Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T04:03:55.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

From infancy to eight: How early maternal mental health, emotion reminiscing, and language shape children’s mental health

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 May 2024

Sophie Russell*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, Australia
Amy L. Bird
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand Te Ngira Institute for Population Research, University of Waikato, Hamilton, New Zealand
Karen Waldie
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Elizabeth Peterson
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Susan M.B. Morton
Affiliation:
Research Institute for Innovative Solutions for Wellbeing and Health, Faculty of Health, University of Technology Sydney, Sydney, Australia
Polly E. Atatoa Carr
Affiliation:
Te Ngira Institute for Population Research, University of Waikato, Hamilton, New Zealand
Karen Salmon
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
Elaine Reese
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand
*
Corresponding author: Sophie Russell; Emails: [email protected]; [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To test the transmission of mental health difficulties from mother to child, we examined mediation through emotion reminiscing conversations and child language. Maternal depression symptoms were measured at 9 months post-partum, and child mental health outcomes were measured at age 8 years. Emotion reminiscing conversations between 1,234 mother-child pairs (624 boys, 610 girls) were recorded as part of a large, diverse, longitudinal cohort Growing Up in New Zealand. The 1,234 reminiscing conversations were transcribed and coded for maternal elaboration and emotion resolution quality (mother and child). The coded reminiscing variables did not mediate the pathway from maternal depression to child mental health outcomes; however, each maternal reminiscing variable together with child language skill serially mediated the relationship from maternal depression symptoms to child-reported anxiety and depression symptoms, and parent-reported child externalizing symptoms. Language as a skill and it’s use as a tool for making shared meaning from past events are highlighted as possible mechanisms for the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties. These findings point to potential opportunities for early interventions, including prevention of and support for postnatal depression, family intervention in reminiscing training, and supporting child language development.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Children learn through the world around them, with caregivers’ behavior, language, and affect influential from infancy across development. Parent-child reminiscing conversations about past events that the child has personally experienced may be an especially important conversational context for children’s development (Salmon & Reese, Reference Salmon and Reese2016). Decades of reminiscing research has focused on implications for children’s autobiographical memory and socio-cognitive development (Fivush, Reference Fivush1991; Laible, Reference Laible2011; Nelson & Fivush, Reference Nelson and Fivush2004). Through story telling about past events, parents and children establish a reference point and create a shared history, strengthening their emotional bonds and family identity (Fivush et al., Reference Fivush, Haden and Reese2006). In the emotion socialization context, discussing emotional past events, known as emotion reminiscing, may be one especially salient mechanism where parents scaffold children’s understanding of the event and their emotions (Fivush, Reference Fivush2007). Because reminiscing takes place outside of the immediate emotional context, there is a greater potential for nuanced emotion learning, where the parent and child may both be more regulated and better able to engage with the emotion content (Siegel, Reference Siegel2020). Parents provide support and serve as role models for handling difficult emotions, and these skills may extend to managing challenging emotions in other contexts as the child grows (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Cumberland and Spinrad1998; Eisenberg, Reference Eisenberg2020). It is important to note that the way mothers reminisce is influenced by cultural values, beliefs, and gender roles, resulting in variations in the focus and content of these conversations (Wang, Reference Wang, Pratt and Fiese2004). Maternal mental health difficulties have been consistently linked with children’s own mental health difficulties (S. H. Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011) and high-quality emotion reminiscing has been suggested as one potential mechanism to prevent transmission of mental health difficulties within families and to promote wellbeing (Valentino, Reference Valentino2011; Wareham & Salmon, Reference Wareham and Salmon2006). Only one study to date has empirically tested this intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties through reminiscing as an indirect pathway (Swetlitz et al., Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021). The current study seeks to extend these findings within a large, diverse, longitudinal sample. In addition, because children’s language skills are related to both mental health (Salmon et al., Reference Salmon, O’Kearney, Reese and Fortune2016) and parental reminiscing (Waters et al., Reference Waters, Camia, Facompré and Fivush2019), this study explored the role of children’s language skills in this potential intergenerational pathway. The current study explores maternal mental health in infancy and later child mental health at age eight, as mediated by emotion reminiscing and child language skills.

Maternal depression and child development

Maternal postnatal depression has significant and long-lasting effects on children’s cognitive and emotional development (Sohr-Preston & Scaramella, Reference Sohr-Preston and Scaramella2006; Waxler et al., Reference Waxler, Thelen and Muzik2011). In line with social learning theory, maternal depression may model maladaptive ways of coping and expressing negative emotion to children (Woodrow, Reference Woodrow2008). Parenting behaviors can be impacted by maternal depression, for example mothers with depression appear to be less consistent and responsive (Lovejoy et al., Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare and Neuman2000). These behaviors may affect the attachment bond and have ongoing consequences. Indeed, maternal depression has been associated with increased child mental health difficulties (S. H. Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Rouse, Connell, Broth, Hall and Heyward2011). Maternal depression is most strongly related to increased negative parenting behaviors, including irritability and hostility towards the child, although it is also associated with disengagement and reduced positive parenting (Lovejoy et al., Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare and Neuman2000). In addition, the timing of maternal depression is associated with child outcomes. Infancy is a critical period of development, with the strongest concurrent associations for maternal depression and negative parenting behaviors (Lovejoy et al., Reference Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare and Neuman2000). It may be that earlier exposure to maternal depression has increased impact due to the formation of the attachment relationship and disruption of the infant’s maturational tasks (S. H. Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999); with older children having more developed coping and language skills to communicate their needs more clearly, and thus have them met with greater ease (Salmon et al., Reference Salmon, O’Kearney, Reese and Fortune2016).

Transmission pathways

S. H. Goodman and Gotlib (Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999) provide a seminal integrative model to explain the relationship between maternal depression and children’s own mental health difficulties. The posited mechanisms for transmission include heritability, dysfunction in neurology, mothers’ cognitive style, emotion and behaviors, and the home environment. The latter two mechanisms present opportunity for intervention through modifying the impact of exposure to maternal maladaptive interactions and reducing a child’s stressful environment. These mechanisms are thought to relate to child mental health difficulties through disruptions in children’s social, emotional, and cognitive capacities. It is important to note that targeting treatment towards parent depression without specifically addressing parenting and parent-child interactions are not sufficient to improve child outcomes (Galbally & Lewis, Reference Galbally and Lewis2017).

Viewed within the integrative model from Goodman and Gotlib (Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999), mother-child reminiscing may be one mechanism that exposes children to the maladaptive cognitions, affect, and behavior of parents with mental health concerns. Reminiscing occurs outside of the “heat of the moment,” where children and parents can hold meaningful discussion about difficult or stressful events (Fivush, Reference Fivush, Moore, Lemmon and Skene2001; Salmon & Reese, Reference Salmon and Reese2016). These conversations can provide an opportunity for children to safely explore the linguistic and emotional world to make sense of their experiences. High-quality reminiscing has been associated with children’s autobiographical memory, language skills, theory of mind, self-concept coherence, self-esteem, and wellbeing (Bird & Reese, Reference Bird and Reese2006; Fivush & Nelson, Reference Fivush and Nelson2006; Fivush & Vasudeva, Reference Fivush and Vasudeva2002; Laible, Reference Laible2011; Reese et al., Reference Reese, Bird and Tripp2007; Wang, Reference Wang2006; Wareham & Salmon, Reference Wareham and Salmon2006).

Maternal mental health and reminiscing

Maternal depression is likely to affect how the mother interacts with the child, particularly regarding her child’s emotions. Parental depression may make it more difficult for a parent to self-regulate, to understand and explain their child’s own emotions (Cimino et al., Reference Cimino, Cerniglia, Tambelli, Ballarotto, Erriu, Paciello, Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2019; Koren-Karie et al., Reference Koren-Karie, Oppenheim and Getzler-Yosef2008) and to attune appropriately to their child’s internal experiences (Franz & McKinney, Reference Franz and McKinney2018). The context of reminiscing, outside the emotional ‘heat of the moment’, provides a space for both parent and child to navigate complex emotional events and feelings (Salmon & Reese, Reference Salmon and Reese2016). Reminiscing has been researched through recording, transcribing, and coding the style and content of parent-child conversations. Conversations are typically coded on either: a frequency-based level, looking at the conversation at the sentence level; or scale-based, considering the conversation as a whole (Grysman & Mansfield, Reference Grysman, Mansfield, Gülgöz and Sahin-Acar2020). Scale-based coding may be more appropriate for emotion reminiscing research, which has been established as uniquely related to child socio-emotional functioning, whereas frequency-based codes were significantly related to child memory accuracy (Leyva et al., Reference Leyva, Reese, Laible, Schaughency, Das and Clifford2020). Utilizing a scale-based approach for mother-child emotion reminiscing may better reflect meaning making across the conversations as a whole, rather than detailed accuracy on a sentence level (Grysman & Mansfield, Reference Grysman, Mansfield, Gülgöz and Sahin-Acar2020).

Why would mothers with depressive symptoms be less likely to elaborate or resolve emotion in later emotion reminiscing conversations? Depressive symptoms can have lasting cognitive effects on selective attention or memory even once the symptoms resolve (Semkovska et al., Reference Semkovska, Quinlivan, O’Grady, Johnson, Collins, O’Connor, Knittle, Ahern and Gload2019). Elaborative reminiscing requires curiosity and exploration, which may be difficult for individuals if they avoid difficult emotions (Tiwari et al., Reference Tiwari, Podell, Martin, Mychailyszyn, Furr and Kendall2008) or do not have the coping skills to regulate themselves or their child (Bridgett et al., Reference Bridgett, Burt, Edwards and Deater-Deckard2015). The empirical research investigating the associations between parent mental health and reminiscing has shown mixed results. In a small study of 42 mother-infant dyads, frequency of emotion references in reminiscing was not associated with maternal depression symptoms (Raikes & Thompson, Reference Raikes and Thompson2006). In a more recent study, mothers with diagnoses of anxiety, depression, anorexia nervosa, or no diagnosis, were compared on the quality of reminiscing with their 8-year-old children (Cimino et al., Reference Cimino, Cerniglia, Tambelli, Ballarotto, Erriu, Paciello, Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2019). Compared to mothers without a diagnosis, mothers with any diagnosis had lower rated maternal sensitive guidance (including closure of emotion), child cooperation and exploration, and emotional coherence. Specifically, mothers with depression and anorexia nervosa diagnoses were also less emotionally matched with their 8-year-old child during reminiscing (Cimino et al., Reference Cimino, Cerniglia, Tambelli, Ballarotto, Erriu, Paciello, Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2019). Reese et al. (Reference Reese, Meins, Fernyhough and Centifanti2019) assessed elaborative reminiscing with mothers and their 3.5-year-old children. They found that earlier maternal depression symptoms negatively predicted elaboration, but this was no longer significant once maternal sensitivity was included in the model. Finally, Russell et al. (Reference Russell, Bird, McNamara and Herbert2023) also found a negative association between parent depression symptoms and parent elaboration within a small cohort of children; however, this association was only observed for community recruited children and not for children recruited from a mental health clinic. The variety of coding used across the existing studies makes for difficulty establishing a clear pattern and calls for the coding of both emotional and elaborative aspects of parent-child conversations. When taken together these findings provide equivocal support for an association between parental mental health and the emotional and elaborative quality of parent-child reminiscing conversations.

Child mental health and reminiscing

There is also evidence of an association between parent-child reminiscing quality and child mental health. Suveg et al., (Reference Suveg, Zeman, Flannery-Schroeder and Cassano2005, 2008) found that parents of children with anxiety disorders showed lower emotional expression and explanation and were less likely to talk about positive emotions when reminiscing compared to dyads without a diagnosis. Moreover, there is evidence of differences in reminiscing based on the externalizing clinical status of the child. Van Bergen et al. (Reference Van Bergen, Salmon and Dadds2018) found that mothers of children with conduct problems were less elaborative and used more negative emotion terms than mothers of typically developing children, whereas Pate et al. (Reference Pate, Salmon, Fortune and O’Kearney2020) found no differences in emotion descriptions or elaboration for mothers of children with oppositional defiant disorder versus those without. In other emotion conversations, similar patterns have been observed: Sales and Fivush (Reference Sales and Fivush2005) found a negative relationship between mothers’ emotion expression and explanations when discussing a chronic stressor, and children’s internalizing and externalizing difficulties. Brumariu and Kerns (Reference Brumariu and Kerns2015) observed lower parent elaboration and higher psychological control in conflict discussions with children with higher anxiety symptoms. Using a similar cohort as the present research from a large longitudinal study in New Zealand: Growing Up in New Zealand, Garnett et al. (Reference Garnett, Reese, Swearingen, Peterson, Salmon, Waldie, D’souza, Atatoa-Carr, Morton and Bird2023) found that higher maternal elaboration during reminiscing was associated with children having fewer emotional problems and greater prosocial behavior at age 8. Overall, these findings suggest that less rich emotion discussion and a less elaborative reminiscing style is associated with greater child mental health problems across both internalizing (inward processes including anxiety and depression) and externalizing (behaviors expressed outward including aggression and hyperactivity) domains.

Transmission pathways

As noted, we know of only one study that has directly tested an indirect pathway from parent mental health to child mental health through reminiscing. Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) followed 206 infants and their mothers into childhood. Maternal depression was measured using the Brief Symptom Inventory-18 (Derogatis et al., Reference Derogatis, Savitz and Maruish2000) during infancy, and at age 7, children’s symptoms were measured using teacher report with the Child Behavior Checklist Teacher’s Report Form (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001). At a home visit when children were age 5, three reminiscing conversations were recorded and coded for elaborative reminiscing using utterance-based coding (Reese et al., Reference Reese, Haden and Fivush1993). Of note, the conversations were not emotionally focused; rather, they focused on novel shared mother-child experiences. Swetlitz et al., also coded for harsh and sensitive parenting (during a free play interaction) and explored this as a potential pathway for the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties as well as reminiscing style. Results indicated support for an indirect pathway through reminiscing, but not via harsh or sensitive parenting. Interestingly, the indirect pathway through less elaborative reminiscing was significant for teacher-reported children’s externalizing, but not teacher-reported internalizing difficulties. This pattern may be due to the inward nature of internalizing symptoms, which may not be as sensitive to detection by others, particularly schoolteachers who may be more aware of externalizing difficulties in a classroom setting (Papandrea & Winefield, Reference Papandrea and Winefield2011; Splett et al., Reference Splett, Garzona, Gibson, Wojtalewicz, Raborn and Reinke2019). Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021), however, did not measure child language, which may also play a role in the intergenerational transmission of mental health through reminiscing.

Language, mental health, and reminiscing

Reminiscing is a linguistic process, and children’s language ability is likely to be both shaped by their reminiscing experience, and contribute to the richness of the conversation. Parent reminiscing style is strongly related to children’s language skills (Salmon & Reese, Reference Salmon and Reese2016; Reese, Reference Reese2002; Waters et al., Reference Waters, Camia, Facompré and Fivush2019). The more complex language a child has, the more likely children are able to communicate accurately and emotionally their internal world and experience of difficult events. Likewise, maternal reminiscing interventions during childhood are associated with improved child narrative and language (Reese & Newcombe, Reference Reese and Newcombe2007) with effects of improved child narrative elaboration and coherence observed into early adolescence (Reese et al., Reference Reese, Macfarlane, McAnally, Robertson and Taumoepeau2020). It therefore makes sense that the social context is also important for this relationship: associations of child language skill have been found with maternal education (Hoff et al., Reference Hoff, Burridge, Ribot and Giguere2018) and socioeconomic status (Noel et al., Reference Noel, Peterson and Jesso2008). Moreover, accounting for ethnicity and child gender was important for clarifying the positive relationship between maternal elaborative reminiscing and child language skills in a recent meta-analysis (Waters et al., Reference Waters, Camia, Facompré and Fivush2019).

Viewed within Goodman and Gotlib’s (Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999) model, children’s language may be a key cognitive capacity that is influenced by parenting mechanisms and underlies the development of child mental health difficulties. Indeed, in a sample of 4432 mother-child dyads from the Growing Up in New Zealand study, Bird et al. (Reference Bird, Reese, Salmon, Waldie, Peterson, Atatoa-Carr and Morton2023) investigated the link between maternal depression symptoms during infancy, mother-child verbal interactions at age 2 and 4 years old, and children’s language development at 4.5 years old. They found a direct negative association between maternal depression symptoms and language skills in children at age 4.5 years old, as well as an indirect effect through self-reported (at 2 years old) and observed (at 4.5 years old) mother-child interactions. As children age, they develop more complex language skills; however, children with emotional or behavioral difficulties often have significantly lower than average language skills, and often these have not been noticed or assessed due to the child’s prevailing psychological issues (Chow & Hollo, Reference Chow and Hollo2022; Hollo et al., Reference Hollo, Wehby and Oliver2013). As lower early language skill has been linked with later mental health outcomes (see Salmon et al., Reference Salmon, O’Kearney, Reese and Fortune2016, for a review), understanding how child language is involved in the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties has important implications for the prevention and effective management of mental health difficulties.

The current study

The aim of the current study was to replicate and extend Swetlitz et al.’s (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) novel finding of an indirect association of parent to child mental health through mother-child reminiscing. Much reminiscing research has been limited by small sample sizes, varying approaches to coding, and potential reporting biases in the measurement of child mental health. We utilized data from Growing Up in New Zealand: a longitudinal pre-birth cohort study that began in 2009-2010 with over 6000 families (Morton et al., Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr, Grant, Robinson, Bandara, Bird, Ivory, Kingi, Liang, Marks, Perese, Peterson, Pryor, Reese, Schmidt, Waldie and Wall2013). The current analyses were based on reminiscing conversations from 1,234 mother-child dyads collected when children were 8 years old. This study extended existing findings by examining: both elaboration and resolution quality of mother-child reminiscing conversations; an independent measure of child language; child internalizing and externalizing symptoms (as reported by parents) and child anxiety and depression symptoms (self-reported by children). Specifically, we tested a serial mediation model of the intergenerational transmission of mental health symptoms, through mother-child reminiscing conversations and children’s language. We hypothesized that 1) maternal depression symptoms would be related to both lower elaboration and resolution quality during reminiscing (Cimino et al., Reference Cimino, Cerniglia, Tambelli, Ballarotto, Erriu, Paciello, Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2019; Reese et al., Reference Reese, Meins, Fernyhough and Centifanti2019), 2) both elaboration and resolution during reminiscing would mediate the relationship between parent and child mental health symptoms (Swetlitz et al., Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021), and 3) parent and child mental health would be related, through both elaboration and resolution during reminiscing and child language (see Figure 1 for a conceptual model).

Figure 1. Conceptual Model Adapted from Goodman and Gotlib (Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999).

Methods

Participants

Data for the current study was drawn from the pre-birth cohort study Growing Up in New Zealand. Participants were invited across three adjacent health boards of central North Island of New Zealand: Auckland, Manukau, and Waikato. The study was designed to investigate environmental factors that affect children’s wellbeing and development (Morton et al., Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr, Grant, Robinson, Bandara, Bird, Ivory, Kingi, Liang, Marks, Perese, Peterson, Pryor, Reese, Schmidt, Waldie and Wall2013). Face-to-face data collection began antenatally and continued at child aged 9-months old and 2, 4.5, 8 and 12 years old to date. The current study has a focus on a mother-child reminiscing task that was collected at the 8-year data collection wave (DCW). A total of 6,848 children are included in the larger cohort which is broadly representative of children born in Aotearoa New Zealand in 2009. The 8-year DCW included 5,012 dyads who completed the reminiscing task, of which approximately one quarter (n = 1404) were randomly selected and transcribed. To avoid repetition of the maternal data, twin and triplet sibling conversations were not included in the current analyses (n = 47). The final sample consisted of only those with complete data at the 8-year DCW for our variables of interest, of which were then coded (n = 1,234; n = 624 boys, n = 610 girls).

Procedure

Growing Up in New Zealand received ethical approval from the New Zealand Ministry of Health’s Northern Y Regional Ethics Committee (Morton et al., Reference Morton, Atatoa Carr, Grant, Robinson, Bandara, Bird, Ivory, Kingi, Liang, Marks, Perese, Peterson, Pryor, Reese, Schmidt, Waldie and Wall2013). At the antenatal DCW (predominantly during the last trimester of pregnancy), mothers answered questions about themselves and their family. Infancy measures were collected (home visit; interviewer administered) at child aged 9 months. At age 8, parents completed an online survey and children completed survey measures with an interviewer face-to-face in the home. The reminiscing conversation was recorded in the family home (with additional informed consent received for audio recording).

Reminiscing task

At the 8-year DCW, mothers and children completed a reminiscing task in their home, which was audio recorded. Parent-child conversations were initially trialed with Growing Up’s pilot group Te Rōpū Pīata/Leading Lights (n ≍ 140). Parents and children were provided with an open-ended discussion prompt, to choose a recent event where the child felt a little bit upset. Feedback from the pilot phase was that event selection was quite time-consuming and the decision was made to provide event selection options. The three most frequently discussed events from the pilot phase were then used in the full sample. Mothers and children chose from one of the three negative emotion topics: a time when the child had (1) a social disagreement, (2) injured themselves or (3) a disappointment (e.g., in a sports game or at school). Images were provided with the prompts to assist with topic selection, and the dyad were asked to talk about the event ‘as you usually would, for as long as you usually would.’ Conversations lasted for approximately 5 to 10 minutes. Within the current sample, the topic of child injury was the most frequently discussed (n = 691), then social disagreement (n = 320) and child disappointment (n = 223).

Reminiscing task coding

Four researchers coded the first ≈ 1,400 transcripts collected in temporal order. Separate coders coded for emotion resolution and maternal elaboration. The coding approaches utilized scale-based coding, which has been suggested as appropriate when examining child socio-emotional wellbeing (Leyva et al., Reference Leyva, Reese, Laible, Schaughency, Das and Clifford2020). Reliability coding was established (see below) and the remainder of the transcripts were coded independently. If any transcripts were unclear, the two coders consulted and decided on a score together to ensure continuity of reliability in coding. Once coding reliability was achieved, the initial coded transcripts were also re-checked.

Emotion resolution

Emotion resolution was adapted from the resolution/closure scale of Koren-Karie et al.’s (2003) Autobiographical Emotion Events Dialogue coding scheme. This scale focused on how mothers and children end their emotion discussion. Emotion resolution was coded on a 9-point scale, for mother and child separately. A low score of 1 would indicate that the mother enhanced negative emotions (e.g., “You could have died, you wouldn’t stop bleeding”); for the child, they similarly may resist their mother’s efforts for resolution and insist on repeating frightening details. A high score of 9 would indicate a conversation the mother or child ending with a positive moral or outcome, emphasizing the child’s strength and resilience. A score of 5 would indicate the mother may try to end the story positively but does not insist and therefore the child is left without resolution of negative feelings; the child may shift to a different part of the story without resolving the negative feelings or may accept a parent’s attempt at resolution without engaging in the ending. A random selection of transcripts was coded by two coders (the first and second author) for reliability (n = 180, 14.5%). An Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) was conducted, with a two-way random-effect model based on single measures and absolute agreement. ICC for maternal emotion resolution was .78, and for child emotion resolution was .79, both considered in the acceptable range (Syed & Nelson, Reference Syed and Nelson2015). The first author coded the remaining transcripts, with regular checks to prevent coding drift.

Maternal elaboration

Maternal elaboration was coded based on the Elaborative Reminiscing Scale (Laible, Reference Laible2004a; Leyva et al., Reference Leyva, Reese, Laible, Schaughency, Das and Clifford2020). The scale is an overall measure of elaborative quality, which is rated on a 5-point Likert scale with one indicating low-elaborative reminiscing and five indicating high-elaborative reminiscing. A low score of 1 indicated that a mother used predominantly factual or closed questions, was repetitive and may have negated the child’s contributions. A high score of 5 indicates the mother utilized mostly open-ended questions, confirmed and elaborated or expanded on the child’s contributions and rather than repeat information, elicited more novel and in-depth information. A mid-score of 3 indicated that a mother used a balance of open and closed questions, elaborated on the child’s contributions moderately, and were moderately repetitive in information provided. A random selection of transcripts was coded for reliability by two independent coders (n = 173, 14%). The two coders utilized this coding in their research investigating behavioral difficulties (Garnett et al., Reference Garnett, Reese, Swearingen, Peterson, Salmon, Waldie, D’souza, Atatoa-Carr, Morton and Bird2023) and infant temperament (Swearingen et al., Reference Swearingen, Reese, Garnett, Peterson, Salmon, Atatoa Carr, Morton and Bird2023). Inter-rater reliability was calculated using ICC with a two-way random-effect model based on single measures and absolute agreement, at .81 in the acceptable range (Syed & Nelson, Reference Syed and Nelson2015).

Measures

Table 1 presents measures of relevance for the current study at each DCW. Further information about the range of measures, data collection waves and scope of the Growing Up in New Zealand study can be found at www.growingup.co.nz.

Table 1. Details of measures included across multiple growing up data collection waves (DCW)

Sociodemographic variables

At the antenatal DCW, mothers self-reported sociodemographic information, which is reported in Table 2. This included maternal age, education level and self-prioritized ethnicity. Education level was reported as highest level of completed qualification. Mother’s self-prioritized ethnicity was allocated to a single level 1 self-prioritized ethnic group for data analysis purposes: New Zealand European, Māori (Indigenous peoples of New Zealand), Pacific Peoples, Asian, MELAA (Middle Eastern, Latin American and African) and Other (Morton et al., Reference Morton, Walker, Gerritsen, Smith, Cha, Bird, Bullen, Atatoa-Carr, Chen, Exeter, Fa’alili-Fidow, Fenaughty, Grant, Kim, Kingi, Lai, Langridge, Marks, Meissel and Wall2020; Statistics New Zealand, 2005). Area-level socioeconomic deprivation was derived from the domicile address of participants, measured using the New Zealand Index of Deprivation (NZDep; Salmond et al., Reference Salmond, Crampton and Atkinson2007) which categorizes the relative deprivation level of small geographical areas across New Zealand from information gathered in the population-based census. Scores range from decile 1 indicating the 10% areas of least socioeconomic deprivation, to 10 – the most deprived areas (Salmond et al., Reference Salmond, Crampton and Atkinson2007).

Table 2. Breakdown of participant demographics for current subsample (N = 1234)

Note. *Middle Eastern, Latin American, and African.

Maternal mental health

Edinburgh postnatal depression scale (EPDS) (Cox et al., Reference Cox, Holden and Sagovsky1987)

The EPDS is a 10-item measure of depression symptoms, commonly used in the postnatal period. Mothers completed the EPDS when infants were 9-months old. Each response item is scored on a four-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher distress. The EPDS has good user acceptability and may be used outside of the immediate perinatal window with satisfactory sensitivity and specificity (J. L. Cox et al., Reference Cox, Chapman, Murray and Jones1996) The EPDS has been widely used within the New Zealand population (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Bird, Peterson, Underwood, Morton and Grant2020; Theunissen et al., Reference Theunissen, D’Souza, Peterson, Walker, Morton and Waldie2022), in both antenatal and postnatal cohorts (Underwood et al., Reference Underwood, Waldie, D’Souza, Peterson and Morton2017). Cronbach’s alpha for the EPDS in the current sample was 0.843.

Child mental health

Child self-reported anxiety

Ten items were used to measure child self-reported anxiety symptoms: eight items came from the PROMIS (Patient Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System Pediatric short form v2.0; Pilkonis et al., Reference Pilkonis, Choi, Reise, Stover, Riley and Cella2011) and two items from the NIH fear toolbox. For all ten items, children self-reported anxiety symptoms in the past seven days, on a Likert scale from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). Examples of questions included “in the past 7 days I felt like something awful might happen” and “in the past 7 days, I worried when I went to bed at night”. The PROMIS measure is validated for children aged 8–17 years old, and is reliable at 0.85 (Irwin et al., Reference Irwin, Stucky, Langer, Thissen, DeWitt, Lai, Varni, Yeatts and DeWalt2010). Cronbach’s alpha for child anxiety in the current sample was 0.872.

Centre for epidemiological studies depression scale for children (Fendrich et al., Reference Fendrich, Weissman and Warner1990)

The CES-D-10 is a screening tool for symptoms of depression in children. Children responded to 10 items where they self-reported symptoms on a 4-point scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (a lot). Items cover emotional symptoms of depressed mood, guilt, hopelessness, and physical symptoms of appetite, movement, and sleep difficulties. A recent validation study for the CES-D-10 using the larger Growing Up in New Zealand sample at the 8-year DCW, reported the tool to be valid and reliable without the ‘hopeful’ item (Cha et al., Reference Cha, Waldie, Neumann, Smith and Walker2022); as such, we have excluded this item and the total score is the sum of the remaining nine items. Cronbach’s alpha for the nine-item scale with the current sample was 0.724.

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire – parent report (R. Goodman, Reference Goodman2001)

Children’s internalizing and externalizing difficulties were measured by mother-report on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) at the 8-year DCW. The SDQ can be completed by a parent or teacher (for children aged 3-16 years old), or by the child themselves (11-16 years old). The SDQ is a 25-item questionnaire, with five subscales: emotional problems; peer problems; hyperactivity; behavioral problems; and prosocial behavior. Overall internalizing or externalizing constructs can be derived from the grouping of subscales and has been recommended as a viable option rather than the separate subscales more suitable for high-risk populations or screening for specific disorders (A. Goodman et al., Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010). The questions are rated on a 3-point scale of 0 ‘not true’, 1 ‘somewhat true’, and 2 ‘certainly true’. The sum of internalizing (emotional problems and peer problems) or externalizing (hyperactivity and behavioral problems) constructs ranges from 0 to 20. The SDQ has been reported to have good reliability and validity and is used in New Zealand in national screening programs in early childhood (R. Goodman, Reference Goodman2001; Richards et al., Reference Richards, Reith, Stitely and Smith2019).

Picture vocabulary test (PVT) (Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Cella, Fox, Havlik, Hendrie and Wagster2010)

To measure children’s receptive vocabulary, the PVT from the NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery was utilized (Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Cella, Fox, Havlik, Hendrie and Wagster2010). The PVT is a measure of receptive vocabulary administered on an iPad for participants aged 3–85 years old. The measure takes approximately four minutes to administer, as it uses computerized adaptive testing to tailor questions based on the previous response. Children were presented with four images and an audio recorded word and asked to select the picture that most accurately corresponds with the word. The application automatically scores the test and provides age-corrected and uncorrected standard scores. The PVT has high test-retest reliability and good construct validity (Gershon et al., Reference Gershon, Cook, Mungas, Manly, Slotkin, Beaumont and Weintraub2014), and the PVT has been successfully utilized in previous Growing Up in New Zealand research (Neumann et al., Reference Neumann, Peterson, Underwood, Morton and Waldie2021).

Data analysis

Analyses were conducted using IBM Statistical Package for the Social Sciences version 28.0. Categorical variables were dummy coded in preparation for analysis (maternal education, ethnicity, and conversation topic). Four dummy variables for maternal self-reported ethnicity were created: New Zealand European vs. other (all other ethnic groups), Māori (Indigenous peoples of New Zealand) vs. other, Asian vs. other and Pacific Peoples vs. other. The remaining category of MELAA (Middle Eastern, Latin American and African) participants (n = 16) was too small to create a separate dummy variable for analysis. Continuous variables were deemed normally distributed and had no univariate outliers. Prior to analysis assumptions were tested, and no significant multicollinearity was detected. Covariates were selected for their known associations with reminiscing in current and previous samples (Garnett et al., Reference Garnett, Reese, Swearingen, Peterson, Salmon, Waldie, D’souza, Atatoa-Carr, Morton and Bird2023; Swearingen et al., Reference Swearingen, Reese, Garnett, Peterson, Salmon, Atatoa Carr, Morton and Bird2023). Multivariable regression analyses were conducted to identify covariates for inclusion in the serial mediation models.

The main analyses consisted of serial mediation modelling with bootstrapping to test the relationship between maternal depression symptoms to child mental health outcomes through reminiscing and child language using Hayes (Reference Hayes2009) PROCESS macro. The PROCESS macro uses bias-corrected bootstrapping based on 5000 bootstrap samples with a 95% confidence interval (CI) (Preacher & Hayes, 2004, 2008). When the confidence intervals do not cross zero, the indirect pathway is supported. Separate serial mediation models were conducted for each combination of the reminiscing measures (elaboration and emotion resolution) and child mental health measures (depression, anxiety, internalizing and externalizing), totaling 12 mediation models which concurrently tested all three hypotheses. When testing specific models and not exploring latent variables, PROCESS produces comparable results to a structural equation modelling approach (Hayes, Reference Hayes, Montoya and Rockwood2017).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Missing data varied across scales (see Table 3). Only complete data for scales were included in the analysis, and analyses were conducted on reduced samples for those variables with missing data. Descriptive statistics conducted for mother and child variables of interest are shown in Table 3. Table 4 details bivariate correlations between study variables. Multivariable regression analyses were conducted with separate models for maternal elaboration, maternal emotion resolution, and child emotion resolution (Table 5) to identify covariates for inclusion in the serial mediation models. For maternal elaboration quality, mother age (β = .013, p = .01), injury topic vs other (β = -.349, p = <.001), and maternal education (β = .224, p = <.001) were significant predictors, together explaining 7.2% of the variance. Demographics of area deprivation, ethnic groups, the disappointment topic, and child gender did not significantly contribute to the model. For maternal emotion resolution, disappointment topic (β = .566, p = <.001), and maternal education (β = .259, p = .002) were significant predictors, together explaining 6.4% of the variance. Mother age, child gender, the injury topic, area deprivation and ethnic groups were not significant predictors for maternal emotion resolution. For child emotion resolution, maternal education (β = .233, p = .008), and the injury (β = .278, p = .001), and disappointment topics (β = .527, p = <.001) were significant predictors, together explaining 3.9% of the variance. Mother age, child gender, area deprivation and ethnic groups were not significant predictors of child emotion resolution. Those variables significantly associated were included as covariates in subsequent reminiscing mediation models.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for main study variables

Table 4. Bivariate correlations between study variables

* p < 0.05. **p < 0.01. ***p < .001.

Table 5. Regression model predicting reminiscing variables

Note. N = 1,234; *p <.05. ** p <.01.

Indirect pathways

Mediation models are reported in Table 6. Across all twelve models, greater maternal depression symptoms were significantly associated with lower elaboration and lower resolution quality, whilst accounting for covariates. Simple mediation revealed no significant indirect pathway between maternal depression symptoms and child symptoms when accounting for covariates in the models. Partial serial mediation was found from maternal depressions symptoms through maternal elaboration and child language skills for models predicting children’s anxiety (effect = .0015, SE = .0010, 95% CIs = .0001; .0041), depression (effect = .0009, SE = .0006, 95% CIs = .0001, .0023; shown in Figure 2 as a visual depiction of the serial mediation models), and externalizing symptoms (effect = .0003, SE = .0003, 95% CIs = .0000; .0011). Similarly, partial serial mediation was observed for models including maternal emotion resolution and child language, for children’s anxiety (effect = .0020, SE = .0013, 95% CIs = .0000; .0050), depression (effect = .0011, SE = .0007, 95% CIs = .0000, .0027), and externalizing symptoms (effect = .0005, SE = .0004, 95% CIs = .0000; .0014). There was no support for an indirect pathway between maternal depression symptoms and child outcomes, through child emotion resolution and child language. Of note, effect sizes are shown in Table 6.

Figure 2. Serial mediation model of maternal depression symptoms to child depression symptoms through elaboration and child language skills.

Note. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 6. Indirect effect coefficients for the serial mediation models of maternal depression on child mental health, mediated through parent-child reminiscing (m1) and child language (m2)

Note. *p < .05. **p < .01 ***. p < .001.

Discussion

Drawing on data from a large, diverse, contemporary longitudinal cohort study, the current findings enrich our understanding of the mechanisms involved in the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties. There was support for our first hypothesis that maternal depression symptoms would be associated with reminiscing. After controlling for covariates, maternal depression symptoms measured during infancy were associated across time with maternal elaboration, maternal emotion resolution, and child emotion resolution at age 8 years in all models. Secondly, we hypothesized that reminiscing outcomes would mediate the pathway between maternal depression symptoms and child mental health outcomes. However, contrary to findings by Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021), we found no simple mediation through reminiscing. Instead, findings supported our third hypothesis that the relationship between maternal depression symptoms and child outcomes would be mediated through both reminiscing and child language skills. That is, together maternal reminiscing and child language skills partially explain the association between parent and child mental health outcomes. Serial mediation with small effect sizes was observed for both maternal elaboration and maternal resolution, but not child resolution, through child language skills. Supported models predicted child self-reported anxiety and depression, and mother-reported externalizing symptoms, but not mother-reported internalizing symptoms. These findings provide novel insight into the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties through mother-child interaction and child language.

Maternal depression symptoms were significantly related to maternal elaborative reminiscing, maternal emotion resolution, and child emotion resolution in all models. Support for a longitudinal association (spanning more than 7 years) between early maternal depression symptoms and later maternal elaboration is notable, considering the mixed findings in past literature. Only Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) has reported a significant longitudinal association after controlling for covariates (cf. Reese et al., Reference Reese, Meins, Fernyhough and Centifanti2019). As suggested by past research, scale-based elaboration coding may better reflect associations with wellbeing or mental health outcomes rather than frequency or utterance-based coding (Leyva et al., Reference Leyva, Reese, Laible, Schaughency, Das and Clifford2020; Valentino et al., Reference Valentino, Nuttall, Comas, McDonnell, Piper, Thomas and Fanuele2014). This may be due to the ability to draw meaning from the conversation in totality, which is likely to be more relevant as children develop greater language and narrative skills in middle childhood (Grysman & Mansfield, Reference Grysman, Mansfield, Gülgöz and Sahin-Acar2020). The longitudinal association between maternal depression and both mother and child emotion resolution quality are also in line with Cimino et al.’s (Reference Cimino, Cerniglia, Tambelli, Ballarotto, Erriu, Paciello, Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2019) concurrent finding of lower maternal sensitive guidance for mothers with a depression diagnosis.

More limited access to coping skills may explain the relationship between maternal depression symptoms and lower emotion resolution, for both mothers and children. As demonstrated by Dyad A (Appendix A), a high-quality resolution may require the parent to scaffold multiple potential ways of coping with the emotions triggered by the event, seeking support from others, and/or preventing this type of event recurring. For some mothers, it may be difficult to resolve emotions due to difficulty recalling specific instances of adaptive information; over general memory (OGM) is not only often comorbid with depression but is a mechanism which predicts its onset and course (Valentino, Reference Valentino2011). OGM may affect the recall of positive details to adaptively resolve the emotion for their children (Gibbs & Rude, Reference Gibbs and Rude2004). OGM has also been implicated in the intergenerational transmission of mental health through reminiscing (Valentino, Reference Valentino2011).

It is noteworthy that two existing longitudinal studies did not find an association between maternal depression symptoms and reminiscing in early childhood (Raikes & Thompson, Reference Raikes and Thompson2006; Reese et al., 2019), particularly given reminiscing style is thought to be consolidated during this developmental period. Given that developmental effects are cumulative (Sroufe, Reference Sroufe2013), associations between maternal depression in infancy and reminiscing in middle childhood may reflect compounding differences in reminiscing which become more apparent as children age. These associations may also reflect transactional relationships between parent mental health and emerging child mental health difficulties, both with influence on the emerging dyadic reminiscing style. Further research is needed measuring parent and child mental health and reminiscing at multiple points across the preschool and childhood years.

Our second and third hypotheses were tested concurrently. The second hypothesis predicted that reminiscing would mediate the pathway between mother and child mental health. However, no simple mediation effect through reminiscing was found, suggesting that although the relationship between early maternal depression and reminiscing at age 8 occurs, a more complex mediation may explain the relationship with child mental health outcomes. Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) previously observed mediation of maternal depression symptoms (6 months post-partum) to child externalizing symptoms (school/teacher reported at 7 years old) through maternal elaborative reminiscing. The differences between studies may be due to differences in research design. Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) did not include child language in their models as a variable or covariate. Indeed, when adding child language skills to the model, there was support for hypothesis 3. We predicted a serial mediation of mental health symptoms from mother to child, through mother-child reminiscing and child language. Serial mediation models predicting child-reported anxiety and depression, and mother-reported child externalizing symptoms were all significantly mediated through maternal reminiscing (elaboration and resolution but not child resolution) and child language skills.

The finding of serial mediation through reminiscing and child language is a novel and significant contribution to our understanding of the intergenerational transmission of mental health. Children who experience lower parental elaboration are more likely to have smaller receptive vocabularies, likely through a lack of exposure to rich and complex language (Salmon et al., Reference Salmon, O’Kearney, Reese and Fortune2016). Interestingly, our findings also supported a serial mediation pathway through parent resolution and child language. As observed in the provided transcript excerpt, a high-quality resolution tends to label internal states, highlight different internal states and perspectives, and extends temporally from what happened in the past to how this could be managed differently in the future. Our findings suggest that this linguistic complexity is related to children’s receptive vocabulary development and in turn associated with reduced externalizing difficulties and anxiety and depression symptoms.

Our serial mediation parallels Swetlitz et al.’s (2019) simple mediation in prediction of externalizing but not internalizing symptoms on the SDQ. As suggested by Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021), these patterns of results may not necessarily suggest a different pathway for internalizing difficulties, but an age-related expression of symptoms. Younger children typically exhibit externalizing symptoms; as children develop there is a shift toward internalized difficulties (Loth et al., Reference Loth, Drabick, Leibenluft and Hulvershorn2014). As noted by Swetlitz et al., in relation to their teacher report measurement of child symptoms, it may be more difficult to capture internalizing symptoms when reported by others due to the covert and inward nature of internalizing symptoms (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Ford, Dyer-Friedman, Tang and Huffman2004). Historically, internalizing symptomology has lower parent and child agreement than externalizing difficulties (Karver, Reference Karver2006; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Ford, Dyer-Friedman, Tang and Huffman2004). The current study addressed this potential limitation by also including two self-reported measures of child internalizing symptoms and it is important to note that all serial mediation models predicting children’s self-reported anxiety and depression symptoms through maternal reminiscing and child language skills were supported.

Notably, none of the models with indirect pathways through child resolution were supported, despite the association between maternal depression symptoms and child emotion resolution. This pattern could indicate that parents’ input in resolution at this age is more influential for children’s mental health or could suggest another mechanism may be associated with child emotion resolution in the mediation process. In their model, Goodman and Gotlib (Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999) predicted the development of vulnerabilities in the intergenerational transmission of maternal mental health to child mental health difficulties. Here we have tested child language skills as a key vulnerability; however, other cognitive and affective skills, such as emotion understanding (Raikes & Thompson, Reference Raikes and Thompson2006) or emotion regulation (Compas et al., Reference Compas, Jaser, Bettis, Watson, Gruhn, Dunbar, Williams and Thigpen2017), may better elucidate a path with children’s emotion resolution quality. In support, Van Bergen and Salmon (Reference van Bergen and Salmon2010) found that both maternal elaboration and discussion of the causes of their child’s emotions were concurrently associated with children’s emotion understanding.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of this study include the large and diverse sample, the longitudinal design, measurement of child mental health across internalizing and externalizing symptoms and measurement of internalizing symptoms based on both parent and child report. The findings should, however, be interpreted alongside consideration of limitations. The effect sizes, although significant, are small. This pattern indicates that there are likely other pathways that also explain the intergenerational transmission of mental health difficulties. Other specific mechanisms that may mediate this pathway include heritability of depression, innate dysfunction of neuroregulatory mechanisms, exposure to stressful environments, and other types of parent-child interactions (Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999). In particular, Swetlitz et al. (Reference Swetlitz, Lynch, Propper, Coffman and Wagner2021) tested their reminiscing pathway against observed harsh-intrusive and sensitive parenting, but we did not have another observed interaction to test alongside reminiscing in this way. In addition, our measure of child language reflects receptive vocabulary knowledge, but expressive language will likely also be important to consider (Aoyagi et al., Reference Aoyagi, Takei, Nishimura, Nomura and Tsuchiya2019; Girard et al., Reference Girard, Pingault, Doyle, Falissard and Tremblay2016; Ripley & Yuill, Reference Ripley and Yuill2005).

Moreover, our study only investigated mother-child dyads; the limited research investigating parent gender differences shows mixed results but suggests maternal and paternal reminiscing may fulfill unique functions (Aznar & Tenenbaum, Reference Aznar and Tenenbaum2015; Buckner & Fivush, Reference Buckner and Fivush IV2000; Pavlova et al., Reference Pavlova, Mueri, Peterson, Graham and Noel2022). As such, these results should be interpreted for mother-child dyads only and further research is needed to examine both intergenerational transmission of paternal mental health difficulties and how one parent’s mental health and/or reminiscing style might buffer or moderate the impact of another parent’s mental health symptoms (Goodman & Gotlib, Reference Goodman and Gotlib1999). The design of this study does not allow for a true longitudinal model; future research should also investigate the association of reminiscing from age 8 to later child mental health (Mitchell & Reese, Reference Mitchell and Reese2022). Future research could also examine whether maternal mental health at other developmental stages (i.e., beyond infancy) is also related to child outcomes through reminiscing and child language. This pathway should also be investigated in a clinical cohort; recent research observed concurrent associations between maternal depression symptoms and quality of elaborative reminiscing, although only for parents in a community cohort, not the clinical cohort of children referred with internalizing symptoms (Russell et al., Reference Russell, Bird, McNamara and Herbert2023). It would also be helpful to extend measurement of maternal mental health beyond depression, for example to consider personality and/or trauma difficulties (Judd et al., Reference Judd, Newman and Komiti2018). Moreover, different trajectories of transmission may appear over time and should be investigated within the context of ongoing cumulative socialization. We also conducted multiple serial mediation models which may increase risk of Type 1 error and replication of these findings in further samples is needed.

Our study included mother’s self-prioritized ethnicity in preliminary regression models to account for any variance; however, this was not a significant predictor of reminiscing and was therefore not included in subsequent mediation models. Despite this, reminiscing occurs within a socio-cultural context. It is likely that more nuanced measurement of reminiscing (particularly utterance and content-based coding) would elucidate differences in reminiscing based on ethnicity. Reminiscing has been shown to be important for Māori, with increased cultural affiliation being associated with more frequent reminiscing about a diverse range of topics (Reese & Neha, Reference Reese and Neha2015). Research using a similar sample from Growing Up in New Zealand found differences in reminiscing as a function of maternal ethnicity; however, maternal ethnicity did not moderate the association between maternal elaboration and child mental health (Garnett et al., Reference Garnett, Reese, Swearingen, Peterson, Salmon, Waldie, D’souza, Atatoa-Carr, Morton and Bird2023). Previous research has also shown European-American mothers to focus more on internal states and causes and consequences, promoting self-understanding, and Chinese mothers to focus more on subjective states with others, reflecting group-oriented relatedness (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Doan and Song2010; Wang & Fivush, Reference Wang and Fivush2005). Of note, Song and Wang (2020) found stronger associations between parent causal, social, and coping talk during reminiscing and positive self-concept for children of Chinese immigrant mothers compared with children of European-American mothers. Further research is needed to understand the specific aspects of parent-child reminiscing that relate to more positive wellbeing outcomes for different cultural groups. At the age 8 DCW, children were asked about their gender identity, however inclusion of this was outside the scope of this study and we opted to include only sex assigned at birth. Gender is socially constructed and there is some evidence of associations between gender typical identity and emotion expression when recalling memories (Grysman et al., Reference Grysman, Merrill and Fivush2017). We would therefore like to acknowledge the limitation in considering only binary gender.

Conclusions

Maternal reminiscing, along with child language, serially mediated the pathway from maternal depression symptoms in infancy to child mental health outcomes of anxiety, depression, and externalizing difficulties. Early maternal depression was consistently linked with both mother and child reminiscing at age 8. These findings highlight the importance of mother-child emotion reminiscing for children’s developing mental health and point to opportunities for disruption of this transmission pathway at three modifiable stages: early maternal depression, mother-child reminiscing conversations, and children’s language acquisition (Corsano et al., Reference Corsano and Guidotti2019; Reese et al., Reference Reese, Sparks and Leyva2010; Reese & Newcombe, Reference Reese and Newcombe2007; van Bergen et al., Reference Van Bergen, Salmon, Dadds and Allen2009). Reminiscing is modifiable for both community and clinical families (Corsano et al., Reference Corsano and Guidotti2019). Moreover, the benefits of reminiscing coaching have been observed with only a handful of sessions (1-6; Corsano et al., Reference Corsano and Guidotti2019), suggesting this may be a cost-effective and scalable approach to promote healthy mother-child relationships and support children’s mental health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424000919

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the funding support by the Australian Rotary Health PhD scholarship from Josephine Margaret Redfern & Ross Edward Redfern (Rotary Club of Granville)

Funding statement

This work was supported by the Australian Rotary Health PhD scholarship from Josephine Margaret Redfern & Ross Edward Redfern (Rotary Club of Granville) (SR, 2020). Growing Up in New Zealand has been funded by New Zealand government ministries including the Ministries of Social Development, Health, Education, Justice and Pacific Island Affairs; the former Ministries of Science Innovation and of Women’s Affairs; the former Department of Labour; the Department of Corrections; Te Puni Kokiri; New Zealand Police; as well as by The University of Auckland and Auckland UniServices Limited. Transcription of the parent-child conversations was supported by a grant from the Oakley Mental Health Research Foundation.

Competing interests

The authors declare they have no known conflict of interest.

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school- age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. ASEBA.Google Scholar
Aoyagi, S.-S., Takei, N., Nishimura, T., Nomura, Y., & Tsuchiya, K. J. (2019). Association of late-onset postpartum depression of mothers with expressive language development during infancy and early childhood: The HBC study. PeerJ, 7(3), e6566. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aznar, A., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2015). Gender and age differences in parent-child emotion talk. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 148155. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12069 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, A., & Reese, E. (2006). Emotional reminiscing and the development of an autobiographical self. Developmental Psychology, 42(4), 613626. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.613 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, A., Reese, E., Salmon, K., Waldie, K., Peterson, E., Atatoa-Carr, P., & Morton, S. (2023). Maternal depressive symptoms and child language development: Exploring potential pathways through observed and self-reported mother-child verbal interactions. Development and Psychopathology, 114. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001311 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and integrative conceptual framework. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 602654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038662 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brumariu, L. E., & Kerns, K. A. (2015). Mother-child emotion communication and childhood anxiety symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 29(3), 416431. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.917070 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, J. P., & Fivush IV, R. (2000). Gendered themes in family reminiscing. Memory, 8(6), 401412. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210050156859 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cha, J. E., Waldie, K. E., Neumann, D., Smith, A., & Walker, C. G. (2022). Psychometric properties and factor structure of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale 10-item short form (CES-D-10) in Aotearoa New Zealand children. Journal of Affective Disorders Reports, 7, 100298. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JADR.2021.100298 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chow, J. C., & Hollo, A. E. (2022). Language skills of students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Intervention in School and Clinic, 58(1), 4650. https://doi.org/10.1177/10534512211047584 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cimino, S., Cerniglia, L., Tambelli, R., Ballarotto, G., Erriu, M., Paciello, M., Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2019). Dialogues about emotional events between mothers with anxiety, depression, anorexia nervosa, and no diagnosis and their children. Parenting, 20(1), 6982. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2019.1642688 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Compas, B. E., Jaser, S. S., Bettis, A. H., Watson, K. H., Gruhn, M. A., Dunbar, J. P., Williams, E., Thigpen, J. C. (2017). Coping, emotion regulation and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review. Physiology & Behavior, 143(9), 939991. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000110 Google ScholarPubMed
Corsano, P., & Guidotti, L. (2019). Parents’ reminiscing training in typically developing and at-risk children: A review. Early Child Development and Care, 189(1), 143156. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2017.1289518 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, J. L., Chapman, G., Murray, D., & Jones, P. (1996). Validation of the edinburgh postnatal depression scale (EPDS) in non-postnatal women. Journal of Affective Disorders, 39(3), 185189. https://doi.org/10.1016/0165-0327(96)00008-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cox, J. L., Holden, J. M., & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression: Development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. British Journal of Psychiatry, 150(6), 782786. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.150.6.782 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, L. R., Savitz, K. L., & Maruish, M. (2000). Handbook of psychological assessment in primary care settings. In The SCL-90-R and brief symptom inventory (BSI) in primary care. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2020). Findings, issues, and new directions for research on emotion socialisation. Developmental Psychology, 56(3), 664670. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000906 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialisation of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9(4), 241273. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fendrich, M., Weissman, M. M., & Warner, V. (1990). Screening for depressive disorder in children and adolescents: Validating the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale for children. American Journal of Epidemiology, 131(3), 538551. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115529 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. (1991). The socialisation of children’s cognition and emotion: Toward an integrative view. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37(1), 5981. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23087339 Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (2001). Owning experience: Developing subjective perspective in autobiographical narratives. In Moore, C., Lemmon, K., & Skene, K. (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives (pp. 3552). Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (2007). Maternal reminiscing style and children’s developing understanding of self and emotion. Clinical Social Work Journal, 35(1), 3746. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10615-006-0065-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaborations: Role of maternal reminiscing style in cognitive and socioemotional development. Child Development, 77(6), 15681588. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2006). Parent-child reminiscing locates the self in the past. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 235251. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X57747 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., & Vasudeva, A. (2002). Remembering to relate: Socioemotional correlates of mother-child reminiscing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3(1), 7390. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0301_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franz, A. O., & McKinney, C. (2018). Parental and child psychopathology: Moderated mediation by gender and parent-child relationship quality. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 49(6), 843852. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-018-0801-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galbally, M., & Lewis, A. J. (2017). Depression and parenting: The need for improved intervention models. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 6165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnett, M., Reese, E., Swearingen, I., Peterson, E., Salmon, K., Waldie, K., D’souza, S., Atatoa-Carr, P., Morton, S., & Bird, A. (2023). Maternal reminiscing and children’s socioemotional development: Evidence from a large pre-birth longitudinal cohort study, growing up in New Zealand. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1-26(5), 678703. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2023.2192276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gershon, R. C., Cella, D., Fox, N. A., Havlik, R. J., Hendrie, H. C., & Wagster, M. V. (2010). Assessment of neurological and behavioral function: The NIH toolbox. The Lancet Neurology, 9(2), 138139. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(09)70335-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershon, R. C., Cook, K. F., Mungas, D., Manly, J. J., Slotkin, J., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. (2014). Language measures of the NIH toolbox cognition battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20(6), 642651. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617714000411 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, B. R., & Rude, S. S. (2004). Overgeneral autobiographical memory as depression vulnerability. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28(4), 511526. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:COTR.0000045561.72997.7c CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girard, L.-C., Pingault, J.-B., Doyle, O., Falissard, B., & Tremblay, R. E. (2016). Developmental associations between conduct problems and expressive language in early childhood: A population-based study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(6), 10331043. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0094-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to use broader internalizing and externalizing subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8), 11791191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9434-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(11), 13371345. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review, 106(3), 458490. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.458 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., & Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grysman, A., & Mansfield, C. D. (2020). Narrative methods in autobiographical memory. In Gülgöz, S., & Sahin-Acar, B. (Eds.), Autobiographical memory development: Theoretical and methodological approaches (pp. 6782). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429022791-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grysman, A., Merrill, N., & Fivush, R. (2017). Emotion, gender, and gender typical identity in autobiographical memory. Memory, 25(3), 289297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408420. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750903310360 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A. F., Montoya, A. K., & Rockwood, N. J. (2017). The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modeling. Australasian Marketing Journal, 25(1), 7681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E., Burridge, A., Ribot, K. M., & Giguere, D. (2018). Language specificity in the relation of maternal education to bilingual children’s vocabulary growth. Developmental Psychology, 54(6), 10111019. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000492 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollo, A., Wehby, J. H., & Oliver, R. M. (2013). Unidentified language deficits in children with emotional and behavioral disorders: A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 80(2), 169186. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440291408000203 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, D. E., Stucky, B., Langer, M. M., Thissen, D., DeWitt, E. M., Lai, J.-S., Varni, J. W., Yeatts, K., & DeWalt, D. A. (2010). An item response analysis of the pediatric PROMIS anxiety and depressive symptoms scales. Quality of Life Research, 19(4), 595607. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-010-9619-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Judd, F., Newman, L. K., & Komiti, A. A. (2018). Time for a new zeitgeist in perinatal mental health. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 52(2), 112116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karver, M. S. (2006). Determinants of multiple informant agreement on child and adolescent behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(2), 242253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-9015-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, Y., Bird, A., Peterson, E., Underwood, L., Morton, S. M. B., & Grant, C. C. (2020). Maternal antenatal depression and early childhood sleep: Potential pathways through infant temperament. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 45(2), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsaa001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren-Karie, N., Oppenheim, D., & Getzler-Yosef, R. (2008). Shaping children’s internal working models through mother-child dialogues: The importance of resolving past maternal trauma. Attachment and Human Development, 10(4), 465483. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730802461482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren-Karie, N., & Oppenhiem, D.Autobiographical emotional events dialogues: Coding manual, science open 2003, https://www.scienceopen.com/document?vid=5a697f21-d3e5-4cc3-a6ae-5bd149e91fe7.Google Scholar
Laible, D. (2004a). Mother-child discourse in two contexts: Links with child temperament, attachment security, and socioemotional competence. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 979992. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.979 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laible, D. (2004b). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child’s past behavior at 30 months: Links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50(2), 159180. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2004.0013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laible, D. (2011). Does it matter if preschool children and mothers discuss positive vs. negative events during reminiscing? Links with mother-reported attachment, family emotional climate, and socioemotional development. Social Development, 20(2), 394411. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00584.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyva, D., Reese, E., Laible, D., Schaughency, E., Das, S., & Clifford, A. (2020). Measuring parents’ elaborative reminiscing: Differential links of parents’ elaboration to children’s autobiographical memory and socioemotional skills. Journal of Cognition and Development, 21(1), 2345. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2019.1668395 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loth, A. K., Drabick, D. A. G., Leibenluft, E., & Hulvershorn, L. A. (2014). Do childhood externalizing disorders predict adult depression? A meta-analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(7), 11031113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-014-9867-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000). Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(5), 561592. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00100-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. L., Ford, C. B., Dyer-Friedman, J., Tang, J., & Huffman, L. C. (2004). Patterns of agreement between parent and child ratings of emotional and behavioral problems in an outpatient clinical setting: When children endorse more problems. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(3), 150155. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004703-200406000-00002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, C., & Reese, E. (2022). Growing memories: Coaching mothers in elaborative reminiscing with toddlers’ benefits adolescents’ turning-point narratives and wellbeing. Journal of Personality, 90(6), 887901. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12703 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, S. M. B., Atatoa Carr, P. E., Grant, C. C., Robinson, E. M., Bandara, D. K., Bird, A., Ivory, V. C., Kingi, T. K. R., Liang, R., Marks, E. J., Perese, L. M., Peterson, E. R., Pryor, J. E., Reese, E., Schmidt, J. M., Waldie, K. E., & Wall, C. (2013). Cohort profile: Growing up in New Zealand. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyr206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, S. M. B., Walker, C., Gerritsen, S., Smith, A., Cha, J., Bird, A., Bullen, P., Atatoa-Carr, P., Chen, R., Exeter, D., Fa’alili-Fidow, J., Fenaughty, J., Grant, C., Kim, H., Kingi, T., Lai, H., Langridge, F., Marks, E. J., Meissel, K.…, Wall, C. Now We Are Eight: Life in Middle Childhood. Growing Up in New Zealand, 2020. Report.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111(2), 486511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.111.2.486 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumann, D., Peterson, E. R., Underwood, L., Morton, S. M. B., & Waldie, K. E. (2021). Exploring the factor structure of the NIH toolbox cognition battery in a large sample of 8-year-old children in Aotearoa New Zealand. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27(8), 825834. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720001265 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noel, M., Peterson, C., & Jesso, B. (2008). The relationship of parenting stress and child temperament to language development among economically disadvantaged preschoolers. Journal of Child Language, 35(4), 823843. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papandrea, K., & Winefield, H. (2011). It’s not just the squeaky wheels that need the oil: Examining teachers’ views on the disparity between referral rates for students with internalizing versus externalizing problems. School Mental Health, 3(4), 222235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-011-9063-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pate, A., Salmon, K., Fortune, C.-A., & O’Kearney, R. (2020). Maternal elaborative language in shared emotion talk with odd children: Relationship to child emotion competencies. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 51(2), 254267. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-019-00927-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlova, M., Mueri, K., Peterson, C., Graham, S. A., & Noel, M. (2022). Mother– and father-child reminiscing about past events involving pain, fear, and sadness: Observational cohort study. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 47(7), 840849. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsac012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilkonis, P. A., Choi, S. W., Reise, S. P., Stover, A. M., Riley, W. T., Cella, D., & PROMIS Cooperative Group (2011). Item banks for measuring emotional distress from the patient reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS®): Depression, anxiety, and anger. Assessment, 18(3), 263283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191111411667 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raikes, H. A., & Thompson, R. A. (2006). Family emotional climate, attachment security and young children’s emotion knowledge in a high-risk sample. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 89104. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151005X70427 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E. (2002). Social factors in the development of autobiographical memory: The state of the art. Social Development, 11(1), 124142. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00190 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Bird, A. L., & Tripp, G. (2007). Children’s self-esteem and moral self: Links to parent-child conversations regarding emotion. Social Development, 16(3), 460478. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00393.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Haden, C. A., & Fivush, R. (1993). Mother-child conversations about the past: Relationships of style and memory over time. Cognitive Development, 8, 403430. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885-2014(05)80002-4 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reese, E., Macfarlane, L., McAnally, H., Robertson, S.-J., & Taumoepeau, M. (2020). Coaching in maternal reminiscing with preschoolers leads to elaborative and coherent personal narratives in early adolescence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 189, 104707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., & Centifanti, L. (2019). Origins of mother-child reminiscing style. Development and Psychopathology, 31(2), 631642. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., & Neha, T. (2015). Let’s kōrero (talk): The practice and functions of reminiscing among mothers and children in Māori families. Memory, 23(1), 99110. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.929705 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., & Newcombe, R. (2007). Training mothers in elaborative reminiscing enhances children’s autobiographical memory and narrative. Child Development, 78(4), 11531170. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01058.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, E., Sparks, A., & Leyva, D. (2010). A review of parent interventions for preschool children’s language and emergent literacy. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10(1), 97117. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798409356987 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, N., Reith, D., Stitely, M., & Smith, A. (2019). The before school check (B4SC): Reporting outcomes and referral rates for all New Zealand children. The New Zealand Medical Journal (Online), 132(1496), 919, https://www.proquest.com/docview/2239533610?pq-origsite=gscholar&fromopenview=true Google ScholarPubMed
Ripley, K., & Yuill, N. (2005). Patterns of language impairment and behavior in boys excluded from school. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(1), 3750. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709905X27696 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, S., Bird, A. L., McNamara, J., & Herbert, J. S. (2023). Exploring the relationship between parent mental health and parent-child emotion reminiscing. Early Child Development and Care, 193(7), 921938. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2023.2174980 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sales, J. M., & Fivush, R. (2005). Social and emotional functions of mother-child reminiscing about stressful events. Social Cognition, 23(1), 7090. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.23.1.70.59196 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, K., O’Kearney, R., Reese, E., & Fortune, C.-A. (2016). The role of language skill in child psychopathology: Implications for intervention in the early years. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 19(4), 352367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-016-0214-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salmon, K., & Reese, E. (2016). The benefits of reminiscing with young children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 233238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmond, C., Crampton, P., & Atkinson, J. (2007). NZDep2006 index of deprivation. In Department of public health. University of Otago. Wellington. https://www.otago.ac.nz/wellington/otago020348.pdf Google Scholar
Semkovska, M., Quinlivan, L., O’Grady, T., Johnson, R., Collins, A., O’Connor, J., Knittle, H., Ahern, E., & Gload, T. (2019). Cognitive function following a major depressive episode: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(10), 851861. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30291-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, D. J. (2020). The developing mind, third edition: how relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are (3rd ed.). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Scaramella, L. V. (2006). Implications of timing of maternal depressive symptoms for early cognitive and language development. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9(1), 6583. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0004-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Splett, J. W., Garzona, M., Gibson, N., Wojtalewicz, D., Raborn, A., & Reinke, W. M. (2019). Teacher recognition, concern, and referral of children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. School Mental Health, 11(2), 228239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12310-018-09303-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (2013). The promise of developmental psychopathology: Past and present. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4pt2), 12151224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000576 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics New Zealand. (2005). Statistical standard for ethnicity. Wellington: Statistics New Zealand. https://aria.stats.govt.nz/aria/ Google Scholar
Suveg, C., Zeman, J., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Cassano, M. (2005). Emotion socialization in families of children with an anxiety disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(2), 145155. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-005-1823-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swearingen, I., Reese, E., Garnett, M., Peterson, E., Salmon, K., Atatoa Carr, P., Morton, S. M. B., & Bird, A. (2023). Maternal reminiscing during middle childhood: Associations with maternal personality and child temperament from the growing up in New Zealand cohort study. Developmental Psychology, 59(12), 22482264. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swetlitz, C., Lynch, S. F., Propper, C. B., Coffman, J. L., & Wagner, N. J. (2021). Examining maternal elaborative reminiscing as a protective factor in the intergenerational transmission of psychopathology. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 49(8), 989999. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00790-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syed, M., & Nelson, S. C. (2015). Guidelines for establishing reliability when coding narrative data. Emerging Adulthood, 3(6), 375387. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167696815587648 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theunissen, G., D’Souza, S., Peterson, E. R., Walker, C., Morton, S. M. B., & Waldie, K. E. (2022). Prenatal determinants of depressive symptoms in childhood: Evidence from growing up in New Zealand. Journal of Affective Disorders, 302, 4149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2022.01.076 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiwari, S., Podell, J. C., Martin, E. D., Mychailyszyn, M. P., Furr, J. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2008). Experiential avoidance in the parenting of anxious youth: Theory, research, and future directions. Cognition & Emotion, 22(3), 480496. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930801886599 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, L., Waldie, K. E., D’Souza, S., Peterson, E. R., & Morton, S. M. B. (2017). A longitudinal study of pre-pregnancy and pregnancy risk factors associated with antenatal and postnatal symptoms of depression: Evidence from growing up in New Zealand. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(4), 915931. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10995-016-2191-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentino, K. (2011). A developmental psychopathology model of overgeneral autobiographical memory. Developmental Review, 31(1), 3254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.05.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, K., Nuttall, A. K., Comas, M., McDonnell, C. G., Piper, B., Thomas, T. E., & Fanuele, S. (2014). Mother-child reminiscing and autobiographical memory specificity among preschool-age children. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 11971207. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034912 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Bergen, P., & Salmon, K. (2010). The association between parent-child reminiscing and children’s emotion knowledge. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 5156.Google Scholar
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., & Dadds, M. R. (2018). Coaching mothers of typical and conduct problem children in elaborative parent-child reminiscing: Influences of a randomized controlled trial on reminiscing behavior and everyday talk preferences. Behavior Research and Therapy, 111, 918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.09.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bergen, P., Salmon, K., Dadds, M. R., & Allen, J. (2009). The effects of mother training in emotion-rich, elaborative reminiscing on children’s shared recall and emotion knowledge. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(3), 162187. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370903155825 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q. (2004). The cultural context of parent-child reminiscing: A functional analysis. In Pratt, M. W. & Fiese, B. (Eds.), Family stories and the life course: Across time and generations. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/341135047 Google Scholar
Wang, Q. (2006). Relations of maternal style and child self-concept to autobiographical memories in Chinese, Chinese immigrant, and European American 3-year-olds. Child Development, 77(6), 17941809. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00974.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q., Doan, S. N., & Song, Q. (2010). Talking about internal states in mother-child reminiscing influences children’s self-representations: A cross-cultural study. Cognitive Development, 25(4), 380393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.08.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Q., & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother–child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European-American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14(3), 473495. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00312.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wareham, P., & Salmon, K. (2006). Mother-child reminiscing about everyday experiences: Implications for psychological interventions in the preschool years. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(5), 535554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2006.05.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, T. E. A., Camia, C., Facompré, C. R., & Fivush, R. (2019). A meta-analytic examination of maternal reminiscing style: Elaboration, gender, and children’s cognitive development. Psychological Bulletin, 145(11), 10821102. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul000021 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxler, E., Thelen, K., & Muzik, M. (2011). Maternal perinatal depression-impact on infant and child development. European Psychiatric Review, 4(1), 4147. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/263442503 Google Scholar
Woodrow, A. (2008). Child development: A social learning theory perspective. Child Care in Practice, 7(2), 130140. https://doi.org/10.1080/13575270108415316 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Conceptual Model Adapted from Goodman and Gotlib (1999).

Figure 1

Table 1. Details of measures included across multiple growing up data collection waves (DCW)

Figure 2

Table 2. Breakdown of participant demographics for current subsample (N = 1234)

Figure 3

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for main study variables

Figure 4

Table 4. Bivariate correlations between study variables

Figure 5

Table 5. Regression model predicting reminiscing variables

Figure 6

Figure 2. Serial mediation model of maternal depression symptoms to child depression symptoms through elaboration and child language skills.Note. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Figure 7

Table 6. Indirect effect coefficients for the serial mediation models of maternal depression on child mental health, mediated through parent-child reminiscing (m1) and child language (m2)

Supplementary material: File

Russell et al. supplementary material

Russell et al. supplementary material
Download Russell et al. supplementary material(File)
File 16.9 KB