Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:22:45.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

¡Casi te caístes!: Variation in second person singular preterit forms in Spanish Children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2021

Elisabeth BAKER*
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
*
Address for correspondence: Elisabeth Baker University of New Mexico Spanish & Portuguese MSC03 2100 1 University of New MexicoAlbuquerque, NM87131-0001USA E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The current study investigates Spanish children's variation between the standard and non-standard forms for second person singular preterit –s (caiste ~ caístes). All second person singular preterit forms were extracted from the spontaneous speech of 78 children in Spain and analyzed for the effects of age, language contact setting, and lexical frequency. Results show that children in contact with Galician and Catalan produce more non-standard than children in non-contact areas like Madrid. Meanwhile, low-frequency verbs (e.g., pillaste) are more likely to occur with the non-standard –s than high-frequency verbs (e.g., fuiste). However, age is not a significant predictor of children's 2sg preterit production. These preliminary findings demonstrate that Spanish children do have the non-standard -s in their speech, and that their 2sg preterit forms are significantly conditioned by language contact and lexical frequency.

Type
Brief Research Report
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aguado-Orea, J., & Pine, J. M. (2015). Comparing different models of the development of verb inflection in early child Spanish. PloS one, 10(3), e0119613.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aguirre, C. (2000). La adquisición de las categorías gramaticales en español. Ediciones de la Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
Aleza, M., & Enguita, J. M. (2010). La lengua española en américa: Normas y usos actuales. Valencia: Universitat de València.Google Scholar
Barnes, S. (2012). ¿Qué dijistes?: A variationist reanalysis of non-standard –s second singular preterit verb forms in Spanish. Selected proceedings of the 201 Hispanic Linguistic Symposium, Somervile, MA: Cascadilla press, 3847.Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J., & Hopper, P. (2001). Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (2007). Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2007. 522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. L. (2015). Language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cappelli, G., Marrero-Aguiar, V., & Albalá, M. J. (1994). Aplicación del sistema MORFO a una muestra de lenguaje infantil. Procesamiento del Lenguaje Natural. 14, 2332.Google Scholar
CHILDES Database BecaCESNo Corpus. doi:10.21415/T5ZG77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
CHILDES Database LlinasOjea Corpus. doi:10.21415/T5K01QCrossRefGoogle Scholar
CHILDES Database FernAguado Corpus. doi:10.21415/T5MK75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, H., Aveledo, F., & Roca, I. (2002). The development of regular and irregular verb inflection in Spanish child language. Journal of Child Language, 29(3), 591622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cousillas, J. (2019). El castellano de Galicia. Una propuesta didáctica para el aprendizaje de ELE. Masters thesis. University of Santiago de Compostela.Google Scholar
Davies, M. (2018). Corpus del Español NOW: 6.2 billion words, 21 countries. Available online at https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/now (2012-present)Google Scholar
Diez-Itza, E. (1995). Procesos fonológicos en la adquisición del español como lengua materna. In Ruiz, J.M., Sheerin, P., & González-Cascos, E. (Eds.), Actas del XI Congreso Nacional de Linguistica Aplicada. Valladolid:Universidad de Valladolid.Google Scholar
Escalante, C. (2015). ¿Qué twiteastes tú? Variation in second person singular preterit –s in Spanish tweets. Entrehojas: Revista de Estudios Hispánicos 5(1), 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández Pérez, M. (2015). Lenguaje infantil y medidas de desarrollo, ENSAYOS (monográfico sobre Educación infantil), 30/2, 5369.Google Scholar
Fernández-Dobao, A., & Herschensohn, J. (2020). Acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology by child bilinguals: Overregularization by heritage speakers and second language learners. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 113.Google Scholar
Fox, J., & Weisberg, S. (2019). An R Companion to Applied Regression, Third edition. Sage, Thousand Oaks CA. https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/.Google Scholar
Johnson, C. M. (1995). Verb errors in the early acquisition of Mexican and Castilian Spanish. In Clark, E.V. (ed.) The proceedings of the twenty-seventh annual child language research forum, Center study language & information, 175186Google Scholar
Labov, W. (2001). Principles of Linguistic Change: Social Factors. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lerner, M. (2016). The acquisition of sociolinguistic variation in a Mexican immigrant community. Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations. 2420.Google Scholar
Liceras, J. M., Fuertes, R. F., Perales, S., Pérez-Tattam, R., & Spradlin, K. T. (2008). Gender and gender agreement in bilingual native and non-native grammars: A view from child and adult functional–lexical mixings. Lingua, 118(6), 827851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linaza, J., Sebastián, M. E., & del Barrio, C. (1981). Lenguaje, comunicación y comprensión. La adquisición del lenguaje. Monografía de Infancia y Aprendizaje, 195198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipski, J. M. (2014). El Español de América. Octava Edición. New York: Longman Group Limited.Google Scholar
López Ornat, S. (1994). La adquisición de la lengua Española. Madrid: Siglo XXI.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (Ed.). (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analysing talk: Vol. 1. Mawah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, K. (2013). Acquisition of variable rules: /s/-lenition in the speech of Chilean Spanish-speaking children and their caregivers. Language Variation and Change, 25, 311340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K., & Schmitt, C. (2012). Variable input and the acquisition of plural morphology. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Language Development 19, 223261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic Acquisition in Bilingual Children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Pereira, M. (1989). The acquisition of morphemes: Some evidence from Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18(3), 289.Google ScholarPubMed
Penny, R. J. (2000). Variation and change in Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Shin, N. L. (2016). Acquiring constraints on morphosyntactic variation: children's Spanish subject pronoun expression. Journal of Child Language, 43(4), 914947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silva-Corvalán, C. (2014). Bilingual language acquisition: Spanish and English in the first six years. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., & Durham, M. (2019). Sociolinguistic Variation in Children's Language: Acquiring Community Norms (Studies in Language Variation and Change). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinoco, A. R. (2017). Variation of the second person singular of the simple past tense in Twitter: Hiciste vs. hicistes ‘you did’. Dialectologia. Special issue, 7, 149167.Google Scholar
Vila, I. (1990). Adquisición y desarrollo del lenguaje. Barcelona: Graó.Google Scholar