Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:50:34.148Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Educational attainment is associated with unconditional helping behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 December 2019

Grace Westlake
Affiliation:
School of Human Sciences, The University of Western Australia, M309, LB 5005, Perth, WA6001, Australia
David Coall
Affiliation:
School of Medical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Joondalup, WA6027, Australia School of Medicine, The University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA6009, Australia
Cyril C. Grueter*
Affiliation:
School of Human Sciences, The University of Western Australia, M309, LB 5005, Perth, WA6001, Australia Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Biological Sciences, The University of Western Australia, Perth, WA6001, Australia
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Altruism is a universal human trait, but little is known about its within-population variation. Socio-economic status (SES) has been found to positively impact altruism, but the specific socio-economic variables behind this relationship have remained elusive. This study aimed to determine which facets of SES predict altruism using a lost letter paradigm and a novel lost letter method. Six hundred letters (half dropped on the pavement, half sent to residential addresses) were distributed in 20 suburbs of Perth (Australia) differing in socio-economic variables. Letters distributed in high-SES neighbourhoods were more likely to be returned than letters distributed in low-SES neighbourhoods. Educational attainment and occupation status were the specific socio-economic variables underlying this association, while economic resources and crime rate were not associated with the likelihood of a letter being returned. These results suggest that altruism blossoms in neighbourhoods that are populated with highly educated individuals working in high-status jobs. The relationship between education and prosocial inclinations may be mediated by cognitive ability, self-control and high levels of socialization. Having experienced sustained exposure to norm-abiding models, more educated people may also be better at internalizing cultural norms of helping behaviour, thus creating a more altruistic environment where they reside.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Media summary: A field experiment suggest that altruism blossoms in neighbourhoods populated with highly educated individuals working in high-status jobs.

Introduction

Cooperation – a behaviour that provides a benefit to another individual – is a human universal. Cooperative behaviour between relatives is typically governed by kin selection, which suggests that individuals can increase their inclusive fitness by helping genetic relatives (Hamilton Reference Hamilton1964). Cooperation involving non-relatives (and sometimes relatives; Allen-Arave et al. Reference Allen-Arave, Gurven and Hill2008) can be explained by direct reciprocity, which involves exchanges of favours between individuals who are expected to have future encounters (Trivers Reference Trivers1971; Axelrod and Hamilton Reference Axelrod and Hamilton1981). Furthermore, individuals can help others in order to enhance their own reputation through indirect reciprocity (Alexander Reference Alexander1987; Nowak and Sigmund Reference Nowak and Sigmund1998). However, these reciprocity explanations require either the recipient or a witness to be aware of the actor's identity. Some altruistic acts (i.e. behaviours that are costly to the actor and beneficial to the recipient) are anonymous whereby the recipient of the act is not aware of the actor's identity and no third party witnesses the act. These types of anonymous altruistic acts reflect unconditional or genuine altruism, which is altruism without an expectation of future reciprocation and without reputational benefits.

Explanations of prosocial behaviour in large groups of unrelated individuals fall into two broad categories. First, the ‘mismatch’ hypothesis posits that helping behaviour in present-day large-scale societies is the result of psychological dispositions for cooperation that evolved mainly in the context of repeated interactions with known individuals in ancestral environments in which the actor would have obtained benefits through any of the aforementioned theories (kin selection, direct and indirect reciprocity; e.g. Burnham and Johnson Reference Burnham and Johnson2005). However, this hypothesis has been challenged on multiple grounds. First, even in past environments, ephemeral interactions with strangers probably took place. Further, this hypothesis makes the questionable assumption that humans lack the ability to adjust behaviour in light of knowledge about the probability of future payoffs from an interaction (Chudek et al. Reference Chudek, Zhao, Henrich, Joyce, Sterelny and Calcott2013; Fehr and Henrich Reference Fehr, Henrich and Hammerstein2003; Raihani and Bshary Reference Raihani and Bshary2015). Second, cultural group selection suggests that groups whose members engage in individually costly, cooperative, altruistic behaviours may have a competitive advantage over other groups whose members do not cooperate, which can lead to the emergence of locally stable equilibria of prosocial norms (Gintis Reference Gintis2000; Henrich Reference Henrich2004; Richerson et al. Reference Richerson, Boyd, Henrich and Hammerstein2003, Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis, Mathew, Newton and Naar2016; Chudek and Henrich Reference Chudek and Henrich2011; but see Mollemen et al. Reference Molleman, Quiñones and Weissing2013; Raihani and Bshary Reference Raihani and Bshary2015). The evolution of cooperative cultural norms within populations is facilitated by an inclination for conformity-based social learning (Henrich and Boyd Reference Henrich and Boyd2001; but see André and Morin Reference André and Morin2011).

Altruistic behaviours do not manifest in human populations at uniform levels (Levine et al. Reference Levine, Norenzayan and Philbrick2001; Richerson et al. Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis, Mathew, Newton and Naar2016). The prevalence of altruism may thus be subject to contextual differences, meaning that it may have different fitness benefits in different contexts. Cross-cultural variation in cooperative tendencies has been shown to be present in many small-scale societies (Henrich et al. Reference Henrich, Boyd, Bowles, Camerer, Fehr, Gintis, McElreath, Alvard and Barr2005, Reference Henrich, McElreath, Barr, Ensminger, Barrett, Bolyanatz, Cardenas, Gurven and Gwako2006, Reference Henrich, Ensminger, McElreath, Barr, Barrett, Bolyanatz, Cardenas, Gurven and Gwako2010), as well as industrialized societies (Hermann et al. Reference Hermann, Thöni and Gächter2008; Roth et al. Reference Roth, Prasnikar, Okuni-Fujiwara and Zamir1991). These population-level differences may be mediated by culturally inherited cooperative norms (Henrich Reference Henrich2004; Henrich et al. Reference Henrich, Boyd, Bowles, Camerer, Fehr, Gintis, McElreath, Alvard and Barr2005, Reference Henrich, McElreath, Barr, Ensminger, Barrett, Bolyanatz, Cardenas, Gurven and Gwako2006; Richerson et al. Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis, Mathew, Newton and Naar2016). Lamba and Mace (Reference Lamba and Mace2011) have suggested that differences in cultural norms can be explained by demographic and ecological differences such as population size and age structures. This suggests that contextual differences can create variation in altruistic behaviour between different populations. However, little is known about the degree to which these factors may affect variation within populations. Cultural group selection suggests that social norms may result in relatively uniform altruistic behaviour within a group. However, there is much evidence to suggest that groups do not always exhibit uniform behaviours, and that within-population variation in altruistic behaviour does exist (Levine et al. Reference Levine, Norenzayan and Philbrick2001; Richerson et al. Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis, Mathew, Newton and Naar2016).

One method that has been used to investigate altruistic behaviour is the lost letter experiment (Milgram et al. Reference Milgram, Mann and Harter1965). The lost letter experiment involves scattering addressed, stamped and sealed envelopes throughout an area, so that each letter looks as though someone accidentally dropped it while on the way to post it. These letters are then naturalistically encountered by random individuals in the area. If choosing to act altruistically, individuals can pick up the letter and post it through a post box, or if choosing to not act altruistically, individuals can ignore it. The rate of letters that are returned is used as a proxy for altruism in a geographical area; a high return rate indicates a high willingness of individuals to be altruistic, while a low return rate indicates a low willingness. The lost letter experiment primarily addresses unconditional altruism, because it is a largely anonymous altruistic act that cannot be reciprocated. The naturalistic nature and resulting high ecological validity make the lost letter experiment a useful tool to investigate heterogeneity in prosocial behaviour on a geographic and socioeconomic scale. Despite the relatively widespread use of the lost letter method to examine human behaviour, it has some limitations such as non-residents encountering letters and the effects of differing pedestrian foot traffic.

It has been widely shown that the letter return rate is positively correlated with increasing socio-economic status (SES); letters distributed in high-SES neighbourhoods are returned at a higher rate than letters distributed in low-SES neighbourhoods (Brown and Reed Reference Brown and Reed1982; Chang et al. Reference Chang, Dee, Wing Tse and Yu2016; Grueter et al. Reference Grueter, Ingram, Lewisson, Bradford, Taba, Coetzee and Sherwood2016; Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012; Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Colléony and Cockerill2011; Silva and Mace Reference Silva and Mace2014). However, the exact mechanisms behind this effect are unknown. Socio-economic status is a multi-dimensional measure that is composed of many different variables such as economic resources, educational attainment, occupation status, employment status, crime and family structure variables such as the marital status of parents. It is unclear which of the many components of SES affect the tendencies of individuals to act altruistically.

One possible dimension of SES that may affect altruism is economic resources. Economically deprived individuals may not have stable access to basic needs such as food and housing and may thus be primarily focused on meeting these immediate needs, leaving them with less time and energy to spend helping others. Conversely, individuals with greater economic resources may have abundant access to their basic needs, leaving them with reserve time and energy to spare for altruistic behaviour (Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012).

Alternative mechanisms through which SES may affect altruism are educational attainment and occupation status. Considering that socialization is an important component of school curricula and facilitates interactions with strangers (Glaeser et al. Reference Glaeser, Ponzetto and Shleifer2007), one could argue that higher levels of helping among the more educated may also reflect better social skills. Individuals with a higher educational attainment may also have a broader arsenal of cognitive abilities that may influence their prosociality. Jones (Reference Jones2008) found that cooperation in Prisoner's Dilemma games among university students increased with the universities’ average SAT score, which suggests that more educated and intelligent individuals may have a greater willingness or ability to cooperate. This was reinforced by Segal and Hershberger (Reference Segal and Hershberger1999), who administered IQ tests and Prisoner's Dilemma games to identical and fraternal twins. They found that pairs with a higher combined IQ were more likely to cooperate with each other. One explanation for these findings is that intelligent individuals may be more patient (Frederick Reference Frederick2005; Warner and Pleeter Reference Warner and Pleeter2001). Curry et al. (Reference Curry, Price and Price2008) found that patient individuals were more cooperative in a series of economic games. They suggested that their preference for future over immediate rewards enabled them to engage successfully in long-term, reciprocal altruism. Additionally, using a cultural group selection perspective, education can be construed as the engine of a process whereby prosocial norms become internalized in a culturally prescribed social group. The upshot is the proliferation of positive attitudes towards helping behaviour among the educated.

Another mechanism by which SES may influence altruism is through environmental stability. Low-SES individuals may lack environmental stability owing to factors such as increased crime and poor healthcare. In these harsher and more unpredictable environments distant rewards are discounted, as they may never be attained, and behaviour is biased towards impulsivity, reward seeking and risk taking (Chisholm Reference Chisholm1999; Coall et al. Reference Coall, Dickins, Nettle and Poiani2012). With shortened time horizons, the willingness of individuals to engage in behaviours without immediate benefits decreases (Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012; Maskin and Fudenberg Reference Maskin and Fudenberg1986). This may cause low-SES individuals to be disinterested in long-term cooperation without immediate reciprocation. Furthermore, higher crime rates may decrease individuals’ trust of others, leading them to behave less altruistically (Alesina and Ferrara Reference Alesina and Ferrara2002). Trust is essential for altruism because individuals must be willing to accept vulnerability in order to cooperate with others (Piff et al. Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010).

Low-SES individuals have also been found to have a lower sense of control over their environments, exhibiting attitudes of helplessness about their socio-economic position (Gallo et al. Reference Gallo, Bogart, Vranceanu and Matthews2005; Johnson and Krueger Reference Johnson and Krueger2006; Kraus et al. Reference Kraus, Piff, Mendoza-Denton, Rheinschmidt and Keltner2012). They may believe that there is no benefit to behaving altruistically as it will not change their environment; no matter how they act, people will treat them the same. Therefore, there is no sense in wasting energy being altruistic. Conversely, high-SES individuals with a greater sense of control over their environment may have confidence that there are long-term reciprocal benefits of engaging altruistically. There is much that we do not know about the relationship between SES and altruism. The dimensions of SES need to be teased apart to establish which specific elements contribute towards these relationships. This will then enable us to focus on the mechanisms by which these elements can affect altruistic tendencies.

Using the lost letter technique, we analysed the association between a variety of area-level socio-economic variables (in particular economic resources, education and occupation and crime rate) and willingness to help a stranger. We also implemented a novel, modified lost letter experiment to rectify some limitations inherent in the traditional lost letter experiment.

Methods

Data collection

A total of 600 letters were distributed among 20 different suburbs of Perth, Western Australia (30 letters per suburb). Suburbs were selected based on a number of selection criteria such as being greater than three-quarters suburban, free from any rural qualities and of sufficient size to distribute 30 letters.

Twenty suburbs were selected to exhibit a range of different SES characteristics across economic resources, education and occupation, and crime rates. Suburbs were also selected such that these factors were not strongly correlated. Table S1 (in the Supplementary Information) lists all 20 suburbs selected and their relevant socio-economic statistics.

Within Perth, 300 letters were distributed using the original lost letter design, called the ‘pavement method’. A further 300 letters were distributed using a novel, refined method, called the ‘letterbox method’ (see below). In each of the 20 suburbs, 15 letters were distributed via the pavement method. Letters were stamped, sealed, and addressed to a residential address. Envelopes contained a note to reduce suspicion in case they were opened, and were coded so that their exact drop location could be identified. Letters were placed address-side up on pedestrian paths at evenly distributed, pre-determined locations within each suburb. Letters were distributed on Friday evenings to avoid postmen encountering the letters on weekdays and in good weather to ensure pedestrians would come across them.

In each of the 20 suburbs, 15 letters were also distributed via the new, modified lost letter design (the letterbox method). Letters were addressed and delivered to the letter boxes of residential houses, but featured a false name of someone who did not live there. Letters were designed to appear as though the sender had accidentally used the incorrect address of the recipient or made an error when writing it. Letters featured a sender address on the back of the letter so that if choosing to act altruistically, recipients of the letter could return the letter to the sender. Letterbox letters featured the same content as the pavement letters, and their distribution locations (addresses they were sent to) were selected to be evenly distributed across each suburb.

Measures

Geographical area-level SES was measured using the Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS) Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage (IRSAD; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2013, 2014a). This index consists of two sub-measures: Index of Economic Resources (IER; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2013, 2014b) and Index of Education and Occupation (IEO; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2013, 2014c), which were both used to measure different socio-economic dimensions. IRSAD aggregates the factors indicative of disadvantage (such as low income, level of education and private dwellings with no internet connection) and advantage (including annual income in 9th and 10th deciles and employed people classified as professionals). IER aggregates the factors indicative of disadvantage (such as low income, occupied dwellings with no cards and occupied dwellings paying rent less than AUD215 per week) and advantage (occupied dwellings with four or more bedrooms, occupied dwellings paying a mortgage greater than AUD2800 per month and people with annual household income greater than AUD78,000). IEO aggregates the factors indicative of disadvantage (such as the percentage of people who work in low-skill occupations, percentage of people who are unemployed and the percentage of people aged 15 and over who have no educational attainment) and advantage (percentage of employed people in a high-skill job and percentage of people aged 15 years and over at a university).These indices were obtained for each suburb, and also for smaller areas within each suburb called statistical areas level 1 (SA1; Australian Bureau of Statistics 2011) to test for within-suburb variation.

Crime rate was measured as the total number of crimes per 100,000 people using statistics of crime provided by the Western Australia Police Force from April 2015 to March 2016. Crime data were not available for areas smaller than a suburb, so the crime statistic was the same for the suburb-level and SA1-level analyses.

Ethics

Ethics was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee at The University of Western Australia in accordance with the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research (Reference number RA/4/1/8257).

Data analysis

To determine which explanatory variable best predicted whether a letter would be retuned or not, a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) with binomial error structure and logit link function (multilevel logistic regression) was run with the following fixed effects: letter distribution method (whether the letter was distributed via the pavement or letterbox method), post boxes (the number of post boxes in a suburb), crime rate and IRSAD. The number of post boxes was included to control for the ease of returning a letter across different suburbs. This was determined using an online post box map locator (Australia Post 2016). The suburb itself was included as a random effect to account for random unmeasured effects in each suburb. All data were analysed from both the pavement and letterbox methods. The main model was run twice, once using suburb-level data and once using SA1-level data (Table S2). This was done to determine the relative significance of each data level, and which level should be retained for subsequent analyses. The results were very similar for both data levels, so SA1 data were retained for the rest of the analysis because they provide more specific information for smaller areas.

The effect of SES on letter return rate was explored further by running two additional models with the sub-measures of IRSAD: IER and IEO. The above model was repeated twice, once by replacing the fixed-effect IRSAD with IER, and once by replacing it with IEO. Models with IRSAD, IER and IEO had to be run separately owing to their shared variables and high degree of collinearity (Pearson correlation coefficient for IRSAD and IEO, 0.86; IRSAD and IER, 0.93). Raw IRSAD, IER and IEO variables were used for analysis (not decile values).

GLMMs assume that fixed effects are not collinear. Therefore, as a diagnostic test, collinearity between crime and all other explanatory variables was examined using Pearson correlations (Dormann et al. Reference Dormann, Elith, Bacher, Buchmann, Carl, Carré, Marquéz, Gruber, Lafourcade, Leitão, Münkemüller, McClean, Osborne, Reineking, Schröder, Skidmore, Zurell and Lautenbach2013). None of the correlation coefficients were over 0.6 (crime and IRSAD, r = −0.55; crime and IER, r = −0.56; crime and IEO, r = −0.43). GLMMs also assume that there are significantly more cases/data points than estimated parameters (Harrison et al. Reference Harrison, Donaldson, Correa-Cano, Evans, Fisher, Goodwin, Robinson, Hodgson and Inger2018), and that there are no influential cases. Both of these assumptions were checked and satisfied. Before fitting the models, we z-transformed all quantitative predictor variables to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 to achieve comparable estimates and to increase the likelihood of model convergence (Schielzeth Reference Schielzeth2010). To check the overall significance of the combined set of predictor variables, we ran likelihood ratio tests comparing each full model with a respective null model containing only the intercept and random effects (Dobson Reference Dobson2002; Forstmeier and Schielzeth Reference Forstmeier and Schielzeth2011). None of the two-way interactions between any of the SES variables and crime and post boxes, respectively, were significant; we thus recalculated the models without the interactions. All GLMMs were computed with the lme4 package (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker and Walker2015) in R (R Core Team 2014) version 3.3.1. Wald confidence intervals were computed using the function ‘confint’.

Results

A total of 302 letters (50% of the number distributed) were returned from both the pavement and letterbox methods (Table 1; Table S3). The full IRSAD model was significantly different from the null model (χ 2 = 772.4, p < 0.001). Table 2 and Figure 1 show that letters distributed in areas with higher IRSAD scores were significantly more likely to be returned than letters distributed in areas with lower IRSAD scores. The likelihood of a letter being returned also depended on the method used, with pavement letters being significantly more likely to be returned than letterbox letters (Table 2; Figure 1). Crime rate and the number of post boxes were not significantly related to letter return rates (Table 2). The variance of the random effect (suburb), i.e. the contribution of the random effect to the variation in the response variable, was 0.094 (SD = 0.306). Additional simplified models (without crime and/or post boxes) are presented in Tables S4–S6.

Table 1. The number (and percentage) of returned and not returned letters as a function of their distribution method

Table 2. Generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) output on whether a letter was returned or not showing fixed effects for crime, letter distribution method, Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage (IRSAD) and number of post boxes. AIC: 784.4

Figure 1. Association of socioeconomic advantage with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IRSAD (Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score (in parentheses) represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

A follow-up GLMM with either IER or IEO was run (Table 3). Both models explained variation in the occurrence of prosocial behaviour associated with picking up a letter better than the respective null models (IER. χ 2 = 777.6, p < 0.001; IEO. χ 2 765.5, p < 0.001). IEO was a significant predictor of whether a letter would be returned or not (Figure 2), but IER was not (Figure 3). The variance of the random effect (suburb) was 0.185 (SD = 0.430). The model with IEO also featured a lower AIC value (786.8) than both the model with IER (798.6) and the model with IRSAD (793.6), indicating that the IEO model was a better fit to the data. The distribution method was again significant for both models, with pavement letters more likely to be returned than letterbox letters. The number of post boxes did not influence letter return rates. Crime was not a significant predictor of letter return rates in the IEO model, but was significant in the IER model (Table 3). The variance of the random effect (suburb) was 0.041 (SD = 0.202). Additional simplified models (without crime and/or post boxes) are presented in Tables S7–S12.

Table 3. GLMMs outputs with either Index of Economic Resources (IER) or Index of Education and Occupation (IEO) as main fixed effects and letter return as a binary response term. AIC for IER model, 789.6; AIC for IEO model, 777.5

Figure 2. Association of education/occupation with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IEO (Index of Education and Occupation) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score in parentheses represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

Figure 3. Association of economic resources with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IER (Index of Economic Resources) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score in parentheses represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

Discussion

The current study revealed substantial and systematic variation in altruistic tendencies across urban suburbs of different socio-economic characteristics. This variance appears to be conditioned by the education and occupation level of residents in the suburb, and was not consistently influenced by economic resources or crime rate.

Socio-economic status

Both the original lost letter experiment and the novel, modified letterbox method provided support for the hypothesis that area-level SES (measured by IRSAD) was positively correlated with helping behaviour. This result is in consensus with previous research reporting a link between SES and letter return rates in the lost letter experiment (Brown and Reed Reference Brown and Reed1982; Grueter et al. Reference Grueter, Ingram, Lewisson, Bradford, Taba, Coetzee and Sherwood2016; Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012; Nettle et al. Reference Nettle, Colléony and Cockerill2011; Silva and Mace Reference Silva and Mace2014). Multiple drivers underlying the lower levels of prosociality in low-SES areas are conceivable, for example time constraints resulting from the need to make ends meet (Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012; Lynam et al. Reference Lynam, Caspi, Moffit, Wikström, Loeber and Novak2000), lower sense of control over the environment (e.g. Gallo et al. Reference Gallo, Bogart, Vranceanu and Matthews2005) or – more mundanely – higher tolerance of litter rates (see Khatib et al. Reference Khatib, Daoud and Shwahneh2007).

However, the above finding is in conflict with studies by Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010, Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) who found upper-class individuals to be less prosocial and more unethical in measures such as willingness to cooperate with a game partner and attitudes on charitable donations. Côté et al. (Reference Côté, House and Willer2015) recently showed that higher-income individuals are not more selfish across the board but a tendency to be less generous emerges only under conditions of high economic inequality. Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) suggested that – among others – this was because high-SES people have abundant resources to deal with the downstream costs of unethical behaviour (e.g. money for a speeding fine), while lower-SES individuals may need to be more careful as they incur greater relative consequences for social deviation. Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) proposed that low-SES individuals have a greater interest in the wellbeing of others because it affects their ability to draw resources from them. Thus lower-class individuals’ willingness to engage in altruistic behaviour can be seen a function of economic interdependence. Relatedly, Amir et al. (Reference Amir, Jordan and Rand2018) invoked an uncertainty management framework to account for the greater prosociality observed in economic games among economically deprived children. In this framework, cooperation with social partners and prosociality reflect the adaptive internalization of a risk-mitigating strategy in the face of uncertain returns associated with early life deprivation.

The difference between the results of Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010, Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) and lost letter-based studies could stem from the fact that the former analysed variation at the individual level, whereas the latter examined neighbourhood-level differences (Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012). Perhaps high-SES neighbourhoods foster altruism, yet within any one neighbourhood, the poorer individuals are more altruistic than the wealthier ones (Holland et al. Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012).

Another reason for the difference between our findings and those of Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010, Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) could be that their experiments measured altruistic tendencies towards people in general (no specific group) in a range of environments. In contrast, the lost letter experiment used in this study measured altruistic behaviours within one's own ‘home environment’ (their suburb or street) towards (presumably) members of their own group; individuals who encountered lost letters would have probably assumed that the letter was distributed by a resident when walking through the area.

Lastly, as suggested by Holland et al. (Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012), the experiments used to analyse altruism by Piff et al. (Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010, Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) may be more competitive than the small, cooperative task of returning a lost letter, resulting in different behaviours. For example, upper-class individuals may be more likely than lower-class individuals to return a letter in a cooperative task, but they may also be more likely to deceive another player in a laboratory-based economic game. Future studies should incorporate multiple measures of altruistic behaviour (such as those used by Piff et al. Reference Piff, Kraus, Côté, Cheng and Keltner2010, Reference Piff, Stancato, Côté, Mendoza-Denton and Keltner2012) to determine if the patterns seen in this study are unique to the lost letter experiment.

Socio-economic variables

The principal aim of this study was to disentangle the association of different socio-economic variables with altruistic behaviour. Crime was predicted to reduce altruism by lowering trust, but a suburb's crime rate was largely unrelated to the expression of prosocial behaviour. Only in the model where economic resources were included did crime rate become significant. Therefore, the variance explained by crime rate may be accounted for by other SES characteristics such as education, which was included in all other models. It may also be that crime has a threshold effect and needs to be at a certain rate before it begins to affect peoples’ altruistic tendencies. The suburbs analysed in this study may not have had sufficient crime rates to demonstrate this effect.

Economic resources, as a characteristic of SES, also did not have a significant effect on letter return rate. This suggests that demographic factors such as individuals’ assets, house prices and average household income are not related to suburb-level altruistic behaviours. Holland et al. (Reference Holland, Silva and Mace2012) suggested that low-SES individuals may be too preoccupied with meeting their individual needs to be willing to spend time helping others. This hypothesis suggests that individuals with more economic resources will be better equipped to meet their needs and will, therefore, have more time and energy to engage altruistically with others. The current dataset does not rule out the hypothesis that, when time itself is not a limited resource, people may be more willing to engage in prosocial behaviours. It should be noted that the location of this study does not experience widespread socio-economic deprivation where a great proportion of individuals do not have access to basic needs such as clean water, food and housing. Perhaps this hypothesis may be relevant in more economically deprived contexts where economic resources may influence altruism.

IEO was found to be significantly associated with whether a letter would be returned or not. The effect of IRSAD on letter return rate may largely be explained by the composite variables that it shares with IEO. This finding suggests that the component of SES that affects a neighbourhood's letter return rate is the education and occupation status of individuals in that suburb. To our knowledge this is a novel finding that has not been reported previously. However, along a similar vein, there is one recent study which documented a positive correlation between historical rates of primary education and civic honesty (Cohn et al. Reference Cohn, Maréchal, Tannenbaum and Zünd2019). Because IEO incorporates both education and occupation variables, we cannot distinguish whether both, or just one or the other, of these variables influence altruistic behaviour within a suburb.

This study has isolated education and occupation as the likely leading socio-economic variables behind the often found relationship between SES and letter return rates. However, we still do not fully understand the mechanism behind this link. We do not know what aspects of education and occupation status may lead individuals within a suburb to behave more altruistically. Education and occupation may also be associated with a third variable that may be driving the patterns in the results. For example, individuals who have achieved a high education level or who are in high-status jobs are more likely to possess greater cognitive abilities (Schmidt and Hunter Reference Schmidt and Hunter2004; Strenze Reference Strenze2007). It may thus be possible that the significant effect of IEO on letter return rate reflects an underlying effect of cognitive ability. Previous studies in behavioural economics have shown a link between cognitive ability and altruistic behaviour (Jones Reference Jones2008). Cognitive ability has been found to be negatively correlated with a preference for immediate rewards and impulsivity (Jensen Reference Jensen1998; de Wit et al. Reference de Wit, Flory, Acheson, McCloskey and Manuck2007). Cognition in more stressful and harsh environments associated with lower SES may be focused more on temporal discounting and lower levels of self-control (Coall et al. Reference Coall, Dickins, Nettle and Poiani2012; Frankenhuis et al. Reference Frankenhuis, Panchanathan and Nettle2016; Mullainathan and Shafir Reference Mullainathan and Shafir2013; Sheehy-Skeffington and Rea Reference Sheehy-Skeffington and Rea2017), conditions that discourage altruistic behaviour (Osiński et al. Reference Osiński, Karbowski and Rusek2017). It is important to note that extrapolating from SES at a relatively crude area-level analysis to individual differences in cognitive ability (and thus altruism) is problematic. The relationship between these factors and education is probably more complex, and dependent on many factors (e.g. opportunity, value placed on education, etc.).

An evolutionary mechanism underlying the finding that education and occupation are the primary drivers of prosocial behaviour may be that educated people have more opportunities to learn, to be taught and to receive feedback and thus are more likely to adopt or maintain cultural norms of prosociality. Individual behavioural decisions (as to whether to act proscocially) are influenced by expectations of the behaviour of others in the local social environment (Bichierri and Xiao Reference Bichierri and Xiao2009). In turn, these decisions also influence the local social environment, by conveying to others information about local norms of cooperative behaviour (cf. Schroeder et al. Reference Schroeder, Pepper and Nettle2014).

Modified lost-letter experiment

The novel letterbox method incorporated in this study featured a significantly lower return rate compared with the original pavement method. Both methods, however, exhibited the same SES patterns in the data. One explanation for the differing return rates is that ignoring a letterbox letter ends all future possibilities for the letter to be returned, but pavement letters may be picked up by someone else (however, one could also argue that receiving a lost letter in someone's letterbox increases the recipient's pressure to do something about it). Another possible explanation is that returning a pavement letter may incur a smaller cost in terms of time and effort, because an individual could already be heading in the direction of a post box, compared who a letterbox recipient who would have to make a separate trip. Furthermore, individuals may behave more prosocially when encountering pavement letters because there is a chance that their actions are being observed by bystanders and influence their reputation (sensu Raihani and Bshary, Reference Raihani and Bshary2015). Additionally, there remains the possibility that letterbox recipients may have uncertainty about what to do with the wrongly addressed letter.

The similar socio-economic patterns found in the results from both methods suggest that the letterbox method may be a useful alternative to the pavement method as it may not be as susceptible to some of the potentially confounding variables such as non-residents encountering the letters and differing rates of pedestrian foot traffic in different neighbourhoods. The letterbox method has additional advantages that should be considered for future experiments. The method enables the letter to be distributed at any time, unlike pavement letters, which must be distributed on wind- and rain-free evenings. The letterbox method also eliminates the lengthy process of distributing letters by hand and provides easy access to remote or rural areas. Additionally, the letterbox method allows for more letters to be distributed in any given area, owing to the elimination of the possibility of individuals encountering multiple letters while walking through a neighbourhood.

Cultural group selection and prosociality

Cultural group selection theory posits that groups whose members engage altruistically with each other are more successful in intergroup competition than groups whose members lack such locally stable cooperative cultural norms (Henrich Reference Henrich2004; Richerson et al. Reference Richerson, Baldini, Bell, Demps, Frost, Hillis, Mathew, Newton and Naar2016). However, the great variation in altruistic tendencies exhibited by the different suburbs suggests that cultural group selection does not function at the scale of the city. Instead, we may see large populations splitting up into smaller sub-groups with their own set of altruistic norms which may be the result of cultural group selection operating on this smaller scale. However, since populations of city suburbs are not natural groups but administrative divisions, it is unclear if these are subject to cultural group selection. Alternatively, variation in altruism attributed to different cultural norms could in fact reflect individual adaptations to different environments with varying levels of socio-economic harshness (Mace and Silva Reference Mace and Silva2016).

Conclusions

This study has provided new insight into human altruistic behaviour and has reinforced that altruistic behaviour is contingent on contextual factors. Specifically, between-suburb variation in altruism was found to co-vary with SES. Education and occupation were the components of SES found to increase the prevalence of unconditional altruism within a suburb. This relationship may be due to differences in individual cognitive ability and self-control and the possibility that education positively correlates with an individual's ability or willingness to adhere to local norms of prosociality. Our study also offers a new variant of the lost letter experiment, the letterbox method, which is advantageous in reducing confounds and easing the logistics of fieldwork. Knowledge about the manifestation of human altruism and community norms across gradients of SES could also have practical implications for charitable organizations and local government policy.

Supplementary Material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2019.16.

Acknowledgments

We thank Rebecca Sear and two reviewers for helpful comments on this manuscript.

Author contributions

GW, DC and CCG designed the study; GW collected data; GW and CCG analysed data; GW, DC and CCG wrote the paper.

Financial support

This work was supported by the University of Western Australia.

Conflicts of interest

We declare no conflicts of interests.

Data availability

The data used in this study are available as Supplementary Files.

References

Alesina, A and Ferrara, EL (2002) Who trusts others? Journal of Public Economics 85, 207234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, RD (1987) The Biology of Moral Systems. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Allen-Arave, W, Gurven, M and Hill, K (2008) Reciprocal altruism, rather than kin selection, maintains nepotistic food transfers on an Ache reservation. Evolution and Human Behavior 29, 305318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amir, D, Jordan, MR and Rand, DG (2018) An uncertainty management perspective on long-run impacts of adversity: the influence of childhood socioeconomic status on risk, time, and social preferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 79, 217226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
André, J-B and Morin, O (2011) Questioning the cultural evolution of altruism. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 24, 25312542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2011) Statistical Area Level 1 (SA1), Australian Government. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/0/7CAFD05E79EB6F81CA257801000C64CD?opendocument/ (accessed 9 May 2016).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2013) Census of population and housing: Socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA), Australia, 2011, catalogue no. 2033.0.55.001, ABS, Canberra. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/[email protected]/DetailsPage/2033.0.55.0012011?OpenDocument (accessed 7 May 2016).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014a) IRSAD, Australian Government. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/2033.0.55.001main+features100042011/ (accessed 9 May 2016).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014b) IER, Australian Government. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/2033.0.55.001main+features10006201/ (accessed 9 May 2016).Google Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014c) IEO, Australian Government. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/2033.0.55.001main+features100072011 (accessed 09 May 2016).Google Scholar
Australia Post (2016) Addressing an envelope. Retrieved from http://auspost.com.au/education/letterwriting/students/addressing.html (accessed 7 June 2016).Google Scholar
Axelrod, R and Hamilton, WD (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science 211, 13901396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D, Maechler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bichierri, C and Xiao, E (2009) Do the right thing: but only if others do so. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 22, 191208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, CE and Reed, MD (1982) Race, cost, and car-status: interacting variables using the lost-letter technique. Psychological Reports 51, 303308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnham, TC and Johnson, DD (2005) The biological and evolutionary logic of human cooperation. Analyse and Kritik 27, 113135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, S, Dee, TS, Wing Tse, C and Yu, L (2016) Be a good samaritan to a good samaritan: field evidence of other-regarding preferences in China. China Economic Review 41, 2333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chisholm, J (1999) Attachment and time preference: relations between early stress and sexual behaviour in a sample of American university women. Human Nature 10, 5183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chudek, M and Henrich, J (2011) Culture–gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 218226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chudek, M, Zhao, W and Henrich, J (2013) Culture–gene coevolution, large-scale cooperation and the shaping of human social psychology. In Joyce, R, Sterelny, K and Calcott, B (eds), Signaling, Commitment, and Emotion (pp. 425457). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coall, DA, Dickins, TE and Nettle, D (2012) Antecedents of teenage pregnancy: using an evolutionary perspective in the search for mechanisms. In Poiani, A (ed.), Pragmatic Evolution: Applications of Evolutionary Theory (pp. 167187). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cohn, A, Maréchal, MA, Tannenbaum, D and Zünd, CL (2019) Civic honesty around the globe. Science 365, 7073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Côté, S, House, J and Willer, R (2015) High economic inequality leads higher-income individuals to be less generous. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 112, 1583815843.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, OS, Price, ME and Price, JG (2008) Patience is a virtue: cooperative people have lower discount rates. Personality and Individual Differences 44, 780785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Wit, H, Flory, JD, Acheson, A, McCloskey, M and Manuck, SB (2007) IQ and nonplanning impulsivity are independently associated with delay discounting in middle-aged adults. Personality and Individual Differences 42, 111121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, A (2002) An Introduction to Generalized Linear Models. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman, Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
Dormann, CF, Elith, J, Bacher, S, Buchmann, C, Carl, G, Carré, G, Marquéz, JR, Gruber, B, Lafourcade, B, Leitão, PJ, Münkemüller, T, McClean, C, Osborne, PE, Reineking, B, Schröder, B, Skidmore, AK, Zurell, D and Lautenbach, S (2013) Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 36, 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E and Henrich, J (2003) Is strong reciprocity a maladaptation? On the evolutionary foundations of human altruism. In Hammerstein, P (ed.), Genetic and Cultural evolution of Cooperation (pp. 5582) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Forstmeier, W and Schielzeth, H (2011) Cryptic multiple hypotheses testing in linear models: overestimated effect sizes and the winner's curse. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology 65, 4755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, WE, Panchanathan, K and Nettle, D (2016) Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments. Current Opinion in Psychology 7, 7680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederick, S (2005) Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives 19, 2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallo, LC, Bogart, LM, Vranceanu, A-M and Matthews, KA (2005) Socioeconomic status, resources, psychological experiences, and emotional responses: a test of the reserve capacity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88, 386399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gintis, H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 206, 169179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glaeser, L, Ponzetto, GA and Shleifer, A (2007) Why does democracy need education? Journal of Economic Growth 12, 7799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grueter, CC, Ingram, JA, Lewisson, JW, Bradford, OR, Taba, M, Coetzee, RE and Sherwood, MA (2016) Human altruistic tendencies vary with both the costliness of selfless acts and socioeconomic status. PeerJ 4, e2610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7, 1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, XA, Donaldson, L, Correa-Cano, ME, Evans, J, Fisher, DN, Goodwin, CED, Robinson, BS, Hodgson, DJ and Inger, R (2018) A brief introduction to mixed effects modelling and multi-model inference in ecology. PeerJ 6, e4794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J (2004) Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 53, 335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J and Boyd, R (2001) Why people punish defectors: weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208, 7989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J, Boyd, R, Bowles, S, Camerer, C, Fehr, E, Gintis, H, McElreath, R, Alvard, M, Barr, A et al. (2005) ‘Economic man’ in cross-cultural perspective: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, 795855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J, McElreath, R, Barr, A, Ensminger, J, Barrett, C, Bolyanatz, A, Cardenas, JC, Gurven, M, Gwako, E et al. (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312, 17671770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J, Ensminger, J, McElreath, R, Barr, A, Barrett, C, Bolyanatz, A, Cardenas, JC, Gurven, M, Gwako, E et al. (2010) Markets, religion, community size and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327, 14801484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermann, B, Thöni, C and Gächter, S (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319, 13621367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holland, J, Silva, AS and Mace, R (2012) Lost letter measure of variation in altruistic behaviour in 20 neighbourhoods. PLoS ONE 7, e43294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, AR (1998) The g factor: The science of mental ability. Politics and the Life Sciences 17, 230232.Google Scholar
Johnson, W and Krueger, RF (2006) How money buys happiness: genetic and environmental processes linking finances and life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 90, 680691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, G (2008) Are smarter groups more cooperative? Evidence from Prisoner's Dilemma experiments, 1959–2003. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organisation 68, 489497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khatib, IA, Daoud, R and Shwahneh, H (2007) Influence of socio-economic factors on street litter generation in the Middle East: effects of education level, age, and type of residence. Waste Management & Research 25, 363370.Google Scholar
Kraus, MW, Piff, PK, Mendoza-Denton, R, Rheinschmidt, ML and Keltner, D (2012) Social class, solopsism, and contextualism: How the rich are different from the poor. Psychological Review 119, 546572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamba, S and Mace, R (2011) Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 1442614430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, RV, Norenzayan, A and Philbrick, K (2001) Cross-cultural differences in helping strangers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32, 543560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynam, DR, Caspi, A, Moffit, TE, Wikström, PO, Loeber, R and Novak, S (2000) The interaction between impulsivity and neighborhood context on offending: the effects of impulsivity are stronger in poorer neighborhoods. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 109, 563574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mace, R and Silva, AS (2016) The role of cultural group selection in explaining human cooperation is a hard case to prove. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39, e45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maskin, E and Fudenberg, D (1986) The folk theorem in repeated games with discounting or with incomplete information. Econometrica 54, 533554.Google Scholar
Milgram, S, Mann, L and Harter, S (1965) The lost-letter technique: a tool of social research. Public Opinion Quarterly 29, 437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molleman, L, Quiñones, AE and Weissing, FJ (2013) Cultural evolution of cooperation: the interplay between forms of social learning and group selection. Evolution and Human Behaviour 34, 342349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mullainathan, S and Shafir, E (2013) Scarcity: Why Having too Little Means so much. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Nettle, D, Colléony, A and Cockerill, M (2011) Variation in cooperative behaviour within a single city. PLoS ONE 6, e26922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, MA and Sigmund, K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393, 573577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osiński, J, Karbowski, A and Rusek, J (2017) The relationship between self-control and altruism in the light of discounting literature. Polish Psychological Bulletin 48, 433439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piff, PK, Kraus, MW, Côté, S, Cheng, BH and Keltner, D (2010) Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 99, 771784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piff, PK, Stancato, DM, Côté, S, Mendoza-Denton, R and Keltner, D (2012) Higher social class predicts increased unethical behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 109, 40864091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, NJ and Bshary, R (2015) Why humans might help strangers. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 9, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Richerson, PJ, Boyd, R and Henrich, J (2003) Cultural evolution of human cooperation. In Hammerstein, P (ed.), Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (pp. 357388). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P, Baldini, R, Bell, AV, Demps, K, Frost, K, Hillis, V, Mathew, S, Newton, EK, Naar, N et al. (2016) Cultural group selection plays an essential role in explaining human cooperation: A sketch of the evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39, e30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roth, AE, Prasnikar, V, Okuni-Fujiwara, M and Zamir, S (1991) Bargaining and market behavior in Jerusalem, Ljubljana, Pittsburgh, and Tokyo: an experimental study. The American Economic Review 81, 10681095.Google Scholar
Schielzeth, H (2010) Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 1, 103113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, FL and Hunter, J (2004) General mental ability in the world of work: occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86, 162173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeder, KB, Pepper, GV and Nettle, D (2014) Local norms of cheating and the cultural evolution of crime and punishment: a study of two urban neighborhoods. PeerJ 2, e450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segal, NL and Hershberger, SL (1999) Cooperation and competition between twins: findings from a Prisoner's Dilemma game. Evolution and Human Behavior 20, 2951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheehy-Skeffington, J and Rea, J (2017) How poverty affects people's decision-making processes. Joseph Rowntree Foundation. Retrieved from https://www.jrf.org.uk/report/how-poverty-affects-peoples-decision-making-processes.Google Scholar
Silva, AS and Mace, R (2014) Cooperation and conflict: field experiments in Northern Ireland. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 281, 20141435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strenze, T (2007) Intelligence and socioeconomic success: A meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence 35, 401426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46, 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, JT and Pleeter, S (2001) The personal discount rate: Evidence from military downsizing programs. American Economic Review 91, 3353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. The number (and percentage) of returned and not returned letters as a function of their distribution method

Figure 1

Table 2. Generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) output on whether a letter was returned or not showing fixed effects for crime, letter distribution method, Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage (IRSAD) and number of post boxes. AIC: 784.4

Figure 2

Figure 1. Association of socioeconomic advantage with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IRSAD (Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score (in parentheses) represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

Figure 3

Table 3. GLMMs outputs with either Index of Economic Resources (IER) or Index of Education and Occupation (IEO) as main fixed effects and letter return as a binary response term. AIC for IER model, 789.6; AIC for IEO model, 777.5

Figure 4

Figure 2. Association of education/occupation with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IEO (Index of Education and Occupation) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score in parentheses represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Association of economic resources with altruism. The percentage of returned letters for the pavement and letterbox methods by the decile mid-point of the IER (Index of Economic Resources) score of the SA1 area in which they were distributed. Note: the average raw score in parentheses represents the mid-point of the decile; it was calculated by averaging all raw scores that fell within each decile.

Supplementary material: File

Westlake et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S12

Download Westlake et al. supplementary material(File)
File 25.8 KB