Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Roberts, Kenneth
and
Cuff, Martin D.
1989.
Categorization studies of 9- to 15-month-old infants: Evidence for superordinate categorization?.
Infant Behavior and Development,
Vol. 12,
Issue. 3,
p.
265.
Reznick, J. Steven
1990.
Visual preference as a test of infant word comprehension.
Applied Psycholinguistics,
Vol. 11,
Issue. 2,
p.
145.
Roberts, Kenneth
and
Jacob, Marianne
1991.
Linguistic versus attentional influences on nonlinguistic categorization in 15-month-old infants.
Cognitive Development,
Vol. 6,
Issue. 4,
p.
355.
Carabine, Bob
1991.
Fuzzy boundaries and the extension of object-words.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 2,
p.
355.
Hallé, Pierre A.
and
Boysson-Bardies, Bénédicte de
1994.
Emergence of an early receptive lexicon: Infants' recognition of words.
Infant Behavior and Development,
Vol. 17,
Issue. 2,
p.
119.
Naigles, Letitia G.
and
Gelman, Susan A.
1995.
Overextensions in comprehension and production revisited: preferential-looking in a study of dog, cat, and cow.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 22,
Issue. 1,
p.
19.
1997.
REFERENCES.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Vol. 62,
Issue. 1,
p.
142.
Schafer, Graham
and
Plunkett, Kim
1998.
Rapid Word Learning by Fifteen‐Month‐Olds under Tightly Controlled Conditions.
Child Development,
Vol. 69,
Issue. 2,
p.
309.
Tincoff, Ruth
and
Jusczyk, Peter W.
1999.
Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds.
Psychological Science,
Vol. 10,
Issue. 2,
p.
172.
Huibregtse, Ineke
Admiraal, Wilfried
and
Meara, Paul
2002.
Scores on a yes-no vocabulary test: correction for guessing and response style.
Language Testing,
Vol. 19,
Issue. 3,
p.
227.
Tan, Seok Hui
and
Schafer, Graham
2005.
Toddlers’ novel word learning: Effects of phonological representation, vocabulary size and parents’ ostensive behaviour.
First Language,
Vol. 25,
Issue. 2,
p.
131.
Schafer, Graham
2005.
Infants Can Learn Decontextualized Words Before Their First Birthday.
Child Development,
Vol. 76,
Issue. 1,
p.
87.
Gelman, Susan A.
and
Gottfried, Gail M.
2006.
Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development.
p.
221.
HOUSTON-PRICE, CARMEL
MATHER, EMILY
and
SAKKALOU, ELENA
2007.
Discrepancy between parental reports of infants' receptive vocabulary and infants' behaviour in a preferential looking task.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 34,
Issue. 4,
p.
701.
Arias-Trejo, Natalia
2010.
Young children’s extension of novel labels to novel animate items in three testing conditions.
International Journal of Behavioral Development,
Vol. 34,
Issue. 3,
p.
206.
BORNSTEIN, MARC H.
and
HENDRICKS, CHARLENE
2012.
Basic language comprehension and production in >100,000 young children from sixteen developing nations.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 39,
Issue. 4,
p.
899.
Hendrickson, Kristi
Mitsven, Samantha
Poulin‐Dubois, Diane
Zesiger, Pascal
and
Friend, Margaret
2015.
Looking and touching: what extant approaches reveal about the structure of early word knowledge.
Developmental Science,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 5,
p.
723.
Davidson, Denise
Rainey, Vanessa R.
Vanegas, Sandra B.
and
Hilvert, Elizabeth
2018.
The effects of type of instruction, animacy cues, and dimensionality of objects on the shape bias in 3‐ to 6‐year‐old children.
Infant and Child Development,
Vol. 27,
Issue. 1,
Ferreira Pinto, Renato
and
Xu, Yang
2021.
A computational theory of child overextension.
Cognition,
Vol. 206,
Issue. ,
p.
104472.
Arias-Trejo, Natalia
Angulo-Chavira, Armando Q.
and
Plunkett, Kim
2025.
The effects of phonological and semantic similarity on early referent identification.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Vol. 249,
Issue. ,
p.
106112.