Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T13:48:09.091Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Timing sensitivity of prenatal cortisol exposure and neurocognitive development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 November 2024

Sean R. Womack*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Brown University Warren Alpert Medical School, Providence, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA
Hannah R. Murphy
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA
Molly S. Arnold
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester School of Arts and Sciences, Rochester, NY, USA Wynne Center for Family Research, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Zoe T. Duberstein
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Rochester School of Arts and Sciences, Rochester, NY, USA Wynne Center for Family Research, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Meghan Best
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA
Xing Qiu
Affiliation:
Biostatistics and Computational Biology, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Richard K. Miller
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA
Emily S. Barrett
Affiliation:
Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Rutgers School of Public Health, Piscataway, NJ, USA Environmental and Occupational Health Sciences Institute, Piscataway, NJ, USA
Thomas G. O’Connor
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA Department of Obstetrics & Gynecology, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, NY, USA Department of Psychology, University of Rochester School of Arts and Sciences, Rochester, NY, USA Wynne Center for Family Research, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA Department of Neuroscience, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Sean R. Womack; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Prenatal glucocorticoid exposure has been negatively associated with infant neurocognitive outcomes. However, questions about developmental timing effects across gestation remain. Participants were 253 mother-child dyads who participated in a prospective cohort study recruited in the first trimester of pregnancy. Diurnal cortisol was measured in maternal saliva samples collected across a single day within each trimester of pregnancy. Children (49.8% female) completed the Bayley Mental Development Scales, Third Edition at 6, 12, and 24 months and completed three observational executive function tasks at 24 months. Structural equation models adjusting for sociodemographic covariates were used to test study hypotheses. There was significant evidence for timing sensitivity. First-trimester diurnal cortisol (area under the curve) was negatively associated with cognitive and language development at 12 months and poorer inhibition at 24 months. Second-trimester cortisol exposure was negatively associated with language scores at 24 months. Third-trimester cortisol positively predicted performance in shifting between task rules (set shifting) at 24 months. Associations were not reliably moderated by child sex. Findings suggest that neurocognitive development is sensitive to prenatal glucocorticoid exposure as early as the first trimester and underscore the importance of assessing developmental timing in research on prenatal exposures for child health outcomes.

Type
Regular Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The prenatal programming hypothesis states that maternal experiences during pregnancy can shape offspring biology and behavior, including disease onset later in life (Barker, Reference Barker1998; Gluckman & Hanson, Reference Gluckman and Hanson2005). A core component of the model is the premise that the fetus adapts to prenatal exposures to maximize evolutionary fitness in a particular postnatal environment (Barker, Reference Barker1998). Although initially proposed in the context of nutritional risk for cardiovascular and metabolic-related outcomes (Barker, Reference Barker1998; Gluckman & Hanson, Reference Gluckman and Hanson2005), this hypothesis has been expanded to include multiple exposures, such as stress physiology, and additional outcomes, including neurodevelopment and non-infectious diseases (Bock et al., Reference Bock, Wainstock, Braun and Segal2015; Zijlmans et al., Reference Zijlmans, Riksen-Walraven and de Weerth2015).

The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and its key products corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH), ACTH, and cortisol, which, among many other biological functions, help regulate the body’s stress response, have been well described in pediatric and adult samples (Gunnar & Quevedo, Reference Gunnar and Quevedo2007; Howland et al., Reference Howland, Sandman and Glynn2017; Lupien et al., Reference Lupien, McEwen, Gunnar and Heim2009). In response to environmental, interpersonal, or psychological stress, CRH is released from the hypothalamus, which stimulates the release of ACTH from the pituitary gland (Karin et al., Reference Karin, Raz, Tendler, Bar, Korem Kohanim, Milo and Alon2020). ACTH activates the adrenal gland to produce the glucocorticoid cortisol, which is the end product of the HPA axis (Karin et al., Reference Karin, Raz, Tendler, Bar, Korem Kohanim, Milo and Alon2020). Cortisol concentrations in circulation follow a natural circadian rhythm characterized by a peak at the transition from sleep to waking, a gradual decline throughout the day, and an increase during sleep (Weitzman et al., Reference Weitzman, Fukushima, Nogeire, Roffwarg, Gallagher and Hellman1971). Additionally, maternal cortisol concentrations naturally increase over the course of pregnancy in response to placental secretion of CRH into the bloodstream (Duthie et al., Reference Duthie and Reynolds2013; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Emanuel, Frim and Majzoub1988).

To protect the developing fetus from excessive exposure to maternal cortisol, the placenta produces 11 β-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 (11β-HSD2), an enzyme that serves as a partial barrier to maternal cortisol by converting active cortisol into inactive cortisone (Schoof et al., Reference Schoof, Girstl, Frobenius, Kirschbaum, Repp, Knerr, Rascher and Dotsch2001). In general, placental production of 11β-HSD2 mirrors the increase in maternal cortisol across pregnancy; 11β-HSD2 production is lowest in early pregnancy and increases throughout gestation (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Emanuel, Frim and Majzoub1988). At the end of gestation, maternal cortisol levels are, on average, 3–5 times higher than pre-pregnancy levels (Sandman et al., Reference Sandman, Glynn, Schetter, Wadhwa, Garite, Chicz-DeMet and Hobel2006), and placental production of 11β-HSD2 drops (Schoof et al., Reference Schoof, Girstl, Frobenius, Kirschbaum, Repp, Knerr, Rascher and Dotsch2001). The resulting increase in fetal exposure to cortisol at the end of gestation is thought to promote the maturation of vital organs and the central nervous system (Drake et al., Reference Drake, Tang and Nyirenda2007; Trejo et al., Reference Trejo, Cuchillo, Machín and Rúa2000).

Notwithstanding the important functional role of glucocorticoids in healthy fetal development, excessive exposure may have a negative impact on fetal development and disrupt postnatal development (Lautarescu et al., Reference Lautarescu, Craig and Glover2020; Reference O’Donnell, O’Connor and GloverO’ Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, O’Connor and Glover2009). Maternal stress and anxiety in pregnancy have attracted research interest, in part, because of their impact on patterns of cortisol production throughout the day (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022; Vergara-Lopez et al., Reference Vergara-Lopez, Bublitz, Mercado, Ziobrowski, Gomez and Stroud2021) and throughout pregnancy (Bublitz et al., Reference Bublitz, Parade and Stroud2014; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022; Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Kessler, Buss, Miller, Grobman, Keenan-Devlin, Borders and Adam2021). As placental production of 11β-HSD2 is lowest in the first trimester (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Emanuel, Frim and Majzoub1988), the developing fetus and core aspects of brain development (e.g., formation of the neocortex, neuronal migration, and establishment of early neuronal connections (Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019; Vlasova et al., Reference Vlasova, Iosif, Ryan, Funk, Murai, Chen, Lesh, Rowland, Bennett, Hogrefe, Maddock, Gandal, Geschwind, Schumann, Van de Water, McAllister, Carter, Styner, Amaral and Bauman2021) may be particularly vulnerable to elevations in maternal cortisol levels. Furthermore, in animal models, maternal experimental exposure to synthetic glucocorticoids has been found to downregulate placental 11β-HSD2 production (Cuffe et al., Reference Cuffe, O’Sullivan, Simmons, Anderson and Moritz2012; Wieczorek et al., Reference Wieczorek, Perani, Nixon, Constancia, Sandovici, Zazara, Leone, Zhang, Arck and Solano2019) and in humans, maternal prenatal anxiety has been linked to downregulation of 11β-HSD2 gene expression (Reference O’Donnell, Jensen, Freeman, Khalife, O’Connor and GloverO’Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Jensen, Freeman, Khalife, O’Connor and Glover2012). Therefore, elevations in maternal cortisol early in pregnancy may impact placental production of 11β-HSD2 long-term, leading to increased fetal exposure to glucocorticoids at later gestational stages.

Consistent with the prenatal programming hypothesis, several studies have found that higher levels of prenatal cortisol exposure are associated with poorer neurocognitive development (Bergman et al., Reference Bergman, Sarkar, Glover and O’Connor2010; Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010; LeWinn et al., Reference LeWinn, Stroud, Molnar, Ware, Koenen and Buka2009; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Fearon, Rice, Ciceri, Molteni and Frigerio2020). Developmental programming models make broad predictions about the timing sensitivity by focusing on in utero exposures. However, few studies of glucocorticoid exposure and neurocognitive development have focused on the more specific developmental question of timing across gestation, and the few that have produced inconsistent findings in terms of the timing of risk sensitivity (Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010; Huizink et al., Reference Huizink, Robles de Medina, Mulder, Visser and Buitelaar2003). For example, hair cortisol levels in the second trimester (thought to reflect first-trimester exposures) have been unrelated or positively related to cognitive and language development in infancy, controlling for exposure at subsequent trimesters (Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Mariño-Narvaez et al., Reference Mariño-Narvaez, Puertas-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez and Peralta-Ramírez2023). Alternatively, Huizink and colleagues (2003) found maternal waking salivary cortisol levels in late gestation (37–38 weeks), but not early (15 – 17 weeks) or mid-gestation (27 – 28 weeks) to predict lower Bayley mental development scores at 3 months. In contrast, higher afternoon salivary cortisol levels in early gestation (15.1 weeks) were associated with a slower trajectory of cognitive development between 3 and 12 months, whereas higher afternoon salivary cortisol concentrations in late gestation (37 weeks) were associated with faster cognitive growth trajectories and higher 12 months cognitive scores (Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010). Exposure to elevated maternal cortisol levels in the third trimester has been linked to higher cognitive ability scores in some samples (Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Head, Buss and Sandman2017) and lower cognitive scores in other samples (LeWinn et al., Reference LeWinn, Stroud, Molnar, Ware, Koenen and Buka2009; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Fearon, Rice, Ciceri, Molteni and Frigerio2020).

Inconsistencies in the literature regarding the role of gestational timing may be explained by methodological limitations or analytic differences in measuring and analyzing cortisol (Zijlmans et al., Reference Zijlmans, Riksen-Walraven and de Weerth2015). For example, few studies testing timing hypotheses have included first-trimester measurements. Furthermore, the limited research including first-trimester assessments has relied on hair samples (Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Mariño-Narvaez et al., Reference Mariño-Narvaez, Puertas-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez and Peralta-Ramírez2023), which correlate only moderately with salivary cortisol measurements and may begin to degrade after one month, potentially introducing noise into estimated levels over longer time periods (Sugaya et al., Reference Sugaya, Izawa, Ogawa, Shirotsuki and Nomura2020). Other studies of gestational timing effects have used limited assessment models, for example, samples collected at only one timepoint in the day, which will not provide a detailed or thorough index of fetal exposure throughout the day.

The current study advances research on developmental or gestational timing effects of cortisol on child neurodevelopment in several ways. First, we include a detailed assessment of prenatal maternal cortisol at each trimester: we calculate cortisol area under the curve with respect to the ground (AUCg) from five diurnal saliva samples starting in the first trimester of pregnancy. Cortisol AUCg reflects the total maternal cortisol output during a period of time (Khoury et al., Reference Khoury, Gonzalez, Levitan, Pruessner, Chopra, Basile, Masellis, Goodwill and Atkinson2015) and provides an estimation of total fetal glucocorticoid exposure throughout a day.

Second, leveraging the cortisol assessments across gestation, we provide a strict test of the timing sensitivity. A timing sensitivity model proposes that there are particular gestational periods where fetal exposure has a greater or diminished impact on neurocognitive development. In contrast, a “general exposure” model would predict that cortisol exposure is similarly/equally predictive of neurocognitive outcomes across gestation. A key requirement in these studies is to account for stability in cortisol levels across gestation, which has not been incorporated into prior analyses, but which has been shown in several commonly used cortisol measures (e.g., cortisol awakening response, diurnal slope, AUCg; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022). For example, the multiple regression approach of including all measures of prenatal maternal cortisol as simultaneous predictors assumes that repeated cortisol measures are independent. Hierarchical linear modeling approaches can identify the impact of the rate of change in maternal cortisol across pregnancy on neurocognitive development but do not address a fundamental question of timing sensitivity, which requires isolating the unique contribution of cortisol at each gestational timepoint. This study makes a novel contribution to the literature by applying simplex (autoregressive) models to isolate the independent contribution of maternal cortisol AUCg at each trimester, and we compare this model to a general exposure model in which the independent contributions of maternal cortisol at each trimester are constrained to contribute equally to neurocognitive development.

Third, we advance this line of research by examining multiple neurodevelopmental phenotypes across multiple occasions of measurement in the child’s first two years. The “programming” model proposes that prenatal exposure effects would not be transient but would instead persist. Longer-term outcomes of prenatal maternal stress, anxiety, and depression on child psychopathology have been reported (Reference O’Donnell, Glover, Barker and O.’ConnorO’ Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Glover, Barker and O.’Connor2014). However, few studies examine neurodevelopmental outcomes on multiple occasions. Much of the previous research on cortisol exposure and cognitive outcomes has focused on infancy (Bergman et al., Reference Bergman, Sarkar, Glover and O’Connor2010; Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010; Huizink et al., Reference Huizink, Robles de Medina, Mulder, Visser and Buitelaar2003), with fewer assessing neurodevelopmental outcomes in later developmental stages (Buss et al., Reference Buss, Davis, Hobel and Sandman2011; LeWinn et al., Reference LeWinn, Stroud, Molnar, Ware, Koenen and Buka2009). An emphasis on cognitive development in early infancy may contribute to inconsistencies in extant findings as cognitive assessments within the first year are typically not strongly associated with cognitive abilities in later developmental stages (Bishop et al., Reference Bishop, Cherny, Corley, Plomin, DeFries and Hewitt2003; Womack et al., Reference Womack, Beam, Davis, Finkel and Turkheimer2022). Measurements of cognitive ability stabilize over the second year of life, and cognitive abilities in toddlerhood are moderately to highly stable into adolescence (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Miyake, Robinson and Hewitt2011; Yu et al., Reference Yu, McCoach, Gottfried and Gottfried2018). In the current study, we include assessments of neurocognitive development through the first two years, and include multiple measures of neurocognitive development including cognitive ability language, and executive functioning.

Finally, a further consideration for research on prenatal cortisol exposure and child neurocognitive development is sex and gender differences. Human and animal studies suggest that males and females may respond differently to prenatal stress exposure (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Devick, Coull, Cowell, Askowitz, Goldson, Wright and Wright2019; Glover & Hill, Reference Glover and Hill2012; Graham et al., Reference Graham, Rasmussen, Entringer, Ward, Rudolph, Gilmore, Styner, Wadhwa, Fair and Buss2019). Findings from animal models suggest that males are more likely than females to show learning and memory impairments following exposure to prenatal stress (Glover & Hill, Reference Glover and Hill2012), but there are comparatively few empirical examples for neurocognitive outcomes in children (Campbell et al., Reference Campbell, Devick, Coull, Cowell, Askowitz, Goldson, Wright and Wright2019). We examine if child gender moderates the association between prenatal cortisol and neurocognitive outcomes.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 253 mother-child dyads recruited as a part of the Understanding Pregnancy Signals and Infant Development study, a prospective longitudinal pregnancy cohort study conducted in Rochester, New York (O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Best, Brunner, Ciesla, Cunning, Kapula, Kautz, Khoury, Macomber, Meng, Miller, Murphy, Salafia, Sefair, Serrano and Barrett2021). The cohort is a part of the Environmental Influences on Child Health Outcomes program (Knapp et al., Reference Knapp, Kress, Parker, Page, McArthur, Gachigi, Alshawabkeh, Aschner, Bastain, Breton, Bendixsen, Brennan, Bush, Buss, Camargo, Catellier, Cordero, Croen, Dabelea and Jacobson2023). Mothers were recruited during the first trimester of pregnancy from outpatient obstetric clinics affiliated with the university. Recruitment spanned from December 2015 to April 2019. To be eligible, mothers had to be 18 years or older, at less than 14 weeks gestation, carrying a singleton, and able to communicate in English. Mothers with a history of psychotic illness, known substance abuse problems, and major endocrine disorders were not eligible. The study was approved by the local IRB; all participants provided signed consent. Of the 326 mothers initially recruited, 294 (90%) were retained through birth. Only full-term infants were included in follow-up assessments after birth. Families that were missing prenatal cortisol measurements and postnatal assessments at 6, 12, and 24 months were not included in analyses (n = 41). Thus, the study sample included participants with at least one cortisol or cognitive measurement.

Independent variable: salivary cortisol samples

Participants provided salivary cortisol samples at five timepoints across a single typical day once per each trimester. We employed the standard passive drool protocol and the timepoints suggested by the MacArthur Network (MacArthur research network on socioeconomic status and health, http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/research/allostatic/notebook/salivarycort.html). Samples were collected at wake and then at + 45 min, +2 hr, +8 hr, and +12 hr after wake-up. Time of collection and day of collection was recorded for every sample (see Table 1 for mean collection times for each sample). Detailed instructions were provided to avoid collection within 30 min of eating, drinking, brushing teeth. Collection, storage, and assay procedures followed established protocol and employed standard commercial kits (Salimetrics, LLC). The assay utilized a capture antibody-coated plate and involved competition with cortisol and horseradish peroxidase-conjugated anti-cortisol. The plate was incubated in the dark, and the reaction was stopped using stop solution. Spectrophotometry at 450 nm with a secondary correction filter at 490 – 492 nm was employed to measure cortisol levels. Quality and accuracy were ensured with checks for R2 value, duplicate CV%, and control values within kit ranges, resulting in average intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation of 2.40% and 11.75%, respectively. Saliva samples were frozen at − 80 °C until analysis. AUCg values with reference to the ground were calculated using a trapezoidal formula (Pruessner et al., Reference Pruessner, Kirschbaum, Meinlschmid and Hellhammer2003). AUCg was selected over other indicators of cortisol output (e.g., awakening response, diurnal slope) as it represents an estimate of total maternal cortisol output (and thus total potential fetal exposure, which is of greatest interest; Fekedulegn et al., Reference Fekedulegn, Andrew, Burchfiel, Violanti, Hartley, Charles and Miller2007). AUCg calculations were restricted to participants who had at least four viable daily cortisol values including the waking sample (other laboratory and data quality information is provided in Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022).

Outcome variables: mental development and executive functioning

Mental development

Infant cognitive abilities were assessed by trained research assistants at approximately 6, 12, and 24 months using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Third Edition (BSID-III) (Bayley, Reference Bayley2006). The BSID-III yields a standardized score for cognitive and language development. The cognitive scale assesses emerging play skills, memory, and information processing; language scale assesses expressive and receptive language skills (Bayley, Reference Bayley2006). Scores on the Bayley are standardized to have a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15.

Executive functioning

Executive functioning was assessed at 24 months from performance on three tasks. The first task, snack delay (Kochanska et al., Reference Kochanska, Murray and Harlan2000), is a task of inhibition in which a trained examiner placed an M&M under a cup and told the child that they could eat the M&M after the examiner rang a bell. A total of 6 trials were administered with varying pause lengths from immediate to 30 s. A trial is considered successful if the child waits the allotted amount of time before consuming an M&M. The task is scored by summing the total number of successful trials. Spin the pots is a task of working memory (Hughes & Ensore, Reference Hughes and Ensor2005). A trained examiner placed stickers in six of eight uniquely colored cups on a lazy susan, covered the cups, and spun the lazy susan. The child was then asked to select a cup with a sticker in it. As soon as a cup was chosen by the child and the sticker was removed, the cup was then returned to the tray. The administrator then covered the cups again with the scarf and the next trial began. A trial was considered successful if the child was able to accurately recall which cups held stickers. The score reflected the total number of successful trials divided by the total trials administered. Reverse categorization is a task of set shifting in which a child first learns a simple rule, followed by a rule reversal (Carlson, Reference Carlson2012). During the learning trial, the child was taught to sort toys into two different colored bins. The sorting rule was then reversed for the rule reversal trials. The task was scored by the total number of toys correctly sorted during the rule reversal trial divided by the number of toys available. For all of the executive functioning tasks, higher scores indicated better performance on the task.

Data integrity

Video recordings of the Bayley and executive functioning assessments were reviewed by trained research assistants and a clinical psychologist and scored for integrity issues on a 0–2 scale; assessments were made blind, as much as possible, to other data. Scores of 0 indicated no threats to data integrity, scores of 1 indicated mild threats to data integrity (e.g., parent provides support, but the child did not change their response), and scores of 2 indicated potential serious threats to integrity (e.g., the child changed their response based on parent input; the administration was prematurely terminated because of child behavior or other circumstance). Cases with an integrity score of 2 were removed for primary analyses (3 six-month cognitive, 3 six-month language, 2 twelve-month cognitive, 1 twelve-month language, 8 twenty-four-month cognitive, 8 twenty-four-month language). For executive functioning, 14 snack delay, 35 reverse categorization, and 43 spin the pots scores received an integrity score of 2 and were omitted from primary analyses. We conducted sensitivity analyses including these cases, and no substantial differences in findings were noted.

Covariates

We created a directed acyclic graph (DAG) to identify potential confounding variables in the association between prenatal cortisol exposure and neurocognitive development (see Supplementary Figure S1). DAGs are graphical representations of causal relationships between exposure and outcome variables and are useful to identify potential confounding variables and guide covariate selection (Digitale et al., Reference Digitale, Martin and Glymour2022; Greenland et al., Reference Greenland, Pearl and Robins1999). Variables in the DAG were selected based on theoretical knowledge and empirical evidence. Covariates included family socioeconomic status, maternal age, maternal anxious and depressive symptoms, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, gravidity, infant sex, and neighborhood dangerousness. Family income-to-needs ratio was used as a proxy for family SES and was calculated by dividing a family’s income by the poverty threshold calculated by the Department of Health and Human Services (United States Department of Health & Human Services, 2022). Neighborhood dangerousness was measured using an abbreviated nine-item version of the Me & my Neighborhood Questionnaire (Pittsburgh Youth Study, 1991). Mothers reported on the frequency of experiences in the neighborhood (e.g., “I heard adults arguing loudly on my street”) over the past year on a four-point Likert scale from Never (0) to Often (3). Scores were summed to create a cumulative neighborhood dangerousness score. Maternal depressive symptoms were assessed using the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987) at each trimester across pregnancy. Maternal prenatal anxious symptoms were assessed using the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., Reference Meyer, Miller, Metzger and Borkovec1990). Repeated EPDS and PSWQ measurements were highly correlated (r’s = 0.70–0.72 and 0.74–0.85 for the EPDS and PSWQ, respectively) and were therefore averaged across pregnancy.

Postnatally, caregivers completed the EPDS and PSWQ at 6, 12, and 24 months. Postnatal EPDS and PSWQ scores were also highly correlated over time (0.46–0.65 for EPDS and 0.78–0.80 for PSWQ) and were averaged to create mean postnatal depressive and anxious symptom scores. Because prenatal and postnatal depressive and anxious symptoms scores were highly correlated (r = 0.72 for EPDS and r = 0.81 for PSWQ), postnatal scores were revisualized on prenatal scores to adjust for multicollinearity. Caregivers also completed the Vocabulary subtest from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Adults, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2008) at the 24-month assessment. Caregiver Vocabulary scores were also included as a covariate to adjust for aspects of the postnatal environment that may be associated with caregiver vocabulary abilities.

Data analysis

Data preparation and descriptive analyses were conducted using the ‘base’ package in R version 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2022). Structural equation models were fit using the ‘lavaan’ package version 0.6-14 in R (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012) to test the timing sensitivity and general exposure models. We fit a simplex model to the repeated cortisol AUCg measurements (see Supplementary Figure S2 for a path diagram). Specifically, we regressed trimester 2 cortisol AUCg onto trimester 1 cortisol AUCg, and trimester 3 cortisol AUCg onto trimesters 1 and 2 cortisol AUCg. The use of a simplex model allowed us to isolate cortisol exposure unique to each trimester (e.g., trimester 3 was adjusted for cortisol levels at trimesters 1 and 2) and test independent associations between cortisol exposure at each trimester and neurocognitive outcomes within the same model. This allowed us to take a conservative approach to testing timing sensitivity as models were adjusted for potential multicollinearity among repeated cortisol measurements.

Neurocognitive outcomes were regressed onto cortisol AUCg at trimesters 1, 2, and 3. Models were fit separately for cognitive (Bayley) and executive functioning outcomes. Although not depicted in Figure 1 for clarity, all six Bayley scores (cognitive and language at 6, 12, and 24 months) were included as individual outcomes in a single model. Similarly, snack delay, spin the pots, and reverse categorization scores were included as individual outcomes in a single executive functioning model. We compared the fit of two models: a timing sensitivity model (see Model 1a in Figure 1) and a general exposure model (see Model 1b in Figure 1). In the timing sensitivity model, all regression paths from the cortisol AUCg measurements were freely estimated. In the general exposure model, the regression paths from cortisol at each trimester and each neurocognitive outcome were constrained to be equal (e.g., cortisol AUCg at trimesters 1, 2, and 3 were constrained to have an equal association with 12-month language abilities). Associations were estimated freely for different outcomes within the same model (e.g., the association between cortisol and 12-month language abilities was allowed to differ from the association between cortisol and 12-month cognitive abilities). The general exposure and timing sensitivity models were nested and, therefore, could be compared using a chi-square difference test (Satorra, Reference Satorra, Heijmans, Pollock and Satorra2000). A significant difference in model fit would indicate that the more constrained model (the general Exposure model) had a poorer fit to the data relative to the timing sensitivity model. Wake time and post-conception age at time of cortisol collection for each sample set were included in the models as time-varying covariates.

Figure 1. Conceptual diagrams of competing theoretical models.

To test whether infant sex moderated associations between cortisol AUCg and neurocognitive outcomes, we fit multi-group structural equation models. Before testing moderation by sex, we confirmed the homogeneity of means and variances assumption by constraining the means and variances for all independent and dependent variables to be the same for males and females, and then compared the fit of the constrained model to a model where the means and variances were freely estimated by sex. We also constrained the autoregressive paths to be equal by sex. Finally, we tested moderation by individually constraining the paths from cortisol AUCg to the cognitive and executive functioning outcomes to be equal by sex and testing differences in model fit. If constraining a regression path from one of the AUCg to a neurocognitive outcome to be equal by sex resulted in a significant worsening of model fit, then that would indicate the presence of moderation (Maruyama, Reference Maruyama and Maruyama1998).

Missing data

Data were missing on independent and dependent variables. Regarding cortisol measurements, 61 participants (24.1%) had no valid measurements, 63 (24.9%) had one valid measurement, 66 (26.1%) had two valid measurements, and 63 (24.9%) had three valid measurements (these estimates also include missing due to laboratory exclusions/extreme values, see Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022). Of the six Bayley outcomes (cognitive and language at 6, 12, and 24 months), 41 (16.2%) had no scores, 37 (14.6%) had two scores, 5 (2.0%) had three scores, 61 (24.1%) had four scores, 7 (2.8%) had five scores, and 102 (40.3%) had six scores. Rates of missing data (because of no administration or exclusion for incomplete assessment) were higher for executive functioning tasks: of the 217 individuals included in the executive functioning analyses, 93 (42.9%) had no executive functioning assessments, 30 (13.8%) had one executive functioning measurement, 47 (21.7%) had two executive functioning measurements, and 48 (22.1%) had three executive functioning measurements. A heatmap depicting patterns of missingness for all the independent and dependent study variables is depicted in Supplementary Figure S3.

Models were fit using maximum likelihood estimation and missing data were accounted for using full information maximum likelihood estimation (FIML; Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). Under conditions of high rates of missingness, FIML has been found to perform similarly to other missing data techniques (e.g., multiple imputation) and yield unbiased parameter estimates when the model is correctly specified (based on RMSEA values), even in instances where there are high rates of missing data (Lee & Shi, Reference Lee and Shi2021).

An assumption of using FIML is that data are missing at random (MAR; Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). MAR occurs when missingness is related to observed constructs, but not related to the missing value itself (Rubin, Reference Rubin1976). For example, under MAR conditions, missing 12-month Bayley Language scores may related to family income, but MAR would be violated if children with stronger language abilities at 12 months were more likely to be missing the 12-month Bayley Language score. Empirically testing for MAR patterns of missingness is not possible as doing so would require knowledge of the missing value (e.g., 12-month Baley Language scores in the previous example; Enders, Reference Enders2013). To explore patterns of missingness, we fitted a series of t tests to explore whether missingness on each of the cortisol measurements or Bayley scores was related to any continuous study covariates (chi-square tests were used for categorical covariates). In exploring patterns of missingness, we also tested whether missing cortisol values and Bayley scores were associated with cortisol values and Bayley scores at other waves (e.g., if missing cortisol AUCg at trimester 1 was associated with cortisol AUCg values at trimesters 2 or 3). To the extent that repeated cortisol and Bayley measurements were associated with one another, this provided an approximation of whether missingness was related to the missing value itself. Results of attrition analyses are presented in Supplementary Tables S1S2 for attrition analyses.

Missing cortisol and cognitive scores were associated with several covariates. Missing trimester 1 cortisol AUCg was not associated with cortisol AUCg at trimesters 2 or 3 and missing trimester 2 cortisol measurements were not associated with cortisol AUCg at trimesters 1 or 3. Individuals missing cortisol AUCg at trimester 3 had lower trimester 2 cortisol AUCg scores (t = 2.4, df = 60.0, p = .022, Cohen’s d = −0.41). Missing Bayley scores at 6, 12, and 24 months were not associated with missing Bayley scores at other waves. Therefore, we proceeded to analyze the data using FIML under the assumption that data are MAR.

Power analyses

Monte Carlo simulations were conducted in Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2017) to test the power of detecting the observed associations between cortisol AUCg at each trimester and each neurocognitive outcome. Power analyses were based on 2,500 simulated datasets of 253 individuals for the Bayley model (218 individuals for the executive functioning model). We coded patterns of missingness in the Monte Carlo simulations to reflect patterns of missingness in the data.

Results

Characteristics of the study sample are presented in Table 2. This sample was racially, ethnically, and socioeconomically diverse. Participants belonged to the following racial and ethnic groups: 7.9% Hispanic, 60.9% non-Hispanic White, 22.9% non-Hispanic Black, 4.3% Asian, and 4.0% other (including Native American, Pacific Islander, biracial, and multi-racial). The average household income was 3.2 times the poverty level, but about a quarter of the sample (23.5%) was living at or below 150% of the poverty line.

Table 1. Sample collection times and raw cortisol measurements

Note. Standard deviations for time are presented in minutes.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the study sample

Note. For analyses, race/ethnicity was recoded as White = 1, nonwhite = 0 due tosmall cell sizes of many of the race/ethnicity categories. AUCg = cortisol area under the curve withrespect to the ground.

Supplementary Figure S4 depicts the raw cortisol concentrations calculated with each sample at each point in pregnancy. Diurnal patterns of cortisol concentrations followed the expected pattern characterized by elevated levels in the morning and a gradual decline over the course of waking hours. Mean cortisol levels (based on AUCg) increased across pregnancy, with the largest increase observed between trimesters 1 and 2 (see Table 2, supplementary Figure S4).

Bivariate correlations

See Table 3 for bivariate correlations between cortisol concentrations and neurocognitive outcomes. AUCg measurements were weakly to modestly positively correlated across pregnancy trimesters. Bayley scores (with the exception of 6-month language) were moderately positively correlated with each other. The executive functioning tasks did not correlate significantly with one another, but better performance on the snack delay and reverse categorization tasks was associated with higher 24-month Bayley scores. There was a general pattern of first and second-trimester measures of maternal cortisol AUCg to be inversely associated with Bayley scores at 12 and 24 months; that contrasted with the findings with third-trimester cortisol AUCg. Associations with executive function measures were generally weaker and less consistent. Supplementary Figure S5 depicts the bivariate associations between cortisol AUCg by trimester and Bayley scores.

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between maternal prenatal cortisol and infant mental development

Note. Pearson correlation coefficients are presented below the diagonal. The n included in each correlation is presented above the diagonal. AUCg = cortisol area under the curve with respect to the ground; T1–T3 = trimesters 1–3; COG = Bayley Cognitive subscale; LAN = Bayley Language subscale. Reverse Cat = Reverse Categorization. *p < .05

Simplex model of cortisol AUCg across pregnancy

The simplex model was fully saturated and, therefore, had perfect fit to the data. Higher cortisol AUCg in trimester 1 was associated with higher cortisol AUCg in trimester 2 (B = 0.38, SE = 0.19, p = .044) and trimester 3 (B = 0.54, SE = 0.13, p < .001). Additionally, higher cortisol AUCg in trimester 2 was associated with higher cortisol AUCg in trimester 3 (B = 0.20, SE = 0.08, p = .016).

Prenatal cortisol exposure and cognitive development

The timing sensitivity model fit the cognitive data well (X 2 = 192.0, df = 169, p = .108, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 0.97). Constraining the associations between cortisol AUCg and Bayley outcomes to be equal across trimester (general exposure) resulted in a significant reduction in model fit (X 2 = 21.8, df = 12, p = 0.040). Associations between cortisol AUCg at each trimester and all Bayley scores are presented in Table 4. Higher first-trimester cortisol AUCg was associated with lower 12-month cognitive (B = −0.18, SE = 0.07, 95% C.I. = −0.30, −0.05) and 12-month language (B = −0.16, SE = 0.05, 95% C.I. = −0.24, −0.06) scores, but not at 6 or 24 months.

Table 4. Parameter estimates of the association between cortisol AUCg across pregnancy and Bayley-III scores at 6, 12, and 24 months

Note. Beta coefficients are unstandardized. Significant associations are bolded for clarity. Covariates included family income-to-needs ratio, maternal age, prenatal and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, prenatal and postnatal maternal anxious symptoms, neighborhood dangerousness, gravidity, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal WAIS Vocabulary scores, and infant sex. LAN stands for Bayley language, COG stands for Bayley cognitive, AUCg stands for cortisol area under the curve with respect to the ground, and T1–T3 stand for trimester 1–trimester 3, respectively.

Second-trimester cortisol AUCg was significantly negatively associated with lower language scores at 24 months (B = −0.15, SE = 0.06, 95% C.I. = −0.26, –0.03). Second-trimester cortisol AUCg was positively associated with cognitive scores at 6 months (B = 0.09, SE = 0.04, 95% C.I. = 0.02, 0.16); significant associations were not found for 6-month language or 12-month cognitive or language scores.

Third-trimester AUCg was not significantly related to cognitive or language outcomes at any age.

Power analyses revealed that there was sufficient power to detect the observed association between first-trimester cortisol AUCg and 12-month language abilities (80.2%) and the observed association between second-trimester cortisol AUCg and 24-month language abilities (80.8%). The study was slightly underpowered to detect the observed association between second-trimester cortisol AUCg and 6-month cognitive abilities (68.0%) and the observed association between first-trimester cortisol AUCg and 12-month cognitive abilities (70.1%).

Prenatal cortisol exposure and executive functioning

See Table 5 for parameter estimates for the prenatal cortisol AUCg models predicting child executive functioning at 2 years of age. The timing sensitivity model also fit better than the general exposure model for executive functioning (X 2 = 14.2, df = 6, p < .028). Higher first-trimester AUCg was associated with poorer performance on the snack delay task (B = -0.14, SE = 0.06, 95% C.I. = −0.26, −0.03) but not on spin the pots or reverse categorization. Second-trimester cortisol exposure was not significantly associated with executive functioning measures. In contrast, one positive association was found: third-trimester cortisol concentrations was positively associated with reverse categorization scores (B = 0.06, SE = 0.02, 90% C.I. = 0.02, 0.09).

Table 5. Parameter estimates of the association between cortisol AUCg across pregnancy and executive functioning scores at 24 months

Note. Beta coefficients are unstandardized. Significant associations are bolded for clarity. To improve the interpretability of the regression coefficients, AUC values were divided by 10 so they were on a similar scale as the executive functioning measures. Covariates included family income-to-needs ratio, maternal age, prenatal and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, prenatal and postnatal maternal anxious symptoms, neighborhood dangerousness, gravidity, maternal pre-pregnancy BMI, maternal WAIS Vocabulary scores, infant sex, and infant age at the time of assessment. AUCg stands for cortisol area under the curve with respect to the ground and T1–T3 stand for trimester 1–trimester 3, respectively.

Power analyses revealed that the study was sufficiently powered to detect the association between third-trimester cortisol concentrations and reverse categorization scores (95.7%). Analyses were underpowered to detect the association between first-trimester cortisol exposure and the snack delay score (58.6%).

Moderation of associations by sex

There was limited evidence of sex moderation. Sex did not significantly moderate any of the paths between trimesters 1, 2, and 3 cortisol AUCg and Bayley scores (see Supplementary Table S3). Only one path between cortisol exposure and an executive functioning outcome was moderated by sex: third-trimester cortisol AUCg was associated with higher snack delay scores for males relative to females (see Supplementary Table S4).

Sensitivity analysis

Sensitivity analyses were conducted including all Bayley and executive functioning scores initially assessed to have a serious threat to integrity. The magnitude and direction of associations between cortisol AUCg and neurocognitive outcomes remained stable after including neurocognitive assessments with serious threats to integrity. Findings for Bayley scores and executive functioning results are presented in Supplementary Tables S5S6, respectively.

Discussion

In this racially and socioeconomically diverse community sample, we found reliable evidence of a developmental timing effect of prenatal maternal cortisol on child neurodevelopment. Exposure to higher cortisol concentrations in the first trimester was reliably associated with poorer cognitive and language abilities at 12 months in both bivariate correlations and structural equation models which accounted for cortisol exposure in later gestation. First-trimester maternal cortisol was also negatively associated with inhibition at 24 months. Additionally, higher second-trimester cortisol concentrations were associated with poorer language abilities at 24 months. Finally, there was limited evidence of a positive association between mid-gestation cortisol levels of 6-month cognitive scores and late gestation cortisol levels and set shifting abilities at 24 months. These findings extend previous research (Bergman et al., Reference Bergman, Sarkar, Glover and O’Connor2010; Caparros-Gonzalez et al., Reference Caparros-Gonzalez, Romero-Gonzalez, Gonzalez-Perez, Lucena-Prieto, Perez- Garcia, Cruz-Quintana and Peralta-Ramirez2019; Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010) by demonstrating sensitivity of early neurocognitive development to maternal cortisol beginning in the first trimester and in suggesting that early exposure effects had independent, persisting effects on multiple measures of child neurodevelopment.

There are several reasons why cortisol exposure early in gestation may be more reliably associated with poorer language and cognitive performance and inhibition than cortisol exposure at later points in gestation. For example, exposure to elevated maternal cortisol early in gestation may be more detrimental to fetal development as concentrations of the barrier enzyme 11β-HSD2 are lowest early in pregnancy (Schoof et al., Reference Schoof, Girstl, Frobenius, Kirschbaum, Repp, Knerr, Rascher and Dotsch2001). Furthermore, the fetal blood–brain barrier, which serves as a secondary barrier for the developing fetal brain after the placenta, is not fully developed until the second trimester (Goasdoué et al., Reference Goasdoué, Miller, Colditz and Björkman2017). Thus, stronger prediction from glucocorticoid exposure in early gestation may reflect a greater effect on, or greater vulnerability of, early brain development (Lautarescu et al., Reference Lautarescu, Craig and Glover2020; Reference O’Donnell, O’Connor and GloverO’ Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, O’Connor and Glover2009). In humans, neurogenesis and gliogenesis begin in the first trimester and precipitate the formation of the neocortex and other brain structures (Leibovitz et al., Reference Leibovitz, Lerman-Sagie and Haddad2022; Monk et al., Reference Monk, Lugo-Candelas and Trumpff2019). Early disruption to the development of these structures may have cascading effects on subsequent development of more complex structures, and eventually early cognition (Vlasova et al., Reference Vlasova, Iosif, Ryan, Funk, Murai, Chen, Lesh, Rowland, Bennett, Hogrefe, Maddock, Gandal, Geschwind, Schumann, Van de Water, McAllister, Carter, Styner, Amaral and Bauman2021). Glucocorticoid receptors (GR) and mineralocorticoid receptors (MR), which are necessary structures for maternal glucocorticoids to have a direct impact on fetal brain development, are fully developed in humans by 24 weeks gestation (Noorlander et al., Reference Noorlander, De Graan, Middeldorp, Van Beers and Visser2006). However, 24 weeks gestation represents the lower limit at which fetuses have been tested for the presence of GRs or MRs, and GRs and MRs are likely present earlier in gestation (Noorlander et al., Reference Noorlander, De Graan, Middeldorp, Van Beers and Visser2006).

Alternatively, the placenta may be a structure by which exposure to elevated maternal glucocorticoids early in gestation may contribute to neurocognitive development. Maternal depression and stress have been associated with elevated cortisol production during pregnancy (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Gu, Wu, Brunner, Panisch, Best, Arnold, Duberstein, Z., Putzig, Carnahan, Growth, Barrett and O’Connor2022) and over time, maternal stress has been found to downregulate placental production of 11β-HSD2 (Reference O’Donnell, Jensen, Freeman, Khalife, O’Connor and GloverO’ Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Jensen, Freeman, Khalife, O’Connor and Glover2012). Downregulation of placental 11β-HSD2 production early in gestation may result in lower placental production of 11β-HSD2 throughout gestation, resulting in prolonged fetal exposure to elevated glucocorticoids. In turn, chronic fetal exposure to elevated levels of glucocorticoids may impair fetal neurodevelopment (Samarasinghe et al., Reference Samarasinghe, Di Maio, Volonte, Galbiati, Lewis, Romero and DeFranco2011) and restrict fetal growth (Gur et al., Reference Gur, Diav-Citrin, Shechtman, Arnon and Ornoy2004), ultimately leading to poorer performance on early neurocognitive assessments. Recent twin study research has found that prenatal environmental experiences account for most of the phenotypic association between birth weight and early cognitive development (Womack et al., Reference Womack, Beam, Giangrande, Tong, Scharf, Finkel and Davis2024), potentially implicating impaired fetal growth in a developmental cascade from which early maternal glucocorticoid production downregulates placental 11β-HSD2 which leads to restricted fetal growth and ultimately compromised neurocognitive development. Elucidating the mechanisms by which early prenatal maternal cortisol leads to impaired neurodevelopment remains an important avenue for future research.

There are parallel lines of evidence emphasizing early prenatal exposure for neurodevelopmental outcomes, perhaps because of neural development and greater permeability of barriers to protect the fetus. One is the maternal immune activation hypothesis, which consistently demonstrates that maternal illness and infection in early pregnancy are associated with neurodevelopmental disorders (Brown et al., Reference Brown, Begg, Gravenstein, Schaefer, Wyatt, Bresnahan, Babulas and Susser2004; Weir et al., Reference Weir, Forghany, Smith, Patterson, McAllister, Schumann and Bauman2015) and with individual differences in neurodevelopment (Ghassabian et al., Reference Ghassabian, Albert, Hornig, Yeung, Cherkerzian, Goldstein, Buka, Goldstein and Gilman2018). The stronger and more consistent effects on cognitive and executive function found for cortisol earlier in gestation may reflect the relatively weaker role of protective mechanisms such as barrier enzymes or a greater vulnerability of early brain development, but there are alternative explanations. For example, maternal cortisol levels naturally increase across gestation and peak near parturition (Duthie et al., Reference Duthie and Reynolds2013; Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Emanuel, Frim and Majzoub1988), presumably reflecting a greater influence of maternal-placenta biology of pregnancy; elevations in maternal cortisol concentrations early in pregnancy may reflect a greater deviation from typical levels relative to elevations at later gestational ages. We are unable to differentiate among these explanations, but our findings do underscore the value in formally testing much-needed developmental timing hypotheses in studies of prenatal exposures.

In line with previous research (Davis & Sandman, Reference Davis and Sandman2010; Davis et al., Reference Davis, Head, Buss and Sandman2017), there was suggestive evidence that elevated levels of maternal cortisol in later gestation had a positive impact on select neurocognitive outcomes. In the present study, this was seen with performance on the reverse categorization task, a task of cognitive flexibility and set shifting abilities. Slight elevations in maternal glucocorticoids in late gestation have been linked with fetal maturation (Drake et al., Reference Drake, Tang and Nyirenda2007; Reference Trejo, Cuchillo, Machín and RúaTrejo et al., Reference Trejo, Cuchillo, Machín and Rúa2000) and postnatal cortical thickness in the frontal regions of the brain (Davis et al., Reference Davis, Head, Buss and Sandman2017), suggesting that exposure to glucocorticoids late in gestation may promote brain development. However, the lack of a reliable association between third-trimester cortisol concentration with other cognitive, language, or executive functioning outcomes in the present study suggests that this may not be a robust effect; furthermore, these findings are in contrast with other studies reporting negative associations between maternal cortisol and cognitive development (LeWinn et al., Reference LeWinn, Stroud, Molnar, Ware, Koenen and Buka2009).

The developmental timing of child neurodevelopmental outcomes is also notable. We observed generally stronger associations at the latter assessments, which agrees with one report indicating that prenatal cortisol exposure was unrelated to cognitive outcomes until 12 months (Lautarescu et al., Reference Lautarescu, Craig and Glover2020). Despite being more proximal to prenatal exposures, cognitive abilities assessed at 6 months differ significantly in complexity and may be more confounded with sleep/wake and other behavioral artifacts than assessments conducted at 12 and 24 months (Graham et al., Reference Graham, Rasmussen, Entringer, Ward, Rudolph, Gilmore, Styner, Wadhwa, Fair and Buss2019). At a minimum, increased error of measurement of cognitive abilities in early infancy at least underscores the need for ongoing assessments past infancy.

Sex did not reliably moderate associations between cortisol exposure and neurocognitive outcomes through age 2 years. A recent meta-analysis of 22 studies observed a modest negative association between maternal stress and anxiety (a proxy for HPA axis physiology) and cognitive outcomes but did not observe any moderation by sex (Delagneau et al., Reference Delagneau, Twilhaar, Testa, van Veen and Anderson2023), which conflicts with animal studies suggesting that sex may moderate the association between prenatal cortisol exposure and learning and memory development (see Glover & Hill, Reference Glover and Hill2012 for a review). Further research in humans that incorporates prenatal sex steroids and other plausible explanations for sex moderation. This is particularly important as studies (including the present study) are often powered to detect main effects of prenatal exposures on developmental outcomes but are underpowered to detect interactions.

This study benefited from several strengths, including a comparatively large and diverse sample for research of this kind, multiple measurements of prenatal cortisol exposure from the first trimester, and a conservative modeling approach to test developmental timing. However, findings should be considered in the context of several limitations. First, although the sample size was large relative to comparable studies, results from a Monte Carlo simulation suggested that the study was underpowered to detect select hypotheses. Second, there were significant rates of missing data on exposures and outcomes, which highlights the challenges in collecting intensive longitudinal data in a community sample, and particularly the burden of collecting diurnal salivary samples. Approximately half of the sample was missing two or more prenatal cortisol measurements, which may have led to inflated standard errors around parameter estimates. Notably, patterns of missingness did not violate the MAR assumption and the FIML approach to handling missingness allowed covariates to inform patterns of missingness, reducing bias in parameter estimates. Third, most of the first-trimester cortisol assessments were collected relatively late in the first trimester; sampling data even earlier in gestation is needed to provide broader coverage of gestational exposure. Fourth, there are limits of the sample, as the collection of diurnal cortisol is burdensome on participants and thereby creates selection biases. Our approach to collecting cortisol measurements over a single day in each trimester represents a snapshot of overall maternal cortisol output over a typical day. Previous research suggests that within-person, cortisol output does not vary significantly when measured on consecutive days (O’Donnell et al., Reference O’Donnell, Glover, Jenkins, Browne, Ben-Shlomo, Golding and O’Connor2013), and collecting saliva samples across multiple days in each trimester would increase participant burden. Nonetheless, collecting additional cortisol samples within trimester may have provided additional information on within-person fluctuations. Additionally, although our indicator of cortisol (AUCg) is thought to reflect total cortisol output (and total potential fetal exposure over a day), AUCg is agnostic to individual differences in the rate and shape of diurnal cortisol trajectories. Fifth, although we controlled for postnatal maternal anxious symptoms, depressive symptoms, and vocabulary abilities, we cannot rule out the possibility that other exposures contributed to the observed effects. For example, our focus here was on prenatal cortisol exposure because of its dominance in the developmental programming research; other prenatal biological exposures, including other steroid hormones and immune markers, require attention moving forward because of their associations with the HPA axis.

In conclusion, we observed a reliable negative association between cortisol exposure in early- to mid-pregnancy, but not late pregnancy, and early problem-solving, language, and inhibition abilities. These findings provide further evidence for the need to collect multiple exposure periods within pregnancy and test developmental timing hypotheses within the prenatal exposure period. Clinical applications of the findings are intriguing but indirect (Knap et al., Reference Knapp, Kress, Parker, Page, McArthur, Gachigi, Alshawabkeh, Aschner, Bastain, Breton, Bendixsen, Brennan, Bush, Buss, Camargo, Catellier, Cordero, Croen, Dabelea and Jacobson2023). For example, if neurocognitive development is more vulnerable to early cortisol exposure, then there is a concomitant need to start prenatal interventions early in pregnancy. That may be challenging; nonetheless, (Branum & Aherns, Reference Branum and Ahrens2017) recruiting at-risk participants prior to conception may be an important way forward (Finer & Zolna, Reference Finer and Zolna2016; Urizar & Muñoz, Reference Urizar and Muñoz2011) if implemented early in gestation.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001287

Funding statement

Funding was provided by NIH grants T32HD101392, OD023349, HD083369, NIEHS grant P30 ES005022 and The Wynne Center for Family Research; the project described in this publication was supported by the University of Rochester CTSA award number UL1 TR002001 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

Barker, D. J. (1998). In utero programming of chronic disease. Clinical Science, 95(2), 115128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development, third edition: Technical manual. Harcourt.Google Scholar
Bergman, K., Sarkar, P., Glover, V., & O’Connor, T. G. (2010). Maternal prenatal cortisol and infant cognitive development: Moderation by infant–mother attachment. Biological Psychiatry, 67(11), 10261032. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.01.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bishop, E. G., Cherny, S. S., Corley, R., Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2003). Development genetic analysis of general cognitive ability from 1 to 12 years in a sample of adoptees, biological siblings, and twins. Intelligence, 31, 3149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00112-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J., Wainstock, T., Braun, K., & Segal, M. (2015). Stress in utero: Prenatal programming of brain plasticity and cognition. Biological Psychiatry, 78(5), 315326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branum, A. M., & Ahrens, K. A. (2017). Trends in timing of pregnancy awareness among US women. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(4), 715726. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2155-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, A. S., Begg, M. D., Gravenstein, S., Schaefer, C. A., Wyatt, R. J., Bresnahan, M., Babulas, V. P., & Susser, E. S. (2004). Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(8), 774780. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.774 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bublitz, M. H., Parade, S., & Stroud, L. R. (2014). The effects of childhood sexual abuse on cortisol trajectories in pregnancy are moderated by current family functioning. Biological Psychology, 103, 152157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.08.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, C., Davis, E. P., Hobel, C. J., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Maternal pregnancy-specific anxiety is associated with child executive function at 6–9 years age. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 14(6), 665676. https://doi.org/10.3109/10253890.2011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R. K., Devick, K. L., Coull, B. A., Cowell, W., Askowitz, T., Goldson, B., Wright, R. O., & Wright, R. J. (2019). Prenatal cortisol modifies the association between maternal trauma history and child cognitive development in a sex-specific manner in an urban pregnancy cohort. Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands), 22(2), 228235. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2018.1553950 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caparros-Gonzalez, R. A., Romero-Gonzalez, B., Gonzalez-Perez, R., Lucena-Prieto, L., Perez- Garcia, M., Cruz-Quintana, F., & Peralta-Ramirez, M. I. (2019). Maternal and neonatal hair cortisol levels are associated with infant neurodevelopment at six months of age. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 8(11), 114. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm8112015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, S. M., 2012). Executive function scale for preschoolers test manual, Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota Google Scholar
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuffe, J. S. M., O’Sullivan, L., Simmons, D. G., Anderson, S. T., & Moritz, K. M. (2012). Maternal corticosterone exposure in the mouse has sex-specific effects on placental growth and mRNA expression. Endocrinology, 153(11), 55005511. https://doi.org/10.1210/en.2012-1479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Head, K., Buss, C., & Sandman, C. A. (2017). Prenatal maternal cortisol concentrations predict neurodevelopment in middle childhood. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 75, 5663. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.10.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., & Sandman, C. A. (2010). The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Development, 81(1), 131148. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01385.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delagneau, G., Twilhaar, E. S., Testa, R., van Veen, S., & Anderson, P. (2023). Association between prenatal maternal anxiety and/or stress and offspring’s cognitive functioning: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 94(3), 779801. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13885 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Digitale, J. C., Martin, J. N., & Glymour, M. M. (2022). Tutorial on directed acyclic graphs. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 142, 264267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2021.08.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, A. J., Tang, J. I., & Nyirenda, M. J. (2007). Mechanisms underlying the role of glucocorticoids in the early life programming of adult disease. Clinical Science, 113(5), 219232. https://doi.org/10.1042/CS20070107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duthie, L., & Reynolds, R. M. (2013). Changes in the maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in pregnancy and postpartum: Influences on maternal and fetal outcomes. Neuroendocrinology, 98(2), 106115. https://doi.org/10.1159/000354702 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C. K. (2013). Dealing with missing data in developmental research. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 2731. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12008 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 430457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fekedulegn, D. B., Andrew, M. E., Burchfiel, C. M., Violanti, J. M., Hartley, T. A., Charles, L. E., & Miller, D. B. (2007). Area under the curve and other summary indicators of repeated waking cortisol measurements. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(7), 651659. https://doi.org/10.1097/PSY CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finer, L. B., & Zolna, M. R. (2016). Declines in unintended pregnancy in the United States, 2008–2011. New England Journal of Medicine, 374(9), 843852. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa1506575 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Robinson, J. L., & Hewitt, J. K. (2011). Developmental trajectories in toddlers’ self-restraint predict individual differences in executive functions 14 years later: a behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology, 47, 14101430. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023750 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghassabian, A., Albert, P. S., Hornig, M., Yeung, E., Cherkerzian, S., Goldstein, R. B., Buka, S. L., Goldstein, J. M., & Gilman, S. E. (2018). Gestational cytokine concentrations and neurocognitive development at 7 years. Translational Psychiatry, 8(1), 6471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0112-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glover, V., & Hill, J. (2012). Sex differences in the programming effects of prenatal stress on psychopathology and stress responses: An evolutionary perspective. Physiology & Behavior, 106, 736740. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.02.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gluckman, P., & Hanson, M. (2005). The fetal matrix: Evolution, development and disease. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goasdoué, K., Miller, S. M., Colditz, P. B., & Björkman, S. T. (2017). The blood-brain barrier; protecting the developing fetal brain. Placenta, 54, 111116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.placenta.2016.12.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, A. M., Rasmussen, J. M., Entringer, S., Ward, E. B., Rudolph, M. D., Gilmore, J. H., Styner, M., Wadhwa, P. D., Fair, D. A., & Buss, C. (2019). Maternal cortisol concentrations during pregnancy and sex-specific associations with neonatal amygdala connectivity and emerging internalizing behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 85(2), 172181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.06.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenland, S., Pearl, J., & Robins, J. M. (1999). Causal diagrams for epidemiologic research. Epidemiology (Cambridge, Mass.), 10(1), 3748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The neurobiology of stress and development. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 145173. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gur, C., Diav-Citrin, O., Shechtman, S., Arnon, J., & Ornoy, A. (2004). Pregnancy outcome after first trimester exposure to corticosteroids: A prospective controlled study. Reproductive Toxicology, 18(1), 93101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.reprotox.2003.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howland, M. A., Sandman, C. A., & Glynn, L. M. (2017). Developmental origins of the human hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Expert Review of Endocrinology & Metabolism, 12(5), 321339. https://doi.org/10.1080/17446651.2017.1356222 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2005). Executive function and theory of mind in 2-year-olds: A family affair? Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 645668. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huizink, A. C., Robles de Medina, P. G., Mulder, E. J., Visser, G. H., & Buitelaar, J. K. (2003). Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(6), 810818. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00166 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karin, O., Raz, M., Tendler, A., Bar, A., Korem Kohanim, Y., Milo, T., & Alon, U. (2020). A new model for the HPA axis explains dysregulation of stress hormones on the timescale of weeks. Molecular Systems Biology, 16(7), e9510. https://doi.org/10.15252/msb.20209510 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khoury, J. E., Gonzalez, A., Levitan, R. D., Pruessner, J. C., Chopra, K., Basile, V. S., Masellis, M., Goodwill, A., & Atkinson, L. (2015). Summary cortisol reactivity indicators: Interrelations and meaning. Neurobiology of Stress, 2, 3443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2015.04.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knapp, E. A., Kress, A. M., Parker, C. B., Page, G. P., McArthur, K., Gachigi, K. K., Alshawabkeh, A. N., Aschner, J. L., Bastain, T. M., Breton, C. V., Bendixsen, C. G., Brennan, P. A., Bush, N. R., Buss, C., Camargo, C. A., Catellier, D., Cordero, J. F., Croen, L., Dabelea, D., ... & Jacobson, L. P. (2023). The environmental influences on child health outcomes (ECHO)-wide cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology, 192(8), 12491263. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwad071 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.220 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lautarescu, A., Craig, M. C., & Glover, V. (2020). Prenatal stress: Effects on fetal and child brain development. International Review of Neurobiology, 150, 1740. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.irn.2019.11.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T., & Shi, D. (2021). A comparison of full information maximum likelihood and multiple imputation in structural equation modeling with missing data. Psychological Methods, 26(4), 466485. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000381 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leibovitz, Z., Lerman-Sagie, T., & Haddad, L. (2022). Fetal brain development: Regulating processes and related malformations. Life, 12(6), 809846. https://doi.org/10.3390/life12060809 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeWinn, K. Z., Stroud, L. R., Molnar, B. E., Ware, J. H., Koenen, K. C., & Buka, S. L. (2009). Elevated maternal cortisol levels during pregnancy are associated with reduced childhood IQ. International Journal of Epidemiology, 38(6), 17001710. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyp200 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mariño-Narvaez, C., Puertas-Gonzalez, J. A., Romero-Gonzalez, B., Gonzalez-Perez, R., & Peralta-Ramírez, M. I. (2023). How prenatal cortisol levels may differentially affect the neurodevelopment of boys and girls. Early Human Development, 187, 105874. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2023.105874 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maruyama, G. M. (1998). Variations on the basic structural equation model. In Maruyama, G. M. (Ed.), Basics of structural equation modeling (pp. 255270). SAGE Publications Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28(6), 487495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monk, C., Lugo-Candelas, C., & Trumpff, C. (2019). Prenatal developmental origins of future psychopathology: Mechanisms and pathways. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 15, 317344. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718-095539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, H. R., Gu, Y., Wu, Q., Brunner, J., Panisch, L. S., Best, M., Arnold, M., Duberstein, S., Z., D., Putzig, J., Carnahan, J., Growth, S. W., Barrett, E. S., & O’Connor, T. G. (2022). Prenatal diurnal cortisol: Normative patterns and associations with affective symptoms and stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 143, 105856. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2022.105856 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus. Statistical analysis with latent variables. User’s Guide, 8.Google Scholar
Nazzari, S., Fearon, P., Rice, F., Ciceri, F., Molteni, M., & Frigerio, A. (2020). Neuroendocrine and immune markers of maternal stress during pregnancy and infant cognitive development. Developmental Psychobiology, 62(8), 11001110. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21967 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noorlander, C. W., De Graan, P. N. E., Middeldorp, J., Van Beers, J. J. B. C., & Visser, G. H. A. (2006). Ontogeny of hippocampal corticosteroid receptors: Effects of antenatal glucocorticoids in human and mouse. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 499(6), 924932. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21162 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, T., Best, M., Brunner, J., Ciesla, A. A., Cunning, A., Kapula, N., Kautz, A., Khoury, L., Macomber, A., Meng, Y., Miller, R. K., Murphy, H., Salafia, C. M., Sefair, A. V., Serrano, J., & Barrett, E. (2021). Cohort profile: Understanding pregnancy signals and infant development (UPSIDE): A pregnancy cohort study on prenatal exposure mechanisms for child health. BMJ Open, 11(4), e044798. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-044798 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K., O’Connor, T. G., & Glover, V. (2009). Prenatal stress and neurodevelopment of the child: Focus on the HPA axis and role of the placenta. Developmental Neuroscience, 31(4), 285292. https://doi.org/10.1159/000216539 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Barker, E. D., & O.’Connor, T. G. (2014). The persisting effect of maternal mood in pregnancy on childhood psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 26(2), 393403. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Jenkins, J., Browne, D., Ben-Shlomo, Y., Golding, J., & O’Connor, T. G. (2013). Prenatal maternal mood is associated with altered diurnal cortisol in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(9), 16301638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Donnell, K. J., Jensen, A. B., Freeman, L., Khalife, N., O’Connor, T. G., & Glover, V. (2012). Maternal prenatal anxiety and downregulation of placental 11β-HSD2. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(6), 818826. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittsburgh Youth Study. (1991) Neighborhood dangerousness scale. Unpublished measure. University of Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., & Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 28(7), 916931. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2022). A language and environment for statistical computing. In R foundation for statistical computing.Google Scholar
Robinson, B. G., Emanuel, R. L., Frim, D. M., & Majzoub, J. A. (1988). Glucocorticoid stimulates expression of corticotropin-releasing hormone gene in human placenta. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 85(14), 52445248. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.85.14.5244 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. B. (1976). Inference and missing data. Biometrika, 63, 581592. https://doi.org/10.2307/2335739 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samarasinghe, R. A., Di Maio, R., Volonte, D., Galbiati, F., Lewis, M., Romero, G., & DeFranco, D. B. (2011). Nongenomic glucocorticoid receptor action regulates gap junction intercellular communication and neural progenitor cell proliferation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(40), 1665716662. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1102821108 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandman, C. A., Glynn, L., Schetter, C. D., Wadhwa, P., Garite, T., Chicz-DeMet, A., & Hobel, C. (2006). Elevated maternal cortisol early in pregnancy predicts third trimester levels of placental corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH): Priming the placental clock. Peptides, 27(6), 14571463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peptides.2005.10.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Satorra, A. (2000). Scaled and adjusted restricted tests in multi-sample analysis of moment structures. In Heijmans, R. D. H., Pollock, D. S. G., & Satorra, A. (Eds.), Innovations in multivariate statistical analysis: A festschrift for heinz neudecker (pp. 233247). Kluwer Academic Publishers CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoof, E., Girstl, M., Frobenius, W., Kirschbaum, M., Repp, R., Knerr, I., Rascher, W., & Dotsch, J. (2001). Course of placental 11beta-hydroxysteroid dehydrogenase type 2 and 15-hydroxyprostaglandin dehydrogenase mRNA expression during human gestation. European Journal of Endocrinology, 145(2), 187192. https://doi.org/10.1530/eje.0.1450187 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, J. E., Kessler, C. L., Buss, C., Miller, G. E., Grobman, W. A., Keenan-Devlin, L., Borders, A. E., & Adam, E. K. (2021). Early and current life adversity: Past and present influences on maternal diurnal cortisol rhythms during pregnancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 63(2), 305319. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.22000 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugaya, N., Izawa, S., Ogawa, N., Shirotsuki, K., & Nomura, S. (2020). Association between hair cortisol and diurnal basal cortisol levels: A 30-day validation study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 116, 104650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104650 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trejo, J. L., Cuchillo, I., Machín, C., & Rúa, C. (2000). Maternal adrenalectomy at the early onset of gestation impairs the postnatal development of the rat hippocampal formation: Effects on cell numbers and differentiation, connectivity and calbindin-D28k immunoreactivity. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 62(5), 644667. https://doi.org/10.1002/1097-4547(20001201)62:5<644::AID-JNR4>3.0.CO;2-3 3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United States Department of Health & Human Services. Office of the assistant secretary for planning and evaluation. (n.d.). In Prior HHS poverty guidelines and federal register references. ASPE, Retrieved April 28, 2023, from, https://aspe.hhs.gov/topics/poverty-economic-mobility/poverty-guidelines/prior-hhs-poverty-guidelines-federal-register-references Google Scholar
Urizar, G. G., & Muñoz, R. F. (2011). Impact of a prenatal cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention on salivary cortisol levels in low-income mothers and their infants. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(10), 14801494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.04.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vergara-Lopez, C., Bublitz, M. H., Mercado, N., Ziobrowski, H. N., Gomez, A., & Stroud, L. R. (2021). Early life stress and latent trait cortisol in adolescent girls: A prospective examination. Stress-the International Journal On the Biology of Stress, 24(6), 10751081. https://doi.org/10.1080/10253890.2021.1998445 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlasova, R. M., Iosif, A. M., Ryan, A. M., Funk, L. H., Murai, T., Chen, S., Lesh, T. A., Rowland, D. J., Bennett, J., Hogrefe, C. E., Maddock, R. J., Gandal, M. J., Geschwind, D. H., Schumann, C. M., Van de Water, J., McAllister, A. K., Carter, C. S., Styner, M. A., Amaral, D. G., & Bauman, M. D. (2021). Maternal immune activation during pregnancy alters postnatal brain growth and cognitive development in nonhuman primate offspring. Journal of Neuroscience, 41(48), 99719987. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-21.2021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (4th ed.). Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Weir, R. K., Forghany, R., Smith, S. E., Patterson, P. H., McAllister, A. K., Schumann, C. M., & Bauman, M. D. (2015). Preliminary evidence of neuropathology in nonhuman primates prenatally exposed to maternal immune activation. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 48, 139146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2015.03.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weitzman, E. D., Fukushima, D., Nogeire, C., Roffwarg, H., Gallagher, T. F., & Hellman, L. (1971). Twenty-four hour pattern of the episodic secretion of cortisol in normal subjects. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 33, 1422. https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-33-1-14 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieczorek, A., Perani, C. V., Nixon, M., Constancia, M., Sandovici, I., Zazara, D. E., Leone, G., Zhang, M., Arck, P., & Solano, M. E. (2019). Sex-specific regulation of stress-induced fetal glucocorticoid surge by the mouse placenta. American Journal of Physiology-Endocrinology and Metabolism, 317(1), E109E120. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpendo.00551.2018 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Womack, S., Beam, C., Giangrande, E., Tong, X. C., Scharf, R. J., Finkel, D., & Davis, D. (2024). Co-recovery of physical size and cognitive ability from infancy to adolescence: A twin study. Child Development, 95(4), 13671383. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.14079 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Womack, S. R., Beam, C. R., Davis, D. W., Finkel, D., & Turkheimer, E. (2022). Genetic and environmental correlates of the nonlinear recovery of cognitive ability in twins. Developmental Psychology, 58(3), 535550. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, H., McCoach, D. B., Gottfried, A. W., & Gottfried, A. E. (2018). Stability of intelligence from infancy through adolescence: An autoregressive latent variable model. Intelligence, 69, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2018.03.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zijlmans, M. A., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & de Weerth, C. (2015). Associations between maternal prenatal cortisol concentrations and child outcomes: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 53, 124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.02.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conceptual diagrams of competing theoretical models.

Figure 1

Table 1. Sample collection times and raw cortisol measurements

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the study sample

Figure 3

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between maternal prenatal cortisol and infant mental development

Figure 4

Table 4. Parameter estimates of the association between cortisol AUCg across pregnancy and Bayley-III scores at 6, 12, and 24 months

Figure 5

Table 5. Parameter estimates of the association between cortisol AUCg across pregnancy and executive functioning scores at 24 months

Supplementary material: File

Womack et al. supplementary material

Womack et al. supplementary material
Download Womack et al. supplementary material(File)
File 6 MB