Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:57:41.120Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children's on-line processing of epistemic modals*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 June 2016

VINCENZO MOSCATI
Affiliation:
University of Siena
LIKAN ZHAN
Affiliation:
Beijing Language and Culture University
PENG ZHOU
Affiliation:
Macquarie University

Abstract

In this paper we investigated the real-time processing of epistemic modals in five-year-olds. In a simple reasoning scenario, we monitored children's eye-movements while processing a sentence with modal expressions of different force (might/must). Children were also asked to judge the truth-value of the target sentences at the end of the reasoning task. Consistent with previous findings (Noveck, 2001), we found that children's behavioural responses were much less accurate compared to adults. Their eye-movements, however, revealed that children did not treat the two modal expressions alike. As soon as a modal expression was presented, children and adults showed a similar fixation pattern that varied as a function of the modal expression they heard. It is only at the very end of the sentence that children's fixations diverged from the adult ones. We discuss these findings in relation to the proposal that children narrow down the set of possible outcomes in undetermined reasoning scenarios and endorse only one possibility among several (Acredolo & Horobin, 1987, Ozturk & Papafragou, 2015).

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

We wish to thank Milena Romano for her help in recruiting and testing our young participants. This study is the result of the joint work of the authors, who equally contributed to the paper. For the sole purpose of the Italian academic system, V. Moscati is directly responsible for the ‘Introduction’ and the ‘Method’ sections. L. Zhan for ‘Results’ and ‘Discussion’ and P. Zhou for the ‘Conclusion’.

References

REFERENCES

Acredolo, C. & Horobin, K. (1987). Development of relational reasoning and premature closure. Developmental Psychology 23(1), 1321.Google Scholar
Barr, D. J. (2008). Analyzing ‘visual world’ eyetracking data using multilevel logistic regression. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 457–74.Google Scholar
Bascelli, E. & Barbieri, M. S. (2002). Italian children's understanding of epistemic and deontic modal verbs dovere (must) and potere (may). Journal of Child Language 29, 87107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, S. R. & Robinson, E. J. (2001). Children's ability to make tentative interpretations of ambiguous messages. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 79, 95114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliss, L. S. (1988). Modal usage by pre-school children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 9, 253–61.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P. & Duff, M. A. (1989). Young children's comprehension of modal expressions. Cognitive Development 4, 369–87.Google Scholar
Choi, Y. & Trueswell, J. C. (2010). Children's (in)ability to recover from garden paths in a verb-final language: evidence for developing control in sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 106, 4161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W. & Weil, J. (1982). Acquisition of epistemic and deontic meaning of modals. Journal of Child Language 9, 659–66.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuczaj, S. A. & Maratsos, M. P. (1975). What children can say before they will. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, 89111.Google Scholar
Noveck, I. A. (2001). When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature. Cognition 78(2), 165–88.Google Scholar
Noveck, I. A., Ho, S. & Sera, M. (1996). Children's understanding of epistemic modals. Journal of Child Language 23(3), 621–43.Google Scholar
Ozturk, O. & Papafragou, A. (2015). The acquisition of epistemic modality: from semantic meaning to pragmatic interpretation. Language Learning and Development 11(3), 191214.Google Scholar
Palmer, F. (1986). Mood and modality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Perkins, M. (1983). Modal expressions in English. London: Frances Pinter.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Portner, P. (2009). Modality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rayner, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2010). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Online: <http://www.r-project.org/>..>Google Scholar
Robinson, E. J. & Whittaker, S. J. (1986). Children's conceptions of meaning–message relationships. Cognition 22, 4160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, S. C. (1982). From deontic to epistemic: an analysis of modals in the history of English, creoles, and language acquisition. In Ahlquist, A. (ed.), Papers from the fifth international conference on historical linguistics (pp. 316323). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Trueswell, J. C. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: the role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology 49, 238–99.Google Scholar
Snedeker, J. & Yuan, S. (2008). Effects of prosodic and lexical constraints on parsing in young children (and adults). Journal of Memory and Language 58, 574608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, S. C., Hadkinson, B. A. & Greenberg, C. (1979). Two levels of inferential behavior in young children. Child Development 50(1), 119–31.Google Scholar
Sophian, C. & Somerville, S. C. (1988). Early developments in logical reasoning – considering alternative possibilities. Cognitive Development 3, 183222.Google Scholar
Stromswold, K. (1990). Learnability and the acquisition of auxiliaries. Unpublished PhD dissertation, available through MIT's Working Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. (1988). Conceptual perspective taking: children's ability to distinguish what they know from what they see. Child Development 59, 703–18.Google Scholar
Tanenhaus, M., Spivey-Knowlton, M., Eberhard, K. & Sedivy, J. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science 268, 1632–4.Google Scholar
Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M. & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition 73, 89134.Google Scholar
Wells, G. (1979). Learning and using the auxiliary verb in English. In Lee, V. (ed.), Cognitive development: language and thinking from birth to adolescence (pp. 250270). London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Zhan, L., Crain, S. & Zhou, P. (2015). The online processing of only if and even if conditional statements: implications for mental models. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 27(3), 367–79.Google Scholar
Zhou, P., Crain, S. & Zhan, L. (2012). Sometimes children are as good as adults: the pragmatic use of prosody in children's on-line sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language 67, 149–64.Google Scholar
Zhou, P., Crain, S., & Zhan, L. (2014). Grammatical aspect and event recognition in children's online sentence comprehension. Cognition 133, 262–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed