Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T22:58:33.229Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Homo Diplomaticus: Mixed-Method Evidence of Variation in Strategic Rationality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 April 2017

Get access

Abstract

Psychology is traditionally used in political science to explain deviations from rationality. Lost in the debate between rationalists and their critics, however, is a sense of whether the kinds of strategic self-interested behavior predicted by these models has psychological microfoundations: what would homo economicus look like in the real world? We argue that strategic rationality varies across individuals and is characterized by a pro-self social-value orientation and a high level of epistemic motivation. Testing our argument in the context of international relations, we employ a laboratory bargaining game and integrate it with archival research on German foreign policy-making in the 1920s. We find in both contexts that even among those interested in maximizing only their own egoistic gains, those with greater epistemic motivation are better able to adapt to the strategic situation, particularly the distribution of power. Our results build a bridge between two approaches often considered to be antithetical to one another.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The IO Foundation 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achen, Christopher H., and Snidal, Duncan. 1989. Rational Deterrence Theory and Comparative Case Studies. World Politics 41 (2):143–69.Google Scholar
Akten zur deutschen ausw ärtigen Politik (ADAP). 1918–1945. Various years. Serie A, Band XII (1. Januar bis 25. April 1925). Göttingen: Archiv des Auswärtigen Amtes.Google Scholar
Baron, Jonathan. 1994. Thinking and Deciding. 2nd ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bayram, A. Burcu. 2017. Due Deference: Cosmopolitan Social Identity and the Psychology of Legal Obligation in International Politics. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S137–S163.Google Scholar
Bernard, Henry, Goetz, Wolfgang, and Wiegler, Paul, eds. 1932. Vermächtnis, der Nachlaß in drei Bänden. Vol. 2: Locarno und Genf. [German version of Stresemann's diaries and papers] Berlin: Ullstein.Google Scholar
Bueno de Mesquita, Bruce. 2014. Principles of International Politics. 5th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage/Congressional Quarterly Press.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, John T., Petty, Richard E., Feinstein, Jeffrey A., and Jarvis, W. Blair G.. 1996. Dispositional Differences in Cognitive Motivation: The Life and Times of Individuals Varying in Need for Cognition. Psychological Bulletin 119 (2):197.Google Scholar
Cohrs, Patrick O. 2006. The Unfinished Peace after World War I: America, Britain, and the Stabilisation of Europe, 1919–1932. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Craig, Gordon A. 1978. Germany, 1866–1945. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De Dreu, Carsten K.W., and Carnevale, Peter J.. 2003. Motivational Bases of Information Processing and Strategy in Conflict and Negotiation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 35:235–91.Google Scholar
Documents on British Foreign Policy (DBFP). 1919–1939. Various years. London: Her Majesty's Stationery Office.Google Scholar
Fearon, James. 1995. Rationalist Explanations for War. International Organization 49 (3):379414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fearon, James, and Wendt, Alexander. 2002. Rationalism v. Constructivism: A Skeptical View. In Handbook of International Relations, edited by Carlsnaes, Walter, Risse, Thomas, and Simmons, Beth A., 52–72. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Fehr, Ernst, and Fischbacher, Urs. 2002. Why Social Preferences Matter: The Impact of Non-Selfish Motives on Competition, Cooperation, and Incentives. The Economic Journal 112 (478):C1C33.Google Scholar
George, Alexander L., and Smoke, Richard. 1974. Deterrence in American Foreign Policy. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Glaser, Charles L. 2010. Rational Theory of International Politics: The Logic of Competition and Cooperation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Grathwol, Robert P. 1980. Stresemann and the DNVP: Reconciliation or Revenge in German Foreign Policy, 1924–1928. Lawrence, KS: Regents of Kansas.Google Scholar
Green, Donald P., and Shapiro, Ian. 1994. Pathologies of Rational Choice Theory: A Critique of Applications in Political Science. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Hafner-Burton, Emilie, Haggard, Stephan, Lake, David A., and Victor, David G.. 2017. The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S1–S31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrmann, Richard K. 2017. How Attachments to the Nation Shape Beliefs About the World: A Theory of Motivated Reasoning. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S61–S84.Google Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, Sears, David O., and Levy, Jack S.. 2013. Introduction: Theoretical Foundations of Political Psychology. In The Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, 2nd ed., edited by Huddy, Leonie, Sears, David O., and Levy, Jack S., 119. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huth, Paul, and Russett, Bruce. 1984. What Makes Deterrence Work? Cases from 1900 to 1980. World Politics 36 (4):496526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, Jon. 1972. Locarno Diplomacy: Germany and the West, 1925–1929. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Jervis, Robert. 1976. Perception and Misperception in International Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Jost, John T., Glaser, Jack, Kruglanski, Arie W., and Sulloway, Frank J.. 2003. Political Conservatism as Motivated Social Cognition. Psychological Bulletin 129 (3):339375.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel. 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kelley, Harold H., and Thibaut, John W.. 1978. Interpersonal Relations: A Theory of Interdependence. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Keohane, Robert O. 1984. After Hegemony: Cooperation and Discord in the World Political Economy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D. 2016. Resolve in International Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D. 2017. Resolve, Time, and Risk. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S109–S136.Google Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Rathbun, Brian C.. 2015. Fair is Fair: Social Preferences and Reciprocity in International Politics. World Politics 67 (4):613–55.Google Scholar
Kertzer, Joshua D., and Renshon, Jonathan. 2014. Putting Things in Perspective: Mental Simulation in Experimental Political Science. Working paper.Google Scholar
Khong, Yuen F. 1992. Analogies at War: Korea, Munich, Dien Bien Phu, and the Vietnam Decision of 1965. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, Arie W., and Webster, Donna M.. 1996. Motivated Closing of the Mind: “Seizing” and “Freezing.” Psychological Review 103 (2):263–83.Google Scholar
Lake, David A., and Powell, Robert. 1999. Strategic Choice and International Relations. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lane, Robert E. 2003. Rescuing Political Science from Itself. In Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, edited by Sears, David O., Huddy, Leonie, and Jervis, Robert, 755–93. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lerner, Jennifer S., and Tetlock, Philip E.. 1999. Accounting for the Effects of Accountability. Psychological Bulletin 125 (2):255–75.Google Scholar
Levitt, Steven D., and List, John A.. 2007. What Do Laboratory Experiments Measuring Social Preferences Reveal About the Real World? Journal of Economic Perspectives 21 (2):153–74.Google Scholar
Lupia, Arthur, McCubbins, Mathew D., and Popkin, Samuel L.. 2000. Elements of Reason: Cognition, Choice, and the Bounds of Rationality. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
MacDonald, Paul K. 2003. Useful Fiction or Miracle Maker: The Competing Epistemological Foundations of Rational Choice Theory. American Political Science Review 97 (4):551–66.Google Scholar
McCubbins, Mathew D., and Turner, Mark. 2012. Going Cognitive: Tools for Rebuilding the Social Sciences. In Grounding Social Sciences in Cognitive Sciences, edited by Sun, Ron, 387414. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 1998. Risk-Taking in International Politics: Prospect Theory in American Foreign Policy. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
McDermott, Rose. 2004. The Feeling of Rationality: The Meaning of Neuroscience for Political Science. Perspectives on Politics 2 (4):691706.Google Scholar
Mercer, Jonathan. 1996. Reputation and International Politics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Mercer, Jonathan. 2005. Rationality and Psychology in International Politics. International Organization 59 (1):77106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Aaron David. 2008. The Much Too Promised Land: America's Elusive Search for Arab-Israeli Peace. New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Minuth, Karl-Heinz, ed. 1977. Akten der Reichskanzlerei Weimarer Republik: Die Kabinette Luther I and II, vols. 1–2. Boppard am Rhein: Harald Boldt Verlag.Google Scholar
Parks, Craig D. 1994. The Predictive Ability of Social Values in Resource Dilemmas and Public Good Games. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20 (4):431–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quattrone, George A., and Tversky, Amos. 1988. Contrasting Rational and Psychological Analyses of Political Choice. American Political Science Review 82 (3):719–36.Google Scholar
Quek, Kai. 2017. Rationalist Experiments on War. Political Science Research and Methods 5 (1):123–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabin, Matthew. 2002. A Perspective on Psychology and Economics. European Economic Review 46 (4):657–85.Google Scholar
Rathbun, Brian C. 2014. Diplomacy's Value: Creating Security in 1920s Europe and the Contemporary Middle East. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Renshon, Jonathan, J. Lee, Julia, and Tingley, Dustin. 2017. Emotions and the Micro-Foundations of Commitment Problems. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S189–S217.Google Scholar
Rescher, Nicholas. 1988. Rationality: A Philosophical Inquiry into the Nature and the Rationale of Reason. Oxford, UK: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Rho, Sungmin, and Tomz, Michael. 2017. Why Don't Trade Preferences Reflect Economic Self-Interest? In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S85–S108.Google Scholar
Riker, William H. 1995. The Political Psychology of Rational Choice Theory. Political Psychology 16 (1):2344.Google Scholar
Saunders, Elizabeth N. 2017. No Substitute for Experience: Presidents, Advisers, and Information in Group Decision Making. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S219–S247.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1955. A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics 69 (1):99118.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1978. Rationality as Process and as Product of Thought. The American Economic Review 68 (2):116.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert A. 1983. Reason in Human Affairs. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, Stephen M., and Levin, Irwin P.. 1996. Need for Cognition and Choice Framing Effects. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 9 (4):283–90.Google Scholar
Snidal, Duncan. 2002. Rational Choice and International Relations. In Handbook of International Relations, edited by Carlsnaes, Walter, Risse, Thomas and Simmons, Beth A., 7394. London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahl, Dale O., and Wilson, Paul W.. 1995. On Players’ Models of Other Players: Theory and Experimental Evidence. Games and Economic Behavior 10: 218–54.Google Scholar
Stanovich, Keith E. 2011. Rationality and the Reflective Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, Keith E., and West, Richard F.. 1998. Individual Differences in Rational Thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 127 (2):161–88.Google Scholar
Stanovich, Keith E., and West, Richard F.. 2000. Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the Rationality Debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 (5):645–65.Google Scholar
Sutton, Eric, ed. 1940. Gustav Stresemann: His Diaries, Letters and Papers. Vols. 2–3. 1940. Translated by Sutton, Eric. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Tingley, Dustin H. 2011. The Dark Side of the Future: An Experimental Test of Commitment Problems in Bargaining. International Studies Quarterly 55 (2):521–44.Google Scholar
Tingley, Dustin. 2017. Rising Power on the Mind. In The Behavioral Revolution and International Relations, supplement, International Organization 71 (S1):S165–S188.Google Scholar
Verhandlungen des Deutschen Reichstags (VDR) . Berlin: Norddeutscher Buchdruckerei [Parliamentary record of the German parliament] Google Scholar
Walt, Stephen M. 1999. Rigor or Rigor Mortis? Rational Choice and Security Studies. International Security 23 (4):548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waltz, Kenneth N. 1979. Theory of International Politics. Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Webster, Donna M., and Kruglanski, Arie W.. 1994. Individual Differences in Need for Cognitive Closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 (6):1049–62.Google Scholar
Wright, Jonathan. 2002. Gustav Stresemann: Weimar's Greatest Statesman. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zagare, Frank C. 1990. Rationality and Deterrence. World Politics 42 (2):238–60.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Rathbun supplementary material

Rathbun supplementary material

Download Rathbun supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 2.3 MB