-year-olds’ cross-situational word learning*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2016

JOHN P. BUNCE
Affiliation:
University of California Merced
ROSE M. SCOTT*
Affiliation:
University of California Merced
*
Address for correspondence: Rose M. Scott, School of Social Sciences, Humanities, and Arts, University of California Merced, 5200 North Lake Road, Merced, CA 95343. tel: (209) 228-4362; e-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

While recent studies suggest children can use cross-situational information to learn words, these studies involved minimal referential ambiguity, and the cross-situational evidence overwhelmingly favored a single referent for each word. Here we asked whether 2·5-year-olds could identify a noun's referent when the scene and cross-situational evidence were more ambiguous. Children saw four trials in which a novel word occurred with four novel objects; only one object consistently co-occurred with the word across trials. The frequency of distracter objects varied across conditions. When all distracter referents occurred only once (no-competition), children successfully identified the noun's referent. When a high-probability competitor referent occurred on three trials, children identified the target referent if the competitor was absent on the third trial (short-competition) but not if it was present until the fourth trial (long-competition). This suggests that although 2·5-year-olds’ cross-situational learning scales up to more ambiguous scenes, it is disrupted by high-probability competitor referents.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by grants from the University of California Merced Graduate Research Council to John Bunce and Rose M. Scott, and by a grant from the Hellman Fellows Fund to Rose M. Scott. We thank Anne Warlaumont for helpful feedback on the manuscript, the staff of the UC Merced Center for Early Cognition and Language for their assistance with data collection, and the parents, children, and students who participated in this research.

References

REFERENCES

Akhtar, N. & Montague, L. (1999). Early lexical acquisition: the role of cross-situational learning. First Language 19, 347–58.Google Scholar
Ankowski, A. A., Vlach, H. A. & Sandhofer, C. M. (2013). Comparison versus contrast: task specifics affect category acquisition. Infant and Child Development 22, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arunachalam, S. & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Meaning from syntax: evidence from 2-year-olds. Cognition 114, 442–6.Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N., Woodward, J. Z., LaMendola, N. P. & Bever, T. G. (1996). Models of word segmentation in fluent maternal speech to infants. In Morgan, J. L. & Demuth, K. (eds), Signal to syntax, 117–34. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Infants’ ability to consult the speaker for clues to word reference. Journal of Child Language 20, 395418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernal, S., Lidz, J., Millotte, S. & Christophe, A. (2007). Syntax constrains the acquisition of verb meaning. Language Learning and Development 3, 325–41.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1978). The child as word learner. In Halle, M., Bresnan, J. & Miller, G. A. (eds), Linguistic theory and psychological reality, 264–93. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cauvet, E., Limissuri, R., Millotte, S., Skoruppa, K., Cabrol, D. & Christophe, A. (2014). Function words constrain on-line recognition of verbs and nouns in French 18-month-olds. Language Learning and Development 10, 118.Google Scholar
Chen, C. & Yu, C. (2015). The effects of learning and retrieval contexts on cross-situational word learning. In Proceedings of the Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-Epirob). Google Scholar
Childers, J. B. (2011). Attention to multiple events helps two-and-a-half-year-olds extend new verbs. First Language 31, 322.Google Scholar
Childers, J. B., Hirshkowitz, A. & Benavides, K. (2014). Attention to explicit and implicit contrast in verb learning. Journal of Cognition and Development 15, 213–37.Google Scholar
Childers, J. B. & Paik, J. H. (2009). Korean- and English-speaking children use cross-situational information to learn novel predicate terms. Journal of Child Language 36, 201–24.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 28, 125–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dautriche, I. & Chemla, E. (2014). Cross-situational word learning in the right situations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40, 892903.Google Scholar
Fazly, A., Alishahi, A. & Stevenson, S. (2010). A probabilistic computational model of cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science 34, 1017–63.Google Scholar
Fennell, C. T. & Waxman, S. R. (2010). What paradox? Referential cues allow for infant use of phonetic detail in word learning. Child Development 81, 1376–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S. & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories. Baltimore, MD: Paul Brookes Publishing.Google Scholar
Fisher, C., Hall, D. G., Rakowitz, S. & Gleitman, L. R. (1994). When it is better to receive than to give: syntactic and conceptual constraints on vocabulary growth. Lingua 92, 333–75.Google Scholar
Fisher, C., Klingler, S. L. & Song, H. J. (2006). What does syntax say about space? 2-year-olds use sentence structure to learn new prepositions. Cognition 101, B1929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. C., Goodman, N. & Tenenbaum, J. (2009). Using speakers’ referential intentions to model early cross-situational word learning. Psychological Science 20, 578–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L. & Lederer, A. (1999). Human simulations of vocabulary learning. Cognition 73, 135–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleitman, L. R. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition 1, 355.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R., Cassidy, K., Nappa, R., Papafragou, A. & Trueswell, J. C. (2005). Hard words. Language Learning and Development 1, 2364.Google Scholar
Harris, M., Jones, D. & Grant, J. (1983). The nonverbal context of mothers’ speech to infants. First Language 4, 2130.Google Scholar
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F. & Westfall, P. (2008). Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biometrical Journal 50, 346–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 434–46.Google Scholar
Jørgensen, R. N., Dale, P. S., Bleses, D. & Fenson, L. (2010). CLEX: a cross-linguistic lexical norms database. Journal of Child Language 37, 419–28.Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. & Wachtel, G. F. (1988). Children's use of mutual exclusivity to constrain the meanings of words. Cognitive Psychology 20, 121–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medina, T. N., Snedeker, J., Trueswell, J. & Gleitman, L. R. (2011). How words can and cannot be learned by observation. PNAS 108, 9014–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messenger, K., Yuan, S. & Fisher, C. (2015). Learning verb syntax via listening: new evidence from 22-month-olds. Language Learning and Development 11, 356–68.Google Scholar
Mintz, T. H. (2006). Finding the verbs: distributional cues to categories available to young learners. In Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (eds), Action meets word: how children learn verbs, 3163. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Namy, L. L. & Clepper, L. E. (2010). The differing roles of comparison and contrast in children's categorization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 107, 291305.Google Scholar
Nappa, R., Wessel, A., McEldoon, K. L., Gleitman, L. R. & Trueswell, J. C. (2009). Use of speaker's gaze and syntax in verb learning. Language Learning and Development 5, 203–34.Google Scholar
Pereira, A. F., Smith, L. B. & Yu, C. (2014). A bottom-up view of toddler word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 178–85.Google Scholar
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D. & R Core Team (2016). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1-124, online: <http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme>..>Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2014). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Online: <http://www.R-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Rubio-Fernández, P. & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science 24, 2733.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M. & Fisher, C. (2009). Two-year-olds use distributional cues to interpret transitivity-alternating verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes 24, 777803.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M. & Fisher, C. (2012). 2·5-year-olds use cross-situational consistency to learn verbs under referential uncertainty. Cognition 122, 163–80.Google Scholar
Scott, R. M. & Roby, E. (2015). Processing demands impact 3-year-olds’ performance in a spontaneous-response task: new evidence for the processing-load account of early false-belief understanding. PLoS ONE 10, e0142405.Google Scholar
Siskind, J. M. (1996). A computational study of cross-situational techniques for learning word-to-meaning mappings. Cognition 61, 3991.Google Scholar
Smith, K., Smith, A. D. M. & Blythe, R. A. (2011). Cross-situational learning: an experimental study of word-learning mechanisms. Cognitive Science 35, 480–98.Google Scholar
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., Landau, B., Gershkoff-Stowe, L. & Samuelson, L. (2002). Object name learning provides on-the-job training for attention. Psychological Science 13, 13–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, L. B. & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word–referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106, 1558–68.Google Scholar
Suanda, S. H., Foster, S. B., Smith, L. B. & Yu, C. (2013). Attentional constraints and statistics in toddlers’ word learning. In Proceedings of the Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-Epirob). CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suanda, S. H., Mugwanya, N. & Namy, L. L. (2014). Cross-situational statistical word learning in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 126, 395411.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M. & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268, 1632–4.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Medina, T. N., Hafri, A. & Gleitman, L. R. (2013). Propose but verify: fast mapping meets cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Psychology 66, 126–56.Google Scholar
Vlach, H. A. & Johnson, S. P. (2013). Memory constraints on infants’ cross-situational statistical learning. Cognition 127, 375–82.Google Scholar
Vouloumanos, A. & Werker, J. F. (2009). Infants’ learning of novel words in a stochastic environment. Developmental Psychology 45, 1611–7.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R. & Booth, A. E. (2001). Seeing pink elephants: fourteen-month-olds’ interpretations of novel nouns and adjectives. Cognitive Psychology 43, 217–42.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R. & Klibanoff, R. S. (2000). The role of comparison in the extension of novel adjectives. Developmental Psychology 36, 571–81.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., Lidz, J. L., Braun, I. E. & Lavin, T. (2009). Twenty four-month-old infants’ interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Cognitive Psychology 59, 6795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yazdi, A. A., German, T. P., Defeyter, M. A. & Siegal, M. (2006). Competence and performance in belief-desire reasoning across two cultures: the truth, the whole truth, and nothing but the true about false belief? Cognition 100, 343–68.Google Scholar
Yu, C. (2008). A statistical associative account of vocabulary growth in early word learning. Language Learning and Development 4, 3262.Google Scholar
Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2007). Rapid word learning under uncertainty via cross-situational statistics. Psychological Science 18, 414–20.Google Scholar
Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2011). What you learn is what you see: using eye movements to study infant cross-situational word learning. Developmental Science 14, 165–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition 125, 244–62.Google Scholar
Yuan, S. & Fisher, C. (2009). ‘Really? She blicked the baby?’ Two-year-olds learn combinatorial facts about verbs by listening. Psychological Science 20, 619–26.Google Scholar
Yuan, S., Fisher, C. & Snedeker, J. (2012). Counting the nouns: simple structural cues to verb meaning. Child Development 83, 1382–99.Google Scholar
Yurovsky, D. & Frank, M. C. (2015). An integrative account of constraints on cross-situational learning. Cognition 145, 5362.Google Scholar
Yurovsky, D., Fricker, D. C., Yu, C. & Smith, L. (2014). The role of partial knowledge in statistical word learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 21, 122.Google Scholar
Yurovsky, D., Hidaka, S., Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2010). Linking learning to looking: habituation and association in infant statistical language learning. In Ohlsson, S. & Catrambone, R. (eds), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 1589–94. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Yurovsky, D., Smith, L. B. & Yu, C. (2013). Statistical word learning at scale: the baby's view is better. Developmental Science 16, 959–66.Google Scholar
Yurovsky, D., Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2013). Competitive processes in cross-situational word learning. Cognitive Science 37, 891921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Z., Shi, R. & Li, A. (2015). Grammatical categorization in Mandarin-Chinese-learning infants. Language Acquisition 22, 105114.Google Scholar