Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T11:02:51.912Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early talk about the past: the origins of conversational stories of personal experience*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Peggy J. Miller*
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Linda L. Sperry
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
*
University of Chicago, Department of Education, Judd Hall, 5835 South Kimbark Avenue, Chicago, IL 60637, USA.

Abstract

This paper explores children's early talk about specific distant past events and its development into conversational stories of personal experience, with special focus on its evaluative function. The data consist of longitudinal home observations of five working-class children and their mothers from ages 2; 0 to 2; 6. Results indicate that during this period the children talked primarily about negative past events, especially events of physical harm; the rate of talk about specific past events doubled; temporally-ordered sequences increased dramatically; and the children became better able to accomplish such talk independently. In addition, one of the most striking findings is the extent to which these 2–year-olds were able to communicate their attitude toward the past event, with speech about the past containing five times more evaluative devices than other speech. These results suggest that (1) by age 2; 6 stories of personal experience emerged in incipient form, and (2) the roots of this genre lie not only in cognitive skills and social interactional support but in the emotional significance of the depicted event.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by a Spencer Foundation Seed Grant awarded to Miller and by a fellowship from the Harris Center for Developmental Studies at the University of Chicago to Sperry. We wish to thank Susan Goldin-Meadow for helpful criticism of the manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Ames, L. B. (1966). Children's stories. Genetic Psychology Monographs 73. 337–96.Google ScholarPubMed
Bloom, L. (1970). Language Development: form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Hood, L. & Lightbown, P. (1974). Imitation in language development: if, when, and why. Cognitive Psychology 6. 380420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. & Bellugi, U. (1964). Three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. Harvard Educational Review 34. 133–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, S. (1986). The role of mother-child interaction in autobiographical recall. Paper read at the symposium entitled ‘Learning to Talk about the Past’, Conference on Human Development.Google Scholar
Fein, G. G. (1981). Pretend play in childhood: an integrative review. Child Development 52. 1095–118.Google Scholar
Forster, E. M. (1927). Aspects of the novel. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Gerhardt, J. (1988). From discourse to semantics: the development of verb morphology and forms of self-reference in the speech of a two-year-old. Journal of Child Language 15. 337–93Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, J. (1986). Memories are made of this: general event knowledge and development of autobiographic memory. In Nelson, K. (ed.), Event knowledge. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1972). Language in the inner city. Philadelphia: The University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1982). Speech actions and reactions in personal narrative. In Tannen, D. (ed.), Analyzing discourse: text and talk. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (ed.), Essays in the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: American Ethnological Society.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. (1983). Representation. In Mussen, P. H., Flavell, J. H. & Markman, E. M. (eds), Handbook of Child Psychology. Vol. 3. Cognitive development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
McCartney, K. A. & Nelson, K. (1981). Children's use of scripts in story recall. Discourse processes 4. 5970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamee, G. D. (1980). The social origins of narrative skills. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Northwestern University.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J. (1982). Amy, Wendy, and Beth: learning language in South Baltimore. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J. & Byhouwer, B. (1987). The acquisition of culture through stories of personal experience. Paper read at the symposium entitled ‘The acquisition of culture through talk’, meeting of the American Ethnological Society, San Antonio.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J. & Sperry, L. L. (1987). The socialization of anger and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33. 131.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: basic building blocks of cognitive development. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. L. (eds), Advances in developmental psychology. Vol. 1. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. & Ross, G. (1980). The generalities and specifics of long-term memory in infants and young children. In Perlmutter, M. (ed.), New directions for child development. Vol. 10. Children's memory. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Nezworski, T., Stein, N. L. & Trabasso, T. (1982). Story structure versus content in children's recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 21. 196206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perlmutter, M. (ed.) (1980). New directions for child development. Vol. 10. Children's memory. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Pitcher, E. G. & Prelinger, E. (1963). Children tell stories: an analysis of fantasy. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1982). Linguistic and social constraints on storytelling. Journal of Pragmatics 6. 509–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1985). Telling the American story. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1986). Narrative organization and disorganization. Talk presented at the workshop/symposium on the ‘Acquisition of temporal structures in discourse’, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Ratner, H. H. (1980). The role of social context in memory development. In Perlmutter, M. (ed.), New directions for child development. Vol. 10. Children's memory. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P. (1980). Narrative time. In Mitchell, W. J. T. (ed.), On narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rosen, H. (1982). The nurture of narrative. Paper read at the meeting of the International Reading Association, Chicago.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Fein, G. G. & Vandenberg, B. (1983). Play. In Mussen, P. H. & Hetherington, E. M. (eds). Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1980). The role of adult-child play in language development. In Rubin, K. (ed.), New directions for child development. Vol. 10. Children's play. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1983). Talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. In Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's language. Vol. 4. New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. (1972). On the analyzability of stories by children. In Gumperz, J. J. & Hymes, D. (eds), Directions in sociolinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Scollon, R. & Scollon, S. B. K. (1981). Narrative, literacy, and face in interethnic communication. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Stein, N. L. & Glenn, C. G. (1982). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. In Freedle, R. O. (ed.), New directions in discourse processing. Vol. 2. Norwood NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Umiker-Sebeok, D. J. (1979). Preschool children's intraconversational narratives. Journal of Child Language 6. 91109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H. (1980). Children's understanding of stories: assimilation by a general schema for actions or coordination of temporal relations. In Wilkening, F., Becker, J. & Trabasso, T. (eds), Information integration by children. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar