Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T15:25:55.018Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Longitudinal panel networks of risk and protective factors for early adolescent suicidality in the ABCD sample

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2024

Gemma T. Wallace*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI, USA
Bradley T. Conner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Colorado State University, Fort Collins, CO, USA
*
Corresponding author: Gemma Tierney Wallace; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Rates of youth suicidal thoughts and behaviors (STBs) are rising, and younger age at onset increases vulnerability to negative outcomes. However, few studies have investigated STBs in early adolescence (ages 10–13), and accurate prediction of youth STBs remains poor. Network analyses that can examine pairwise associations between many theoretically relevant variables may identify complex pathways of risk for early adolescent STBs. The present study applied longitudinal network analysis to examine interrelations between STBs and several previously identified risk and protective factors. Data came from 9,854 youth in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study cohort (Mage = 9.90 ± .62 years, 63% white, 53% female at baseline). Youth and their caregivers completed an annual measurement battery between ages 9–10 through 11–12 years. Panel Graphical Vector Autoregressive models evaluated associations between STBs and several mental health symptoms, socioenvironmental factors, life stressors, and substance use. In the contemporaneous and between-subjects networks, direct associations were observed between STBs and internalizing symptoms, substance use, family conflict, lower parental monitoring, and lower school protective factors. Potential indirect pathways of risk for STBs were also observed. Age-specific interventions may benefit from prioritizing internalizing symptoms and early substance use, as well as promoting positive school and family support.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Suicidal thoughts and behaviors (STBs) are a critical public health problem among youth worldwide, and especially in the United States (National Institute of Mental Health, 2023). Suicide is the second-leading cause of death among individuals aged 10–14, and rates of death by suicide among adolescents (ages 15–19) alarmingly increased by 29% between 2014 and 2020 (United Health Foundation, 2023). STBs include suicidal ideation (i.e., thoughts about suicide) and suicidal behaviors (e.g., preparatory acts and interrupted, actual, and aborted suicide attempts) (Posner et al., Reference Posner, Brown, Stanley, Brent, Yershova, Oquendo, Currier, Melvin, Greenhill, Shen and Mann2011). Most youth STB research to date has been conducted in samples aged 13–18, but rates of STBs begin to increase after approximately age 10 (Nock et al., Reference Nock, Green, Hwang, McLaughlin, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2013). Although recent studies have investigated childhood STBs (DeVille et al., Reference DeVille, Whalen, Breslin, Morris, Khalsa, Paulus and Barch2020; e.g., Harman et al., Reference Harman, Kliamovich, Morales, Gilbert, Barch, Mooney, Feldstein Ewing, Fair and Nagel2021; Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020; Raffagnato et al., Reference Raffagnato, Iannattone, Fasolato, Parolin, Ravaglia, Biscalchin, Traverso, Zanato, Miscioscia and Gatta2022), there is still a relative paucity of studies on STBs in early adolescence (ages 10–13) compared to older age groups (Ayer et al., Reference Ayer, Colpe, Pearson, Rooney and Murphy2020; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). Younger age at onset for STBs increases vulnerability to subsequent severe suicidality and mental health difficulties (e.g., Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Dewa and Phare2012). Thus, improving understanding of risk for STBs in early adolescence could inform early intervention efforts that could have protective impacts across the lifespan (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Copeland et al., Reference Copeland, Goldston and Costello2017; Reinherz et al., Reference Reinherz, Tanner, Berger, Beardslee and Fitzmaurice2006).

In alignment with the Social-Ecological Framework, youth STBs are influenced by interrelations between risk and protective factors that occur at the individual (e.g., psychological constructs), interpersonal (e.g., family relationships), and environmental (e.g., neighborhood factors) levels (Cramer & Kapusta, Reference Cramer and Kapusta2017). Thus, it is important to consider risk and protective factors from multiple levels, including both individual differences and social-environmental factors, when assessing STB risk (see Carballo et al. (Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020), Cha et al. (Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018), and de Sousa et al. (Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017) for reviews of STB risk and protective factors among youth). At the individual level, mental health symptoms are often identified as the most common risk factors for youth STBs. Depressive and other internalizing symptoms (e.g., negative cognitions) are salient risk factors (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018); however, STBs are a transdiagnostic phenomenon and have also been associated with sleep disturbances, externalizing, attention, thought, social, and other mental health symptoms (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020). Substance use is another common individual risk factor for youth STBs, which also occurs transdiagnostically across mental health symptoms (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020). At the interpersonal and environmental levels, several factors are robust correlates of youth STBs. In early adolescence, the family and school environment appear to have the largest impact on youth STBs, with lower family conflict, higher parental monitoring, and greater school engagement and support protecting against STBs (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Fotti et al., Reference Fotti, Katz, Afifi and Cox2006; Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Esposito-Smythers and Leichtweis2015; Sedgwick et al., Reference Sedgwick, Epstein, Dutta and Ougrin2019; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). Peer and friendship factors appear to become more important as youth transition into adolescence (Telzer et al., Reference Telzer, van Hoorn, Rogers and Do2018) and may be less salient during childhood and early adolescence. Additionally, youth STBs are often preceded by social-environmental stressors and adversity, such as negative life events (e.g., maltreatment, trauma, loss, and social stressors (e.g., bullying)), financial and material hardship, and perceptions of lower safety in a youth’s neighborhood (e.g., Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; King et al., Reference King, Schwab-Stone, Flisher, Greenwald, Kramer, Goodman, Lahey, Shaffer and Gould2001; Pan & Spittal, Reference Pan and Spittal2013).

However, despite rigorous literature identifying these risk and protective factors, the etiological mechanisms underlying youth STBs remain unclear (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018) and accurate prediction of future STBs is poor (Belsher et al., Reference Belsher, Smolenski, Pruitt, Bush, Beech, Workman, Morgan, Evatt, Tucker and Skopp2019; Franklin et al., Reference Franklin, Ribeiro, Fox, Bentley, Kleiman, Huang, Musacchio, Jaroszewski, Chang and Nock2017; Millner et al., Reference Millner, Robinaugh and Nock2020). This may be related to constraints of common study designs in psychiatric research that can obfuscate important information, including cross-sectional data (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Guzmán et al., Reference Guzmán, Cha, Ribeiro and Franklin2019), case-control frameworks (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Ambler, Danese, Elliott, Hariri, Harrington, Hogan, Poulton, Ramrakha, Rasmussen, Reuben, Richmond-Rakerd, Sugden, Wertz, Williams and Moffitt2020), and use of predictive models that cannot account for interrelations between many risk and protective factors (de Beurs, Reference de Beurs2017). STBs appear to result not from one or a few predisposing factors, but from complex interplay between many risk and protective factors (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Fazel & Runeson, Reference Fazel and Runeson2020; Millner et al., Reference Millner, Robinaugh and Nock2020; de Beurs, Reference de Beurs2017). Thus, considering how risk and protective factors from multiple life domains interrelate with each other to influence STBs could increase understanding of how risk for STBs emerges and progresses across early adolescence (Cramer & Kapusta, Reference Cramer and Kapusta2017).

The network approach to psychopathology offers a useful framework for studying how interrelations between different risk and protective factors may influence STBs. The network approach posits that instead of resulting from one shared cause, mental health symptoms may have causal relations with each other (Borsboom, Reference Borsboom2017; Fried et al., Reference Fried, van Borkulo, Cramer, Boschloo, Schoevers and Borsboom2017). Thus, the network approach to psychopathology conceptualizes mental health concerns as emergent from interactive systems of psychological factors. The network approach is based on graph theory, in which psychological variables represent nodes and the pairwise associations between nodes (i.e., the strength of the association between each pair of variables) represent edges (Menczer et al., Reference Menczer, Fortunato and Davis2020). The network structure can be visualized graphically and statistically analyzed to identify patterns among the nodes (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018; Hevey, Reference Hevey2018; Menczer et al., Reference Menczer, Fortunato and Davis2020). While network models cannot be interpreted as causal because they are based on observational data, they can be used to generate causal hypotheses about complex interrelations between several variables of interest (Borsboom & Cramer, Reference Borsboom and Cramer2013).

Approaching the study of STBs from a network perspective may increase understanding of how complex risk and protective pathways emerge (de Beurs, Reference de Beurs2017). By estimating bivariate associations between each pair of variables while adjusting for all other variables in the model, network models provide valuable information about the structure of relationships between variables in high-dimensional data. This multivariate structure is not as clearly revealed in other analytic approaches; while multiple regression models provide similar statistical information regarding the prediction of specific outcomes, network models have the advantage of providing clear and powerful visualizations of the multivariate structure for all variables in the model (Borsboom et al., Reference Borsboom, Deserno, Rhemtulla, Epskamp, Fried, McNally, Robinaugh, Perugini, Dalege, Costantini, Isvoranu, Wysocki, van Borkulo, van Bork and Waldorp2021). This can identify nodes that are highly or sparsely associated with the other nodes in the network (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018; Menczer et al., Reference Menczer, Fortunato and Davis2020). For example, certain nodes may have strong connections with many other nodes, suggesting they may have the ability to activate or suppress the network as a whole. Other nodes may have only one or a few connections with other nodes and may peripherally influence (or be influenced by) the network (Borgatti, Reference Borgatti2005). Thus, network models can provide information about complex patterns of conditional dependence in multivariate data. The network approach may therefore help to map both direct and potential indirect pathways of risk among previously identified risk and protective factors for STBs (Shiratori et al., Reference Shiratori, Tachikawa, Nemoto, Endo, Aiba, Matsui and Asada2014; de Beurs, Reference de Beurs2017).

A small but growing body of literature has applied network approaches to the study of youth STBs (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Díez-Gómez, de la Barrera, Sebastian-Enesco, Ortuño-Sierra, Montoya-Castilla, Lucas-Molina, Inchausti and Pérez-Albéniz2020, Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Al-Halabí, Pérez-Albéniz and Debbané2022; Gijzen et al., Reference Gijzen, Rasing, Creemers, Smit, Engels and De Beurs2021; Li & Kwok, Reference Li and Kwok2023; Ou et al., Reference Ou, Yang, Chen, Li, Yang, Lu, Li, Huang, Ma, Lv, Zhao, Qing, Ju and Zhang2023; Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Cheng, Su, Xu, Zhang, Huang, Sun, Wang, Gong and Zhou2023). Most studies to date have focused on STBs’ relations with psychological constructs only, such as mental health symptoms and cognitive-affective constructs (e.g., Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Díez-Gómez, de la Barrera, Sebastian-Enesco, Ortuño-Sierra, Montoya-Castilla, Lucas-Molina, Inchausti and Pérez-Albéniz2020; Gijzen et al., Reference Gijzen, Rasing, Creemers, Smit, Engels and De Beurs2021; Li & Kwok, Reference Li and Kwok2023). For example, cross-sectional associations were observed between suicidal ideation, depressive symptoms, affect, and self-esteem in a sample aged 13–16 years (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Díez-Gómez, de la Barrera, Sebastian-Enesco, Ortuño-Sierra, Montoya-Castilla, Lucas-Molina, Inchausti and Pérez-Albéniz2020). In another study, subjective happiness and hopelessness, moderated by self-efficacy, were prospectively associated with STBs across two timepoints among adolescents aged 9–15 years (Li & Kwok, Reference Li and Kwok2023). Less research has extended network models to consider domains of STB risk and protective factors other than psychological constructs (e.g., socioenvironmental factors and stressors). Cross-sectional network studies have identified direct and potential indirect associations between STBs, mental health symptoms, and problems with peers and/or bullying in samples of adolescents aged 12–20 (Zhong et al., Reference Zhong, Cheng, Su, Xu, Zhang, Huang, Sun, Wang, Gong and Zhou2023) and 14–80 (Fonseca-Pedrero et al., Reference Fonseca-Pedrero, Al-Halabí, Pérez-Albéniz and Debbané2022). Another recent study estimated cross-sectional networks of adolescent psychological constructs and parenting styles (Ou et al., Reference Ou, Yang, Chen, Li, Yang, Lu, Li, Huang, Ma, Lv, Zhao, Qing, Ju and Zhang2023). These important studies substantiate that STBs involve complex systems of risk and protective factors. However, the cross-sectional or two-timepoint designs of previous research preclude evaluating longitudinal and within-person associations between STBs and risk and protective factors. To our knowledge, no previous research has employed longitudinal network analyses to investigate how risk and protective factors from multiple life domains relate to STBs across early adolescence.

Present study

The goal of this study was to examine how risk and protective factors from multiple life domains relate to each other, both within and across domains, and to STBs during the transitional period from childhood into early adolescence. We applied a longitudinal network perspective to examine pairwise relations between several previously identified correlates of STBs in a cohort of 9,854 youth followed from ages 9–10 to 11–12 years as part of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. We evaluated several constructs that have been robustly identified as STB risk and protective factors, including mental health symptoms (internalizing, externalizing, attention problems, social problems, thought problems, and sleep problems), socioenvironmental factors (family conflict, parental monitoring, and school protective factors), stressors (stressful life events, material hardship, and neighborhood safety), and substance use (e.g., Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). Most of these constructs have also been identified as STB risk and protective factors in cross-sectional studies of the ABCD sample specifically, increasing confidence in their salience (DeVille et al., Reference DeVille, Whalen, Breslin, Morris, Khalsa, Paulus and Barch2020; Harman et al., Reference Harman, Kliamovich, Morales, Gilbert, Barch, Mooney, Feldstein Ewing, Fair and Nagel2021; Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020; van Velzen et al., Reference van Velzen, Toenders, Avila-Parcet, Dinga, Rabinowitz, Campos, Jahanshad, Rentería and Schmaal2021). To our knowledge, this study represents the first application of a longitudinal panel network approach to examine early adolescent suicidality. Illuminating pathways of direct and potential indirect risk for early onset STBs could identify early predisposing factors and inform the timing and targets of specific interventions. Given that network models are data driven, we did not make causal hypotheses about effect directions and expected that the models would identify new and complex relations among the risk and protective factors for STBs.

Methods

Participants and procedures

The ABCD study design and protocols have been described in detail elsewhere (Auchter et al., Reference Auchter, Hernandez Mejia, Heyser, Shilling, Jernigan, Brown, Tapert and Dowling2018; Feldstein Ewing et al., Reference Feldstein Ewing, Chang, Cottler, Tapert, Dowling and Brown2018; Garavan et al., Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa, Jernigan, Potter, Thompson and Zahs2018; Karcher & Barch, Reference Karcher and Barch2021; Saragosa-Harris et al., Reference Saragosa-Harris, Chaku, MacSweeney, Guazzelli Williamson, Scheuplein, Feola, Cardenas-Iniguez, Demir-Lira, McNeilly, Huffman, Whitmore, Michalska, Damme, Rakesh and Mills2022). Currently in its fifth year of data collection, the same cohort will be followed longitudinally for 10 years, from ages 9–10 to 19–20 years. A comprehensive measurement battery is administered annually, with parent- and youth-report measures of mental and physical health, culture and environment, neurocognition, and substance use. Study protocols are provided at https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/protocols/. The present study used data from the first three annual waves: Baseline (aged 9–10 years, N = 11,878), Year 1 (aged 10–11 years, N = 11,235), and Year 2 (aged 11–12 years, N = 10,414).

The ABCD study oversampled siblings (Garavan et al., Reference Garavan, Bartsch, Conway, Decastro, Goldstein, Heeringa, Jernigan, Potter, Thompson and Zahs2018; Karcher & Barch, Reference Karcher and Barch2021), and the baseline sample included 8,150 singletons, 1,600 non-twin siblings, 2,100 twins, and 30 triplets (Palmer et al., Reference Palmer, Sheth, Marshall, Adise, Baker, Chang, Clark, Coronado, Dagher, Diaz, Dowling, Gonzalez, Haist, Herting, Huber, Jernigan, LeBlanc, Lee, Lisdahl, Neigh, Patterson, Renshaw, Rhee, Tapert, Thompson, Uban, Sowell and Yurgelun-Todd2021). To avoid nested data within households, one sibling was randomly selected from each family (i.e., the sample consisted of unrelated participants). This resulted in an analytic sample of N = 9,854 at baseline, N = 9,286 at Year 1, and N = 8,629 at Year 2. Table 1 presents demographic characteristics and rates of STBs in the analytic sample, as well as the full ABCD sample before dropping sibling participants. Sample characteristics were highly consistent across the original and analytic samples. At each timepoint, 8.0–8.7% of youth reported any form of STBs.

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics and STB endorsement across study timepoints

Note: 1 The analytic sample. 2 The original ABCD sample, before randomly dropping sibling participants. 3 Variables only measured at baseline. Gender and sexual identities were self-reported by youth; all other demographics items were assessed by parent/caregiver report. Rates of missingness in the analytic sample increased over time due to study attrition. STBs = suicidal thoughts and behaviors; AIAN/NHPI = American Indian/Alaska Native or Native Hawaiian and other Pacific Islander; HS = high school.

Measures

The ABCD measurement battery has been described in detail (see Barch et al. (Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz, Hudziak, Jernigan, Tapert, Yurgelun-Todd, Alia-Klein, Potter, Paulus, Prouty, Zucker and Sher2017) for details on the mental and physical health measures, Lisdahl et al. (Reference Lisdahl, Sher, Conway, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Nixon, Tapert, Bartsch, Goldstein and Heitzeg2018) for substance use measures, Zucker et al. (Reference Zucker, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Paulus, Arroyo, Fuligni, Morris, Sanchez and Wills2018) for socioenvironmental measures, and Hoffman et al. (Reference Hoffman, Clark, Orendain, Hudziak, Squeglia and Dowling2019) for stress exposure measures). Our analyses included constructs available in the ABCD data that (1) previous literature identified as a theoretically relevant risk or protective factor for youth STBs, and (2) had available data from all three annual waves and from one consistent reporter, either parent or youth (see Table 2 for the list of variables included in the present study).

Table 2. ABCD measures, reporter, and data availability of variables used in the current study

Note: 1 Due to inconsistent data availability over time, stressful life events were coded as a composite score of items measuring traumatic and/or negative significant life events from the KSADS PTSD module and Life Events Scale (see Measures).

Suicidal thoughts and behaviors

Youth report of STBs was measured with the Suicide Module of the computerized Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for the DSM-5 (KSADS-COMP) (KSAD-COMP LLC, 2024 Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Kobak, Kearney, Milham, Andreotti, Escalera, Alexander, Gill, Birmaher, Sylvester, Rice, Deep and Kaufman2020). Participants responded to binary items assessing the presence (1) or absence (0) of nine STBs: passive suicidal ideation, active but non-specific suicidal ideation, suicidal ideation with a specific method, active suicidal ideation with intent, active suicidal ideation with a plan, preparatory actions toward suicidal behavior, interrupted suicide attempt(s), aborted suicidal attempt(s), and suicide attempt(s). While different forms of STBs have different implications for intervention (e.g., passive ideation versus attempt) (Klonsky et al., Reference Klonsky, Qiu and Saffer2017; Nock et al., Reference Nock, Green, Hwang, McLaughlin, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2013), any level of STBs in this age range is clinically concerning and confers vulnerability to long-term negative outcomes (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Thompson et al., Reference Thompson, Dewa and Phare2012). Thus, suicidality was modeled as one variable reflecting a count of the number of STB items a youth endorsed at each wave (i.e., ever at baseline, or since the previous measurement occasion at Years 1–2). Previous research using the ABCD sample used a similar coding scheme, in which suicidality was defined as endorsement of one or more KSADS STB items (Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020).

Mental health symptoms

Parent/caregiver report of their child’s mental health symptoms over the past 6 months was measured with the ASEBA Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach2009). Parents responded to each item on a 3-point scale of 0 = not true (as far as you know), 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, and 2 = very true or often true. Raw scores were calculated for the following CBCL Syndrome Scales (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach2009): Social Problems (11 items, e.g., “Complains of loneliness”; within ω = .53, between ω = .85), Thought Problems (11 items, e.g., “Can’t get their mind off certain thoughts”; within ω = .44, between ω = .82), and Attention Problems (10 items, e.g., “Fails to finish things they start”; within ω = .69, between ω = .94). Internalizi ng was a sum of 12 items from the Anxious/Depressive syndrome scale (e.g., “Fears going to school”), eight items from the Withdrawn/Depressed scale (e.g., “Would rather be alone than with others”), and 10 items from the Somatic Complaints scale (e.g., “Feels dizzy or lightheaded”) (30 items total; within ω = .80, between ω = .93). Externalizing was a sum of 14 items from the Rule Breaking scale (e.g., “Doesn’t seem to feel guilty after misbehaving”) and 18 items from the Aggressive Behavior scale (e.g., “Cruelty, bullying, or meanness to others”) (32 items total; within ω = .85, between ω = .96) (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach2009). To avoid latent confounding (Hallquist et al., Reference Hallquist, Wright and Molenaar2021), CBCL items that directly overlapped with other study variables were omitted, including the two STB items, three substance use items, and two sleep problem items. Coding schemes for the CBCL variables are provided in the Appendix.

Parent/caregiver reports of their child’s sleep problems over the past 6 months were measured using the Sleep Disturbances Scale for Children (SDSC) (Bruni et al., Reference Bruni, Ottaviano, Guidetti, Romoli, Innocenzi, Cortesi and Giannotti1996), a 26-item scale assessing six domains of sleep-related problems (e.g., “The child has difficulty getting to sleep at night”). Parents responded to items on a 5-point scale of 1 = never to 5 = always (daily), where higher values reflected more sleep difficulties. S leep problems were modeled as a total sum score of the 26 SDSC items (within ω = .82, between ω = .85).

Socioenvironmental factors

Youth report of perceived family conflict was assessed with the PhenX Family Conflict subscale of the Family Environment Subscale (Moos & Moos, Reference Moos and Moos1994). Youth responded to nine items on a binary scale where 0 = false and 1 = true (e.g., “We fight a lot in our family”). Family conflict was modeled as a sum score of the nine items (within ω = .51, between ω = .90), with higher scores representing higher levels of family conflict.

Youth report of perceived parental monitoring was measured with the ABCD Parental Monitoring Survey (Karoly et al., Reference Karoly, Callahan, Schmiege and Feldstein Ewing2016; Stattin & Kerr, Reference Stattin and Kerr2003). Youth responded to five items on a 5-point Likert-style scale of 1 = never to 5 = always or almost always (e.g., “How often do your parents/guardians know where you are?”). Parental monitoring was modeled as a sum score of the five items, and items were reverse coded such that higher values reflected lower parental monitoring (within ω = .35, between ω = .75).

Youth report of school protective factors was measured using the PhenX School Risk and Protective Factors Survey (SRPF) (Arthur et al., Reference Arthur, Briney, Hawkins, Abbott, Brooke-Weiss and Catalano2007). The SRPF includes three subscales that measure youth’s perception of their school environment (6 items, e.g., “In my school, students have lots of chances to help decide things like class activities and rules”), school involvement (4 items, e.g., “In general, I like school a lot”), and school disengagement (2 items, e.g., “Usually, school bores me”). Youth responded on a 4-point scale of 1 = NO!, 2 = no, 3 = yes, and 4 = YES! School protective factors was a sum score of the 12 SRPF items, where higher values represented higher levels of school protective factors (school disengagement items were reverse coded; within ω = .77, between ω = .89).

Stressors

Parent/caregiver report of neighborhood safety was assessed using the PhenX Neighborhood Safety/Crime Survey (Echeverria et al., Reference Echeverria, Diez-Roux and Link2004; Mujahid et al., Reference Mujahid, Diez Roux, Morenoff and Raghunathan2007). Parents responded to three items on a 5-point Likert-style scale of 1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree (e.g., “I feel safe walking in my neighborhood, day or night”). Neighborhood safety was a sum score of the three items (within ω = .77, between ω = .95); items were reverse coded such that higher scores represented lower neighborhood safety.

Parent/caregiver report of family material hardship was measured in the PhenX Demographics Survey (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Albaugh, Avenevoli, Chang, Clark, Glantz, Hudziak, Jernigan, Tapert, Yurgelun-Todd, Alia-Klein, Potter, Paulus, Prouty, Zucker and Sher2017). Parents responded to seven binary items about types of material hardship their family had experienced in the past 12 months due to financial difficulties (0 = no, 1 = yes; e.g., “In the past 12 months, has there been a time when you and your immediate family needed food but couldn’t afford to buy it or couldn’t afford to go out to get it?”). Material hardship was a count of the number of material hardship items parents/caregivers endorsed, where higher values reflected more types of material hardship.

Parent/caregiver report of stressful life events their child experienced was measured as a composite of items from the KSADS-COMP Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) module (KSAD-COMP LLC, 2024 Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Kobak, Kearney, Milham, Andreotti, Escalera, Alexander, Gill, Birmaher, Sylvester, Rice, Deep and Kaufman2020) and the Life Events Scale (LES) (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Compas, Thurm, McMahon and Gipson2004; Hoffman et al., Reference Hoffman, Clark, Orendain, Hudziak, Squeglia and Dowling2019). The KSADS PTSD module asked parents if their child had experienced 17 types of potentially traumatic events (0 = no, 1 = yes; e.g., “Learned about the sudden unexpected death of a loved one”). The KSADS PTSD module was scored as the number of events parents endorsed their child having ever experienced at baseline, or since the previous measurement occasion at Year 2. The LES similarly assessed parents’ knowledge about 26 significant life experiences their child experienced in the past year, and if the event was “mostly good” or “mostly bad” (e.g., “Someone in family died”). The LES was scored as the number of “mostly bad” events parents endorsed for their child. The KSADS PTSD module was administered to parents at baseline and Year 2 (i.e., not available for Year 1). The LES was administered to parents at Years 1–2 only (i.e., not available at baseline). Given that stressful life events are salient risk factors for STBs, these measures were integrated to provide a proxy for youth’s stressful life events at all three waves. Stressful life events were modeled as the KSADS PTSD score at baseline, the LES score at Year 1, and the mean of the KSADS PTSD and LES scores at Year 2. The raw KSADS PTSD and LES scores were positively correlated (ρ = 0.37), and each score was standardized to account for different response scales on the two measures. While the KSADS PTSD module and LES have content overlap for several items, they do not assess identical stressors. Results that involve this variable should be interpreted with these considerations in mind.

Substance use

Youth report of substance use was measured by the ABCD Timeline Follow-back Interview (TLFB) (Lisdahl et al., Reference Lisdahl, Sher, Conway, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Nixon, Tapert, Bartsch, Goldstein and Heitzeg2018). The young age of participants (10–12 years) is before age at onset for most substance use, and rates of engagement in moderate substance use were low in the ABCD cohort (Lisdahl et al., Reference Lisdahl, Sher, Conway, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Nixon, Tapert, Bartsch, Goldstein and Heitzeg2018; Martz et al., Reference Martz, Heitzeg, Lisdahl, Cloak, Ewing, Gonzalez, Haist, LeBlanc, Madden, Ross, Sher, Tapert, Thompson and Wade2022). Thus, we focused on low-level use (e.g., a sip or puff) of the three most used substances: alcohol, nicotine, and cannabis (Lisdahl et al., 2021; Martz et al., Reference Martz, Heitzeg, Lisdahl, Cloak, Ewing, Gonzalez, Haist, LeBlanc, Madden, Ross, Sher, Tapert, Thompson and Wade2022). Youth were first asked if they had ever heard of several substances. If they responded yes, they were then asked if they had tried each substance (0 = no, 1 = yes). Alcohol use was assessed with “Have you ever tried a sip of alcohol such as beer, wine or liquor (rum, vodka, gin, whiskey)?” Nicotine use was assessed with “Have you ever tried a puff from a tobacco or electronic cigarette, Juul, vape pens, e-hookah, cigar or pipe?” Cannabis use was assessed with “Have you ever tried a puff or eaten any marijuana, also called pot, grass, weed or ganja?” Substance use was modeled as a count of the three substances youth reported having tried (measured as ever having used at baseline, or having used since the last measurement occasion at Years 1–2).

Statistical analyses

Data wrangling and analyses were conducted in R version 4.3.0 (R Core Team, 2023). Within-person omegas are typically lower than between-person omegas in multilevel data, especially when the number of measurement occasions is small (three in the present study) (Geldhof et al., Reference Geldhof, Preacher and Zyphur2014; Rush & Hofer, Reference Rush and Hofer2017). All variables were assessed for multivariate normality and stationarity assumptions, and multilevel omegas were calculated for scale-scored variables (Wiley, Reference Wiley2020). Prior to analysis, all variables were standardized (i.e., z-scored) across all three waves of data (while in a long format). Because youth age is a theoretically relevant covariate of both STBs and the risk and protective factors (e.g., Smetana & Rote, Reference Smetana and Rote2019), data were detrended for the linear effects of age. A series of bivariate linear regressions were conducted in which each variable was regressed on age, and the resulting residuals for each variable were modeled as nodes in the networks (Burger et al., Reference Burger, Hoekstra, Mansueto and Epskamp2022; Fried et al., Reference Fried, Papanikolaou and Epskamp2022; Ram et al., Reference Ram, Brose and Molenaar2013). Regression coefficients for age predicting each variable are shown in Supplemental Table S1. Sensitivity analyses using non-detrended versions of study variables indicated that detrending did not meaningfully impact the results (O’Driscoll et al., Reference O’Driscoll, Epskamp, Fried, Saunders, Cardoso, Stott, Wheatley, Cirkovic, Naqvi, Buckman and Pilling2022).

Network analyses were conducted using Panel Graphical Vector Autoregressive (Panel GVAR) models via the psychonetrics package (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020, Reference Epskamp2022), which enables estimation of temporal network models in panel data with three or more timepoints. We encourage readers to review Epskamp (Reference Epskamp2020) for detailed descriptions of Panel GVAR models and example R code for the psychonetrics package. Panel GVAR models integrate multivariate modeling with Gaussian Graphical Models, a type of network model for continuous data, and Graphical Vector Autoregressive Models, a type of model for estimating multilevel temporal effects (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020). In our Panel GVAR model, the network nodes (i.e., variables) were suicidality and the 13 different risk and protective factors (14 nodes total, listed in Table 2). All nodes were modeled as observed (i.e., non-latent) variables. Network edges represented the statistical associations between each pair of nodes after adjusting for all other variables in the model. In our Panel GVAR model, all edges were estimated as fixed (i.e., not allowed to randomly vary across participants) partial correlations between each pair of nodes (Epskamp & Fried, Reference Epskamp and Fried2018). Undirected edges represented bidirectional partial correlations between variables within the same timepoint, while directed edges represented predictive (unidirectional) partial correlations between variables across timepoints (Hevey, Reference Hevey2018).

Panel GVAR models simultaneously estimate three network structures: temporal, contemporaneous, and between-subjects networks (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020; Jordan et al., Reference Jordan, Winer and Salem2020). The temporal network provides directed within-person lagged partial correlations and autocorrelations across timepoints. Edges in the temporal network are considered predictive because they represent a variable from a previous timepoint predicting either itself (autocorrelation) or another variable (bivariate correlation) at a subsequent timepoint. The contemporaneous network provides undirected within-person partial correlations between each pair of nodes within the same timepoint, after controlling for temporal effects. In other words, these are bidirectional associations rather than predictive relations. Lastly, the between-subjects network provides undirected partial correlations that reflect associations between overall variable means across all timepoints. Again, these are bidirectional associations rather than predictive relations (see Epskamp (Reference Epskamp2020) for detailed descriptions of the three network structures). Panel GVAR models used the nlminb optimizer (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2022), and missing data was handled using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML). Network structures were visualized using qgraph (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom2012, Reference Epskamp, Costantini, Halsbeck, Isvoranu, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Boorsboom2023).

Model selection and evaluation

Model fit was evaluated using multiple indices: root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and Bayesian information criterion (BIC). Model χ 2 was also reported but not interpreted due to sample size sensitivity (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013). Panel GVAR models are relatively new, and there are not well-established criteria for evaluating model fit, but recommendations for structural equation models can be used as guidelines (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020; O’Driscoll et al., Reference O’Driscoll, Epskamp, Fried, Saunders, Cardoso, Stott, Wheatley, Cirkovic, Naqvi, Buckman and Pilling2022). To select the best-performing model, edge selection was conducted via recursive step-down and step-up model searches. In the step-down pruning step, edges not significant at p < .01. were iteratively removed and the model was refit with these edges fixed at 0 (Blanken et al., Reference Blanken, Isvoranu and Epskamp2022). In the subsequent step-up search, edges were iteratively added until the best-performing BIC was obtained (Blanken et al., Reference Blanken, Isvoranu and Epskamp2022; Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020). To assess stability of the edges, we used a 25% case-drop bootstrap resampling procedure (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020), in which 100 models were refit using a random 75% of the sample. Edges retained in higher proportions of bootstrapped models are considered more stable (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018).

Node importance was evaluated via centrality metrics, including node strength, betweenness, and closeness (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018; Epskamp & Fried, Reference Epskamp and Fried2018; Hevey, Reference Hevey2018). Node strength, calculated as the sum of all absolute edge weights connected to a node, represents how strongly a node directly relates to other nodes in the network. High strength suggests a node has strong relations with other variables in the network. Closeness and betweenness quantify a node’s indirect associations with all other nodes in the network. Closeness represents the average shortest path (measured by geodesic distance [Opsahl et al., Reference Opsahl, Agneessens and Skvoretz2010]) between a specific node and all other nodes. Betweenness represents the number of times a node is on the shortest path (measured by geodesic distance) between other nodes. High closeness and betweenness suggest nodes can indirectly influence and be influenced by other nodes in the model (Deserno et al., Reference Deserno, Isvoranu, Epskamp and Blanken2022; Hevey, Reference Hevey2018). All centrality indices were z-scored, and stability of the centrality indices was assessed in the case-drop bootstrap resampling procedure.

Results

Descriptive statistics and rates of missingness for study variables are presented in Table 3. Multilevel bivariate correlations and intraclass correlations for study variables are shown in Table 4. Variables were generally non-normal and had missing data for 6.41–8.60% of cases (Table 3). Although FIML assumes multivariate normality, simulation studies suggest it can be robust to non-normality, particularly in large sample sizes (Jobst et al., Reference Jobst, Heine, Auerswald and Moshagen2021). The bootstrapping procedure can be used to evaluate the stability of network analysis results when variables are non-normal (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020).

Table 3. Unstandardized descriptive statistics and missingness for variables across study timepoints (N = 9,854)

Note: Rates of missingness increased over time due to study attrition. SD = standard deviation.

Table 4. Multilevel correlations and intraclass correlations for study variables in the full sample (N = 9,854)

Note: Correlations represent Spearman’s rho coefficients, estimated using the psych package (Revelle, Reference Revelle2024).

Model results

The model demonstrated excellent fit to the data: BIC = 903970.85, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.96, χ 2 [df = 764] = 7304.65 (p < 0.05). Network structures are shown in Figure 1. Corresponding correlation coefficients for the temporal, contemporaneous, and between-subjects networks are presented in Tables 5 –6, respectively. The bootstrap inclusion probabilities for each edge are shown in Supplemental Table S2. Centrality indices, including strength, betweenness, and closeness, for the full sample model are shown in Figure 2. Supplemental Figure 1 presents average centrality values for the network structures across the 100 case-dropped random subsamples from the bootstrapped models. Given the emphasis of the present study, we focused our interpretation on direct and potential indirect associations between STBs and other nodes in the networks.

Figure 1. Pruned network structures for the panel GVAR model (N = 9,854). Edge color represents effect direction (blue = positive, red = negative), while edge thickness represents effect strength (darker, thicker edges denote larger effects). Edges not shown were pruned during model selection. (a) Arrows represent lagged directed partial correlations and autocorrelations in the temporal network. (b) Lines represent undirected partial correlations in the contemporaneous network. (c) Lines represent undirected partial correlations in the between-subjects network. Corresponding numeric results are presented in Tables 5– 6.

Figure 2. Node centrality metrics for the panel GVAR model in the full sample (N = 9,854). Centrality metrics are shown in the metric of z-scores. (a) In the temporal network, In-Strength centrality represents the sum of all incoming absolute edge weights to a node, while Out-Strength centrality represents the sum of outgoing absolute edge weights from a node. (b–c) In the contemporaneous and between-subjects networks, Strength centrality represents the sum of all absolute edge weights connected to a node. Closeness represents the average shortest path between a specific node and all other nodes. Betweenness represents the number of times a node is on the shortest path between other nodes (Hevey, Reference Hevey2018).

Table 5. Estimated directed partial correlations for the temporal network (N = 9,854)

Note: Values represent lagged and autoregressive (i.e., across timepoints) directed partial correlations at the within-person level. Values on the diagonal are autocorrelations. The psychonetrics package does not currently provide standard errors for partial directed correlations (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2022). See Figure 1a for graphical representation of the temporal network structure.

Table 6. Estimated undirected partial correlations for the contemporaneous (lower triangle) and between-subjects (upper triangle) networks (N = 9,854)

Note: Lower triangle values represent contemporaneous (i.e., same measurement occasion) undirected partial correlations at the within-person level after adjusting for temporal effects and other variables in the model. Upper triangle values represent undirected partial correlations at the between-person level (i.e., associations between overall variable means) after adjusting for other variables in the model. Standard errors are provided in parentheses. See Figures 1bc for graphical representation of the contemporaneous and between-subjects network structures.

In the pruned, estimated temporal network, autocorrelations were observed for all nodes in the network (Figure 1a; Table 5). The bootstrapping procedure indicated high stability of all autoregressive effects (observed in ≥ 92% of bootstrapped models; Supplemental Table S1). Thus, higher STBs at an earlier timepoint were associated with higher STBs at a later timepoint. Several lagged correlations were also observed between the mental health symptom, socioenvironmental, and stressor nodes. STBs were not directly associated with other nodes in the temporal network. With regard to centrality, attention problems, social problems, and thought problems had the highest in-strength centrality, indicating relatively strong predictive effects on other nodes in the model (Figure 2a). Externalizing, family conflict, and material hardship had the highest values for both out-strength and betweenness centrality, indicating they were most predicted by other nodes and had relatively strong indirect associations with other nodes in the network. However, centrality indices for the temporal network differed somewhat between the full sample and 25% case-drop bootstrapped models (Supplemental Figure 1), suggesting that node centrality for the temporal effects varied across different subsets of participants in the sample.

In the pruned, estimated contemporaneous network, direct associations were observed between STBs and five other nodes: internalizing symptoms, substance use, family conflict, lower parental monitoring, and lower school protective factors (Figure 1b; Table 6). The bootstrapping procedure indicated high stability of these direct effects (included in ≥ 89% of bootstrapped models; Supplemental Table S1). Potential indirect pathways to STBs were also observed. Internalizing was associated with the other five mental health symptom dimensions (externalizing, thought problems, attention problems, sleep problems, and social problems), material hardship, and stressful life events. Each of these internalizing edges was included in 100% of the bootstrapped models, suggesting internalizing may potentially be a pathway through which other mental health symptoms, material hardship, and stressful life events associate with suicidality. Further, pairwise edges were observed between family conflict, parental monitoring, school protective factors, and substance use, indicating elevation in one of these constructs associates with elevation in the others, which may increase overall risk for STBs. Mental health symptom nodes had the highest strength and closeness centrality, including internalizing, externalizing, attention problems, and social problems (Figure 2b). Internalizing, attention problems, and school protective factors displayed the highest betweenness centrality. The centrality indices for the contemporaneous network were largely consistent between the original model and the bootstrap resampling procedure (Supplemental Figure 1), indicating stability of these indices.

The overall structure of the between-subjects network was similar to the contemporaneous network, although additional edges were observed (Figure 1c; Table 6). STBs again had direct associations with internalizing symptoms, substance use, family conflict, lower parental monitoring, and lower school protective factors. An additional direct association was observed between STBs and stressful life events. The bootstrapping procedure generally indicated effect stability, although the edge between STBs and parental monitoring was only observed in 31% of the bootstrapped models (Supplemental Table S1). Internalizing, parental monitoring, and attention problems had the highest strength centrality (Figure 2c). Parental monitoring and attention problems displayed the highest closeness centrality, while parental monitoring, material hardship, and attention problems had the highest betweenness centrality. Centrality indices for the between-subjects network were highly consistent across the original model and the bootstrap resampling procedure (Supplemental Figure 1).

Discussion

The present study applied a longitudinal network approach to elucidate potential pathways of risk and protection for STBs during the critical developmental period from late childhood into early adolescence, an age range that has been relatively understudied in youth STB research. Models examined risk and protective factors for STBs from multiple salient life domains, including mental health symptoms, socioenvironmental factors, life stressors, and substance use. Risk and protective factors representing multiple life domains directly associated with STBs, highlighting the value of applying a social-ecological lens to youth STB research (Cramer & Kapusta, Reference Cramer and Kapusta2017). Further, potential indirect pathways to STBs were observed, in which constructs that are directly associated with STBs are also associated with other risk and protective factors in the network. By elucidating the multivariate structure of STBs and several risk and protective factors, our results identified potential pathways through which interrelations between different risk and protective factors may influence early adolescent STBs.

Effect timescales and sizes

It is important to consider timescales when interpreting longitudinal effects (Burger et al., Reference Burger, Hoekstra, Mansueto and Epskamp2022; Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020). In the present study, measurement occasions were spaced approximately 1 year apart. However, the timescales for the causal effects of many risk and protective factors on STBs may be shorter than 1 year (Berman & Silverman, Reference Berman and Silverman2014). For example, suicidality commonly co-occurs with internalizing symptoms (Goldston et al., Reference Goldston, Daniel, Erkanli, Reboussin, Mayfield, Frazier and Treadway2009). Past-year STBs would hence be expected to have a stronger association with internalizing symptoms assessed during the same timepoint (in this case, the CBCL measured past 6-month symptoms), compared to internalizing symptoms measured 1–2 years previously. The contemporaneous network structure is therefore most likely to capture the putatively causal associations between variables in our model (Burger et al., Reference Burger, Hoekstra, Mansueto and Epskamp2022). The lack of robust associations between STBs and other variables in the temporal networks indicates that the 1-year time-lag between measurement occasions may be too long to capture meaningful impacts of the other variables on STBs. Moreover, the presence of several consistent contemporaneous effects with mostly null temporal effects suggests that the effects of other variables on STBs may diminish over time. Thus, while it is important to control for temporal lagged and autoregressive effects in the networks (Usami et al., Reference Usami, Murayama and Hamaker2019), we primarily focus our interpretation on the contemporaneous network structure. Further, observed effect sizes were generally small (r < .3) in both full and partial correlations between study variables. This is consistent with other publications using the ABCD data, in which effect sizes are smaller than what would be expected given previous literature on the measured variables (Gonzalez et al., Reference Gonzalez, Thompson, Sanchez, Morris, Gonzalez, Feldstein Ewing, Mason, Arroyo, Howlett, Tapert and Zucker2021; Owens et al., Reference Owens, Potter, Hyatt, Albaugh, Thompson, Jernigan, Yuan, Hahn, Allgaier and Garavan2021).

Direct pathways of risk and protection for STBs

In contemporaneous network structure, STBs had direct, positive associations with five risk and protective factors: higher family conflict, lower parental monitoring, lower school protective factors, higher internalizing symptoms, and higher substance use. While these risk and protective factors have been robustly identified in previous STB literature, observing these effects in network models bolsters confidence in their importance; these effects were consistent after controlling for all other pairwise associations in the networks. Thus, results emphasize that family and school environments serve as contexts for salient social experiences in this age range (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Fotti et al., Reference Fotti, Katz, Afifi and Cox2006; Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Esposito-Smythers and Leichtweis2015; Sedgwick et al., Reference Sedgwick, Epstein, Dutta and Ougrin2019; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). Youth who perceived higher levels of expressed emotion and aggression in their family, and their caregivers being less involved in and aware of their daily activities, were more likely to report STBs. Additionally, youth who reported lower engagement and support in their schools were at greater risk for STBs.

It is interesting that internalizing symptoms were directly associated with STBs while other mental health symptom dimensions were not. Previous literature has emphasized the transdiagnostic and comorbid nature of STBs (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018), and other mental health symptom dimensions (e.g., externalizing) have been associated with STBs in studies of the ABCD cohort (DeVille et al., Reference DeVille, Whalen, Breslin, Morris, Khalsa, Paulus and Barch2020; Harman et al., Reference Harman, Kliamovich, Morales, Gilbert, Barch, Mooney, Feldstein Ewing, Fair and Nagel2021; Janiri et al., Reference Janiri, Doucet, Pompili, Sani, Luna, Brent and Frangou2020; van Velzen et al., Reference van Velzen, Toenders, Avila-Parcet, Dinga, Rabinowitz, Campos, Jahanshad, Rentería and Schmaal2021). However, these studies did not use a network approach that adjusted for temporal and other pairwise effects in models. Among early adolescents, internalizing may be a more important mental health risk factor for STBs compared to other symptom dimensions. This finding warrants replication in other samples.

The direct association between substance use and STBs is also noteworthy. Both STBs and substance use can be harmful in this age range, and the co-occurrence of STBs and substance use during early adolescence may compound the risk for negative long-term outcomes (Effinger & Stewart, Reference Effinger and Stewart2012). STBs also had a direct effect with stressful life events in the between-subjects network, suggesting that, overall, youth with a lifetime history of stressful life events were more likely to report lifetime history of STBs. This is in line with previous research identifying adverse life events as correlates of STBs (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; King et al., Reference King, Schwab-Stone, Flisher, Greenwald, Kramer, Goodman, Lahey, Shaffer and Gould2001; Pan & Spittal, Reference Pan and Spittal2013). Lastly, the observed autoregressive effect for STBs in the temporal network affirms that individuals with a history of STBs have elevated risk for future STBs, and risk assessment over time should account for this (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Bailey, Witt, Stefanac, Milner, Currier, Pirkis, Condron and Hetrick2018).

Potential indirect pathways of risk and protection for STBs

Potential indirect pathways to STBs were also observed in the contemporaneous network structure. While internalizing was the only mental health node that exhibited a direct association with STBs, internalizing had high centrality and was associated with the other five mental health symptom dimensions, stressful life events, and material hardship. Thus, internalizing may potentially provide a pathway through which life stressors and other mental health symptoms contribute to risk of STBs. For example, externalizing problems could potentially lead to criticism from others, which could negatively impact mood and self-esteem, thereby contributing to thoughts of suicide. It might be the case that having externalizing symptoms only increases the propensity for experiencing STBs if individuals have co-occurring internalizing symptoms, thus explaining some of the inconsistencies in the literature examining links between externalizing symptoms and STBs (Piqueras et al., Reference Piqueras, Soto-Sanz, Rodríguez-Marín and García-Oliva2019; Witte et al., Reference Witte, Gauthier, Huang, Ribeiro and Franklin2018). Further, stressful life events may be more likely to impact STBs if youth experience resulting internalizing difficulties. However, because these effects were in the contemporaneous network, we cannot make assumptions about the temporal order of associations between internalizing, these other risk factors, and STBs.

Pairwise associations were also observed between substance use and all three of the socioenvironmental risk factors (family conflict, parental monitoring, and school protective factors). Because these factors are all related to STBs and to each other, results suggest the potential for a feedback system in which these factors could conjointly increase risk for STBs. For example, low parental monitoring can increase a youth’s risk for early initiation of substance use (Dever et al., Reference Dever, Schulenberg, Dworkin, O’Malley, Kloska and Bachman2012) and poorer school performance and engagement (Lowe & Dotterer, Reference Lowe and Dotterer2013). This could potentially contribute to higher family conflict, which could reinforce ongoing school and substance use concerns (Timmons & Margolin, Reference Timmons and Margolin2015). Thus, activation of one or more of these risk factors may increase the other risk factors, exacerbating and potentially perpetuating overall risk for STBs (Borsboom, Reference Borsboom2017; Dablander et al., Reference Dablander, Pichler, Cika and Bacilieri2020). However, because these variables were all related, intervening on one of these risk factors might have protective effects on the other nodes. Hence, in addition to direct effects, interrelations between risk and protective factors may impact the development and course of STBs. Many of these effects may be obscured in traditional models that do not consider pairwise relations between all variables. Thus, the present study supports the use of longitudinal network analyses to provide information about complex systems of risk and protection for STBs (de Beurs, Reference de Beurs2017). Nonetheless, our use of observational data precludes interpreting these potential indirect pathways as causal. We encourage future research to prospectively test these indirect effects in mediation models and to use shorter measurement timescales that are more likely to capture the causal timeframe of these effects (i.e., less than 12 months apart).

Clinical implications

To date, relatively few suicide-focused interventions have been specifically tailored to late childhood and early adolescence (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Bailey, Witt, Stefanac, Milner, Currier, Pirkis, Condron and Hetrick2018). Our results identified risk and protective factors that are directly related to STBs and could be amenable to prevention and early intervention initiatives (McGorry & Mei, Reference McGorry and Mei2018). Internalizing may be a more central risk factor for STBs than other mental health symptom dimensions in this age range, and intervention programs that provide age-appropriate treatment for internalizing symptoms could be valuable for reducing and preventing early onset STBs (Wasserman et al., Reference Wasserman, Carli, Iosue, Javed and Herrman2021). Given its high centrality in the network model, intervening on internalizing symptoms might also have subsequent protective effects on other risk factors for STBs. Furthermore, results suggest that psychosocial interventions designed for early adolescents may benefit from prioritizing increased school support and engagement, as well as improving family support, engagement, and communication. Interventions that prevent or delay the initiation of recreational substance use may also have protective effects against STBs (Stockings et al., 2016). Universal school-based interventions have shown promise for protecting against mental health concerns broadly (e.g., Catalano, Reference Catalano, Hawkins, Kosterman, Bailey, Oesterle, Cambron and Farrington2021), and early research suggests universally implemented programs can effectively integrate curricula focused on STBs specifically (Calear et al., Reference Calear, Christensen, Freeman, Fenton, Grant, van Spijker and Donker2016; Schilling et al., Reference Schilling, Aseltine and James2016; Wasserman et al., Reference Wasserman, Hoven, Wasserman, Wall, Eisenberg, Hadlaczky, Kelleher, Sarchiapone, Apter, Balazs, Bobes, Brunner, Corcoran, Cosman, Guillemin, Haring, Iosue, Kaess, Kahn and Carli2015). Continued development and evaluation of such school-based programs is recommended.

Limitations and future directions

Results from the present study should be considered in the context of some limitations. First, while the ABCD data has many strengths, measurement waves are spaced approximately 1 year apart. Because the causal timescales between STBs and risk and protective factors are likely shorter than 1 year, measuring these constructs at shorter timescales would better capture proximal associations between variables (Harmer et al., Reference Harmer, Lee, Duong and Saadabadi2021). Relatedly, although the ABCD study battery allowed for examination of a wide range of STB risk and protective factors, measures provided sparse detail about the specific timing, severity, and frequency of many constructs of interest.

Second, due to statistical assumptions of panel GVAR models, all study variables needed available data from three annual waves. Some relevant constructs were not included due to not having available data at all timepoints (e.g., impulsivity, peer experiences). Static risk factors that can influence long-term risk for suicide but were not repeatedly measured were also not considered (e.g., family history of suicide). In the case of our stressful life events variable, data for this construct was available from all three waves but the measures differed across timepoints (KSADS PTSD module at baseline and Year 2, LES at Years 1–2). Given the salience of stressful life events as a risk factor for youth STBs in extensive prior literature (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017), we elected to merge these measures to retain this construct in our models. Sensitivity analyses suggested this analytic decision did not introduce bias to the model. Nonetheless, results involving the stressful life events variable should be interpreted with this measurement limitation in mind, and replication of our results in other samples will increase confidence in how stressful life events relate to STBs and other risk and protective factors.

Third, analyses relied on either self- or parent-report data. We note that most of the direct associations observed for STBs (self-reported by youth) were with other variables youth self-reported on (family conflict, parental monitoring, school protective factors, and substance use). Similarly, many of the parent-report variables had strongest associations with other parent-report variables in the networks (mental health symptoms and life stressors). All the examined risk and protective factors have been robustly associated with youth STBs in previous research regardless of measure reporter (Carballo et al., Reference Carballo, Llorente, Kehrmann, Flamarique, Zuddas, Purper-Ouakil, Hoekstra, Coghill, Schulze, Dittmann, Buitelaar, Castro-Fornieles, Lievesley, Santosh and Arango2020; Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). Nonetheless, it is possible that bias for larger effects among variables from the same reporter could have influenced the observed network structures. Replicating this study using data from both parent and youth reports for each variable will increase confidence in results.

Fourth, despite the use of a school-based population-level sampling design to reduce selection bias, the ABCD sample includes overrepresentation of dominant social identities. Minoritized identity status and the intersectionality of multiple minoritized identities are associated with STBs (e.g., VanBronkhorst et al., Reference VanBronkhorst, Edwards, Roberts, Kist, Evans, Mohatt and Blankenship2021). Relations between STB risk and protective factors may also vary across social identity groups (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018; Klonsky et al., Reference Klonsky, Qiu and Saffer2017; Wiglesworth et al., Reference Wiglesworth, Clement, Wingate and Klimes-Dougan2022). Some groups of youth with elevated risk for STBs compared to the general population, such as youth involved in foster care, Child Protective Services, and the juvenile justice system (Teplin et al., Reference Teplin, Stokes, McCoy, Abram and Byck2015), are less likely to participate in population-based research studies (Feldstein Ewing et al., Reference Feldstein Ewing, Chang, Cottler, Tapert, Dowling and Brown2018; Sharma et al., Reference Sharma, McDonald, Bledsoe, Grad, Jenkins, Moran, O’Hara and Pester2021). Thus, replication of study results in more diverse and underserved populations of youth is warranted (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Franz, Guzmán, Glenn, C., Kleiman and Nock2018). Future work should also examine how minoritized identity status and experiences of minority stress and social safety influence the development and longitudinal course of STBs in this sample (e.g., Diamond & Alley, Reference Diamond and Alley2022).

Conclusions

Despite the aforementioned limitations, the present study provides valuable information about potential risk and protective pathways for STBs among early adolescents. There is a critical need for research focusing on STBs during this developmental stage, as rates of youth STBs are rising and early adolescents have received relatively little attention in STB literature compared to older age groups (Ayer et al., Reference Ayer, Colpe, Pearson, Rooney and Murphy2020; Nock et al., Reference Nock, Green, Hwang, McLaughlin, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Kessler2013; de Sousa et al., Reference de Sousa, Santos, Silva, Perrelli and Sougey2017). This study represents a novel extension of the network approach to psychopathology to increase understanding of early onset STBs. Results emphasize that family and school experiences are salient social risk factors for STBs in early adolescents. Additionally, internalizing problems appear to be a more important risk factor than other mental health symptoms in this age range, and internalizing could possibly be a pathway through which stressful life events and other mental health symptoms contribute to STBs. Substance use was also associated with elevated risk for STBs. Results also suggest the potential for feedback systems of risk, in which the activation of multiple risk factors might exacerbate risk for STBs. Age-specific early interventions for STBs may benefit from focusing on increased social support in family and school domains, identifying and intervening on internalizing symptoms, and preventing early onset substance use.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001597.

Data availability

The data the support the findings of this study are available from the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive.

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Drs. Kimberly Henry, Emily Merz, Nathaniel Riggs, and Bailey Fosdick for their helpful comments on this project while serving as committee members for G. Wallace’s doctoral dissertation. We are also grateful to the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study consortium, principal investigators, research staff members, and the NIMH Data Archive for providing access to the open-source data used in this study. This study also would not have been possible without the ABCD Study participants and families, who generously contributed their time and data to support scientific research.

Funding statement

The first author’s (G. Wallace) effort was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (T32MH126426; MPI: L. Weinstock, I. Miller). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health.

Competing interests

The authors have no known conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

Achenbach, T. M. (2009). The Achenbach System of Emprically Based Assessment (ASEBA): Development, findings, theory and applications. University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.Google Scholar
Allen, K., & Goldman-Mellor, S. (2018). Neighborhood characteristics and adolescent suicidal behavior: Evidence from a population-based study. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 48(6), 677689. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic & statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Arthur, M. W., Briney, J. S., Hawkins, J. D., Abbott, R. D., Brooke-Weiss, B. L., & Catalano, R. F. (2007). Measuring risk and protection in communities using the communities that care youth survey. Evaluation and Program Planning, 30(2), 197211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2007.01.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auchter, A. M., Hernandez Mejia, M., Heyser, C. J., Shilling, P. D., Jernigan, T. L., Brown, S. A., Tapert, S. F., & Dowling, G. J. (2018). A description of the ABCD organizational structure and communication framework. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayer, L., Colpe, L., Pearson, J., Rooney, M., & Murphy, E. (2020). Advancing research in child suicide: A call to action. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(9), 10281035. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2020.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Albaugh, M. D., Avenevoli, S., Chang, L., Clark, D. B., Glantz, M. D., Hudziak, J. J., Jernigan, T. L., Tapert, S. F., Yurgelun-Todd, D., Alia-Klein, N., Potter, A. S., Paulus, M. P., Prouty, D., Zucker, R. A., & Sher, K. J. (2017). Demographic, physical and mental health assessments in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale and description. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 5566. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.10.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsher, B. E., Smolenski, D. J., Pruitt, L. D., Bush, N. E., Beech, E. H., Workman, D. E., Morgan, R. L., Evatt, D. P., Tucker, J., & Skopp, N. A. (2019). Prediction models for suicide attempts and deaths: A systematic review and simulation. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 642651. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, A. L., & Silverman, M. M. (2014). Suicide risk assessment and risk formulation part II: Suicide risk formulation and the determination of levels of risk. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 44(4), 432443. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanken, T. F., Isvoranu, A.-M., & Epskamp, S. (2022). Estimating network structures using model selection. In Network psychometrics with R. Routledge.Google Scholar
Borgatti, S. P. (2005). Centrality and network flow. Social Networks, 27(1), 5571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2004.11.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D. (2017). A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry, 16(1), 513. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20375 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D., & Cramer, A. O. J. (2013). Network analysis: An integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 91121. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D., Deserno, M. K., Rhemtulla, M., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., McNally, R. J., Robinaugh, D. J., Perugini, M., Dalege, J., Costantini, G., Isvoranu, A.-M., Wysocki, A. C., van Borkulo, C. D., van Bork, R., & Waldorp, L. J. (2021). Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science. Nature Reviews Methods Primers, 1(1), 58. https://doi.org/10.1038/s43586-021-00055-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruni, O., Ottaviano, S., Guidetti, V., Romoli, M., Innocenzi, M., Cortesi, F., & Giannotti, F. (1996). The Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC). Construction and validation of an instrument to evaluate sleep disturbances in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Sleep Research, 5(4), 251261. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1996.00251.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burger, J., Hoekstra, R. H. A., Mansueto, A. C., & Epskamp, S. (2022). Network estimation from time series and panel data. In Network psychometrics with R. Routledge.Google Scholar
Calear, A. L., Christensen, H., Freeman, A., Fenton, K., Grant, J. B., van Spijker, B., & Donker, T. (2016). A systematic review of psychosocial suicide prevention interventions for youth. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 25, 467482. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-015-0783-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carballo, J. J., Llorente, C., Kehrmann, L., Flamarique, I., Zuddas, A., Purper-Ouakil, D., Hoekstra, P. J., Coghill, D., Schulze, U. M. E., Dittmann, R. W., Buitelaar, J. K., Castro-Fornieles, J., Lievesley, K., Santosh, P., Arango, C., & the STOP Consortium.(2020). Psychosocial risk factors for suicidality in children and adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(6), 759776. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-01270-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Ambler, A., Danese, A., Elliott, M. L., Hariri, A., Harrington, H., Hogan, S., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., Rasmussen, L. J. H., Reuben, A., Richmond-Rakerd, L., Sugden, K., Wertz, J., Williams, B. S., & Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Network Open, 3(4), e203221. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.3221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalano, RF, Hawkins, J. D., Kosterman, R., Bailey, J. A., Oesterle, S., Cambron, C., & Farrington, D. P. (2021). Applying the social development model in middle childhood to promote healthy development: Effects from primary school through the 30s and across generations. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology 7(1):6686. doi: 10.1007/s40865-020-00152-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cha, C. B., Franz, P. J., Guzmán, M., Glenn, E., C., R., Kleiman, E. M., & Nock, M. K. (2018). Annual research review: Suicide among youth – epidemiology, (potential) etiology, and treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(4), 460482. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copeland, W. E., Goldston, D. B., & Costello, E. J. (2017). Adult associations of childhood suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A prospective, longitudinal analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(11), 958965.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.08.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cramer, R., & Kapusta, N. (2017). A social-ecological framework of theory, assessment, and prevention of suicide. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1756. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dablander, F., Pichler, A., Cika, A., & Bacilieri, A. (2020). Anticipating critical transitions in psychological systems using early warning signals: Theoretical and practical considerations, 28(4), 765790. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/5wc28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Beurs, D. (2017). Network analysis: A novel approach to understand suicidal behaviour. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 14(3), 219. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph14030219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Sousa, G. S.de, Santos, M. S. P. D., Silva, A. T. P.da, Perrelli, J. G. A., & Sougey, E. B. (2017). Suicide in childhood: A literatura review. Ciencia & Saude Coletiva, 22(9), 30993110. https://doi.org/10.1590/1413-81232017229.14582017.Google Scholar
Deserno, M. K., Isvoranu, A.-M., Epskamp, S., & Blanken, T. F. (2022). Descriptive analysis of network structures. In Network psychometrics with R. Routledge.Google Scholar
Dever, B. V., Schulenberg, J. E., Dworkin, J. B., O’Malley, P. M., Kloska, D. D., & Bachman, J. G. (2012). Predicting risk-taking with and without substance use: The effects of parental monitoring, school bonding, and sports participation. Prevention Science, 13(6), 605615. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-012-0288-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeVille, D. C., Whalen, D., Breslin, F. J., Morris, A. S., Khalsa, S. S., Paulus, M. P., & Barch, D. M. (2020). Prevalence and family-related factors associated with suicidal ideation, suicide attempts, and self-injury in children aged 9 to 10 years. JAMA Network Open, 3(2), e1920956. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.20956,CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, L. M., & Alley, J. (2022). Rethinking minority stress: A social safety perspective on the health effects of stigma in sexually-diverse and gender-diverse populations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 138, 104720. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Echeverria, S. E., Diez-Roux, A. V., & Link, B. G. (2004). Reliability of self-reported neighborhood characteristics. Journal of Urban Health: Bulletin of the New York Academy of Medicine, 81(4), 682701. https://doi.org/10.1093/jurban/jth151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Effinger, J. M., & Stewart, D. G. (2012). Classification of co-occurring depression and substance abuse symptoms predicts suicide attempts in adolescents. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 42(4), 353358. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1943-278X.2012.00092.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S. (2020). Psychometric network models from time-series and panel data. Psychometrika, 85(1), 206231. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-020-09697-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S. (2022). Psychonetrics: Structural Equation Modeling and Confirmatory Network Analysis (0.10) [R].Google Scholar
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 195212. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S., Costantini, G., Halsbeck, J., Isvoranu, A., Cramer, A., Waldorp, L., Schmittmann, V., & Boorsboom, D. (2023), qgraph: Graph plotting methods, psychometric data visualization and graphical model estimation (1.9.4) [R] Google Scholar
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O. J., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. (2012). Qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. Journal of Statistical Software, 48, 118. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i04.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S., & Fried, E. I. (2018). A tutorial on regularized partial correlation networks. Psychological Methods, 23(4), 617634. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazel, S., & Runeson, B. (2020). Suicide. New England Journal of Medicine, 382(3), 266274. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1902944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Chang, L., Cottler, L. B., Tapert, S. F., Dowling, G. J., & Brown, S. A. (2018). Approaching retention within the ABCD study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 130137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Al-Halabí, S., Pérez-Albéniz, A., & Debbané, M. (2022). Risk and protective factors in adolescent suicidal behaviour: A network analysis. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(3), 1784.https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19031784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Díez-Gómez, A., de la Barrera, U., Sebastian-Enesco, C., Ortuño-Sierra, J., Montoya-Castilla, I., Lucas-Molina, B., Inchausti, F., & Pérez-Albéniz, A. (2020). Suicidal behaviour in adolescents: A network analysis. Revista De Psiquiatria y Salud Mental, 17(1), S. 1888–9891(20)30032-X. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsm.2020.04.007.Google Scholar
Fotti, S. A., Katz, L. Y., Afifi, T. O., & Cox, B. J. (2006). The associations between peer and parental relationships and suicidal behaviours in early adolescents. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne De Psychiatrie, 51(11), 698703. https://doi.org/10.1177/070674370605101106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franklin, J. C., Ribeiro, J. D., Fox, K. R., Bentley, K. H., Kleiman, E. M., Huang, X., Musacchio, K. M., Jaroszewski, A. C., Chang, B. P., & Nock, M. K. (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 187232. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, E. I., Papanikolaou, F., & Epskamp, S. (2022). Mental health and social contact during the COVID-19 pandemic: An ecological momentary assessment study. Clinical Psychological Science, 10(2), 340354. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026211017839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fried, E. I., van Borkulo, C. D., Cramer, A. O. J., Boschloo, L., Schoevers, R. A., & Borsboom, D. (2017). Mental disorders as networks of problems: A review of recent insights. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 52(1), 110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-016-1319-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garavan, H., Bartsch, H., Conway, K., Decastro, A., Goldstein, R. Z., Heeringa, S., Jernigan, T., Potter, A., Thompson, W., & Zahs, D. (2018). Recruiting the ABCD sample: Design considerations and procedures. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 1622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geldhof, G. J., Preacher, K. J., & Zyphur, M. J. (2014). Reliability estimation in a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis framework. Psychological Methods, 19(1), 7291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gijzen, M. W. M., Rasing, S. P. A., Creemers, D. H. M., Smit, F., Engels, R. C. M. E., & De Beurs, D. (2021). Suicide ideation as a symptom of adolescent depression. A network analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 278, 6877. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldston, D. B., Daniel, S. S., Erkanli, A., Reboussin, B. A., Mayfield, A., Frazier, P. H., & Treadway, S. L. (2009). Psychiatric diagnoses as contemporaneous risk factors for suicide attempts among adolescents and young adults: Developmental changes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(2), 281290. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez, R., Thompson, E. L., Sanchez, M., Morris, A., Gonzalez, M. R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Mason, M. J., Arroyo, J., Howlett, K., Tapert, S. F., & Zucker, R. A. (2021). An update on the assessment of culture and environment in the ABCD Study®: Emerging literature and protocol updates over three measurement waves. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 52, 101021. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., & Gipson, P. Y. (2004). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective effects. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 33(2), 412425. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzmán, E. M., Cha, C. B., Ribeiro, J. D., & Franklin, J. C. (2019). Suicide risk around the world: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 54(12), 14591470. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-019-01759-x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallquist, M. N., Wright, A. G. C., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2021). Problems with centrality measures in psychopathology symptom networks: Why network psychometrics cannot escape psychometric theory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 56(2), 199223. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2019.1640103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harman, G., Kliamovich, D., Morales, A. M., Gilbert, S., Barch, D. M., Mooney, M. A., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Fair, D. A., & Nagel, B. J. (2021). Prediction of suicidal ideation and attempt in 9 and 10 year-old children using transdiagnostic risk features. PLOS ONE, 16(5), e0252114. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harmer, B., Lee, S., Duong, T.vi H., & Saadabadi, A. (2021). Suicidal ideation. In StatPearls. StatPearls publishing. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK565877/ Google Scholar
Hevey, D. (2018). Network analysis: A brief overview and tutorial. Health Psychology and Behavioral Medicine, 6(1), 301328. https://doi.org/10.1080/21642850.2018.1521283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, E. A., Clark, D. B., Orendain, N., Hudziak, J., Squeglia, L. M., & Dowling, G. J. (2019). Stress exposures, neurodevelopment and health measures in the ABCD study. Neurobiology of Stress, 10, 100157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Janiri, D., Doucet, G. E., Pompili, M., Sani, G., Luna, B., Brent, D. A., & Frangou, S. (2020). Risk and protective factors for childhood suicidality: A US population-based study. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(4), 317326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30049-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jobst, L. J., Heine, C., Auerswald, M., & Moshagen, M. (2021). Effects of multivariate non-normality and missing data on the root mean square error of approximation. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 28(6), 851858. https://doi-org.revproxy.brown.edu/10.1080/10705511.2021.1933987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, D. G., Winer, E. S., & Salem, T. (2020). The current status of temporal network analysis for clinical science: Considerations as the paradigm shifts? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 76(9), 15911612. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karcher, N. R., & Barch, D. M. (2021). The ABCD study: Understanding the development of risk for mental and physical health outcomes. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 131142. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0736-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karoly, H. C., Callahan, T., Schmiege, S. J., & Feldstein Ewing, S. W. (2016). Evaluating the hispanic paradox in the context of adolescent risky sexual behavior: The role of parent monitoring. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 41(4), 429440. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsv039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, R. A., Schwab-Stone, M., Flisher, A. J., Greenwald, S., Kramer, R. A., Goodman, S. H., Lahey, B. B., Shaffer, D., & Gould, M. S. (2001). Psychosocial and risk behavior correlates of youth suicide attempts and suicidal ideation. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(7), 837846. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200107000-00019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klonsky, E. D., Qiu, T., & Saffer, B. Y. (2017). Recent advances in differentiating suicide attempters from suicide ideators. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 30(1), 1520. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
KSAD-COMP LLC (2024). KSADS-COMP: The gold standard in child and adolescent psychiatric diagnoses.www.ksads-comp.com. Accessed July 1, 2024.Google Scholar
Li, Y., & Kwok, S. Y. C. L. (2023). A longitudinal network analysis of the interactions of risk and protective factors for suicidal potential in early adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 52(2), 306318. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-022-01698-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisdahl, K. M., Sher, K. J., Conway, K. P., Gonzalez, R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Nixon, S. J., Tapert, S., Bartsch, H., Goldstein, R. Z., & Heitzeg, M. (2018). Adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Overview of substance use assessment methods. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 8096. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisdahl, K. M., Tapert, S., Sher, K. J., Gonzalez, R., Nixon, S. J., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Conway, K. P., Wallace, A., Sullivan, R., Hatcher, K., Kaiver, C., Thompson, W., Reuter, C., Bartsch, H., Wade, N. E., Jacobus, J., Albaugh, M. D., Allgaier, N., Anokhin, A. P., … ABCD Consortium (2021). Substance use patterns in 9-10 year olds: Baseline findings from the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 227, 108946. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2021.108946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowe, K., & Dotterer, A. M. (2013). Parental monitoring, parental warmth, and minority youths’ academic outcomes: Exploring the integrative model of parenting. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 42(9), 14131425. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-013-9934-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martz, M. E., Heitzeg, M. M., Lisdahl, K. M., Cloak, C. C., Ewing, S. W. F., Gonzalez, R., Haist, F., LeBlanc, K. H., Madden, P. A., Ross, J. M., Sher, K. J., Tapert, S. F., Thompson, W. K., & Wade, N. E. (2022). Individual-, peer-, and parent-level substance use-related factors among 9- and 10-year-olds from the ABCD study: Prevalence rates and sociodemographic differences. Drug and Alcohol Dependence Reports, 3, 100037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dadr.2022.100037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGorry, P. D., & Mei, C. (2018). Early intervention in youth mental health: Progress and future directions. BMJ Ment Health, 21(4), 182184. https://doi.org/10.1136/ebmental-2018-300060.Google ScholarPubMed
Menczer, F., Fortunato, S., & Davis, C. A. (2020). A first course in network science. Higher Education from Cambridge University Press. Available at https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108653947 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. B., Esposito-Smythers, C., & Leichtweis, R. N. (2015). Role of social support in adolescent suicidal ideation and suicide attempts. The Journal of Adolescent Health: Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 56(3), 286292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.10.265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millner, A. J., Robinaugh, D. J., & Nock, M. K. (2020). Advancing the understanding of suicide: The need for formal theory and rigorous descriptive research. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 704716. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.06.007.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1994). Family environment scale manual (3rd edn). Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Mujahid, M. S., Diez Roux, A. V., Morenoff, J. D., & Raghunathan, T. (2007). Assessing the measurement properties of neighborhood scales: From psychometrics to ecometrics. American Journal of Epidemiology, 165(8), 858867. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwm040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nock, M. K., Green, J. G., Hwang, I., McLaughlin, K. A., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2013). Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: Results from the national comorbidity survey replication adolescent supplement. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(3), 300310. https://doi.org/10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Driscoll, C., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., Saunders, R., Cardoso, A., Stott, J., Wheatley, J., Cirkovic, M., Naqvi, S. A., Buckman, J. E. J., & Pilling, S. (2022). Transdiagnostic symptom dynamics during psychotherapy. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 10881. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-14901-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opsahl, T., Agneessens, F., & Skvoretz, J. (2010). Node centrality in weighted networks: Generalizing degree and shortest paths. Social Networks, 32(3), 245251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socnet.2010.03.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ou, W., Yang, Y., Chen, Y., Li, Y., Yang, S., Lu, Y., Li, L., Huang, M., Ma, M., Lv, G., Zhao, X., Qing, Y., Ju, Y., & Zhang, Y. (2023). Bridge symptoms between parenting styles and proximal psychological risk factors associated with adolescent suicidal thoughts: A network analysis. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 17(1), 129. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-023-00674-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M. M., Potter, A., Hyatt, C. S., Albaugh, M., Thompson, W. K., Jernigan, T., Yuan, D., Hahn, S., Allgaier, N., & Garavan, H. (2021). Recalibrating expectations about effect size: A multi-method survey of effect sizes in the ABCD study. PloS One, 16(9), e0257535. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0257535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, C. E., Sheth, C., Marshall, A. T., Adise, S., Baker, F. C., Chang, L., Clark, D. B., Coronado, C., Dagher, R. K., Diaz, V., Dowling, G. J., Gonzalez, M. R., Haist, F., Herting, M. M., Huber, R. S., Jernigan, T. L., LeBlanc, K., Lee, K., Lisdahl, K. M., Neigh, G., Patterson, M. W., Renshaw, P., Rhee, K. E., Tapert, S., Thompson, W. K., Uban, K., Sowell, E. R., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. (2021). A comprehensive overview of the physical health of the adolescent brain cognitive development study cohort at baseline. Frontiers in Pediatrics, 9, 734184. https://doi.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2021.734184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, S. W., & Spittal, P. M. (2013). Health effects of perceived racial and religious bullying among urban adolescents in China: A cross-sectional national study. Global Public Health, 8(6), 685697. https://doi.org/10.1080/17441692.2013.799218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piqueras, J. A., Soto-Sanz, V., Rodríguez-Marín, J., & García-Oliva, C. (2019). What is the role of internalizing and externalizing symptoms in adolescent suicide behaviors? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 16(14), 2511. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16142511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, K., Brown, G. K., Stanley, B., Brent, D. A., Yershova, K. V., Oquendo, M. A., Currier, G. W., Melvin, G. A., Greenhill, L., Shen, S., & Mann, J. J. (2011). The columbia-suicide severity rating scale: Initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(12), 12661277. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Raffagnato, A., Iannattone, S., Fasolato, R., Parolin, E., Ravaglia, B., Biscalchin, G., Traverso, A., Zanato, S., Miscioscia, M., & Gatta, M. (2022). A pre-adolescent and adolescent clinical sample study about suicidal ideation, suicide attempt, and self-harming. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education, 12(10), 14411462. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12100100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ram, N., Brose, A., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2013). Dynamic factor analysis: Modeling person-specific process. In The Oxford handbook of quantitative methods: Statistical analysis (vol. 2, p. 441457). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reinherz, H. Z., Tanner, J. L., Berger, S. R., Beardslee, W. R., & Fitzmaurice, G. M. (2006). Adolescent suicidal ideation as predictive of psychopathology, suicidal behavior, and compromised functioning at age 30. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(7), 12261232. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.2006.163.7.1226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, William (2024). psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research. Northwestern University. R package version 2.4.6.Google Scholar
Robinson, J., Bailey, E., Witt, K., Stefanac, N., Milner, A., Currier, D., Pirkis, J., Condron, P., & Hetrick, S. (2018). What works in youth suicide prevention? A systematic review and meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine, 4-5, 5291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2018.10.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rush, J., & Hofer, S. M. (2017). Design-based approaches for improving measurement in developmental science. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 82(2), 6783. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saragosa-Harris, N. M., Chaku, N., MacSweeney, N., Guazzelli Williamson, V., Scheuplein, M., Feola, B., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Demir-Lira, E., McNeilly, E. A., Huffman, L. G., Whitmore, L., Michalska, K. J., Damme, K. S., Rakesh, D., & Mills, K. L. (2022). A practical guide for researchers and reviewers using the ABCD study and other large longitudinal datasets. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 55, 101115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2022.101115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schilling, EA, Aseltine, RH Jr, James, A. (2016). The SOS suicide prevention program: Further evidence of efficacy and effectiveness. Prevention Science 17(2), 157166. doi: 10.1007/s11121-015-0594-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sedgwick, R., Epstein, S., Dutta, R., & Ougrin, D. (2019). Social media, internet use and suicide attempts in adolescents. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 32(6), 534541. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharma, J., McDonald, C. P., Bledsoe, K. G., Grad, R. I., Jenkins, K. D., Moran, D., O’Hara, C., & Pester, D. (2021). Intersectionality in research: Call for inclusive, decolonized, and culturally sensitive research designs in counselor education. Counseling Outcome Research and Evaluation, 0(0), 110. https://doi.org/10.1080/21501378.2021.1922075.Google Scholar
Shiratori, Y., Tachikawa, H., Nemoto, K., Endo, G., Aiba, M., Matsui, Y., & Asada, T. (2014). Network analysis for motives in suicide cases: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 68(4), 299307. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., & Rote, W. M. (2019). Adolescent-parent relationships: Progress, processes, and prospects. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 4168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2003). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71(4), 10721085. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Mental Health. (2023). Suicide. Mental Health Information. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/suicide Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2013). Using multivariate statistics (6th edn). Pearson Education, Inc.Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., van Hoorn, J., Rogers, C. R., & Do, K. T. (2018). Social influence on positive youth development: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 54, 215258. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2017.10.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teplin, L. A., Stokes, M. L., McCoy, K. P., Abram, K. M., & Byck, G. R. (2015). Suicidal ideation and behavior in youth in the juvenile justice system: A review of the literature. Journal of Correctional Health Care : The Official Journal of the National Commission On Correctional Health Care, 21(3), 222242. https://doi.org/10.1177/1078345815587001.Google Scholar
Thompson, A. H., Dewa, C. S., & Phare, S. (2012). The suicidal process: Age of onset and severity of suicidal behaviour. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47(8), 12631269. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-011-0434-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmons, A. C., & Margolin, G. (2015). Family conflict, mood, and adolescents’ daily school problems: Moderating roles of internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Child Development, 86(1), 241258. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Townsend, L., Kobak, K., Kearney, C., Milham, M., Andreotti, C., Escalera, J., Alexander, L., Gill, M. K., Birmaher, B., Sylvester, R., Rice, D., Deep, A., & Kaufman, J. (2020). Development of three web-based computerized versions of the kiddie schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia child psychiatric diagnostic interview: Preliminary validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(2), 309325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.05.009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
United Health Foundation (2023). 2022 Health Of Women And Children Report. America’s Health Rankings. https://www.americashealthrankings.org/learn/reports/2022-health-of-women-and-children-report Google Scholar
Usami, S., Murayama, K., & Hamaker, E. L. (2019). A unified framework of longitudinal models to examine reciprocal relations. Psychological Methods, 24(5), 637657. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Velzen, L. S., Toenders, Y. J., Avila-Parcet, A., Dinga, R., Rabinowitz, J. A., Campos, A. I., Jahanshad, N., Rentería, M. E., & Schmaal, L. Predictors of suicidal thoughts and behavior in children: Results from penalized logistic regression analyses in the ABCD study . (2021).https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.15.2125.1736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanBronkhorst, S. B., Edwards, E. M., Roberts, D. E., Kist, K., Evans, D. L., Mohatt, J., & Blankenship, K. (2021). Suicidality among psychiatrically hospitalized lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer, and/or questioning youth: Risk and protective factors. LGBT Health, 8(6), 395403. https://doi.org/10.1089/lgbt.2020.0278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wasserman, D., Carli, V., Iosue, M., Javed, A., & Herrman, H. (2021). Suicide prevention in childhood and adolescence: A narrative review of current knowledge on risk and protective factors and effectiveness of interventions. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry, 13(3), e12452. https://doi.org/10.1111/appy.12452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wasserman, D, Hoven, CW, Wasserman, C, Wall, M, Eisenberg, R, Hadlaczky, G, Kelleher, I, Sarchiapone, M, Apter, A, Balazs, J, Bobes, J, Brunner, R, Corcoran, P, Cosman, D, Guillemin, F, Haring, C, Iosue, M, Kaess, M, Kahn, JP, ... Carli, V. (2015). School-based suicide prevention programmes: the SEYLE cluster-randomised, controlled trial. Lancet 385(9977), 15361544. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61213-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiglesworth, A., Clement, D. N., Wingate, L. R., & Klimes-Dougan, B. (2022). Understanding suicide risk for youth who are both Black and Native American: The role of intersectionality and multiple marginalization. Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 52(4), 668682. https://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12851.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiley, J. (2020). multilevelTools: Multilevel and Mixed Effects Model Diagnostics and Effect Sizes (0.1.1) [R].Google Scholar
Witte, T. K., Gauthier, J. M., Huang, X., Ribeiro, J. D., & Franklin, J. C. (2018). Is externalizing psychopathology a robust risk factor for suicidal thoughts and behaviors? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(9), 16071625. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, S., Cheng, D., Su, J., Xu, J., Zhang, J., Huang, R., Sun, M., Wang, J., Gong, Y., & Zhou, L. (2023). A network analysis of depressive symptoms, psychosocial factors, and suicidal ideation in 8686 adolescents aged 12-20 years. Psychiatry Research, 329, 115517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zucker, R. A., Gonzalez, R., Feldstein Ewing, S. W., Paulus, M. P., Arroyo, J., Fuligni, A., Morris, A. S., Sanchez, M., & Wills, T. (2018). Assessment of culture and environment in the adolescent brain and cognitive development study: Rationale, description of measures, and early data. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 32, 107120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample demographic characteristics and STB endorsement across study timepoints

Figure 1

Table 2. ABCD measures, reporter, and data availability of variables used in the current study

Figure 2

Table 3. Unstandardized descriptive statistics and missingness for variables across study timepoints (N = 9,854)

Figure 3

Table 4. Multilevel correlations and intraclass correlations for study variables in the full sample (N = 9,854)

Figure 4

Figure 1. Pruned network structures for the panel GVAR model (N = 9,854). Edge color represents effect direction (blue = positive, red = negative), while edge thickness represents effect strength (darker, thicker edges denote larger effects). Edges not shown were pruned during model selection. (a) Arrows represent lagged directed partial correlations and autocorrelations in the temporal network. (b) Lines represent undirected partial correlations in the contemporaneous network. (c) Lines represent undirected partial correlations in the between-subjects network. Corresponding numeric results are presented in Tables 5–6.

Figure 5

Figure 2. Node centrality metrics for the panel GVAR model in the full sample (N = 9,854). Centrality metrics are shown in the metric of z-scores. (a) In the temporal network, In-Strength centrality represents the sum of all incoming absolute edge weights to a node, while Out-Strength centrality represents the sum of outgoing absolute edge weights from a node. (b–c) In the contemporaneous and between-subjects networks, Strength centrality represents the sum of all absolute edge weights connected to a node. Closeness represents the average shortest path between a specific node and all other nodes. Betweenness represents the number of times a node is on the shortest path between other nodes (Hevey, 2018).

Figure 6

Table 5. Estimated directed partial correlations for the temporal network (N = 9,854)

Figure 7

Table 6. Estimated undirected partial correlations for the contemporaneous (lower triangle) and between-subjects (upper triangle) networks (N = 9,854)

Supplementary material: File

Wallace and Conner supplementary material

Wallace and Conner supplementary material
Download Wallace and Conner supplementary material(File)
File 133.6 KB