Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T00:10:01.699Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Exclusive breastfeeding mitigates the association between prenatal maternal pandemic-related stress and children sleep problems at 24 months of age

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2024

Isabella Lucia Chiara Mariani Wigley
Affiliation:
FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study, Turku Brain and Mind Center, Department of Clinical Medicine, University of Turku, Turku, Finland Centre for Population Health Research, Turku University Hospital and University of Turku, Turku, Finland
Sarah Nazzari*
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
Massimiliano Pastore
Affiliation:
Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua, Padova, Veneto, Italy
Serena Grumi
Affiliation:
Developmental Psychobiology Lab, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
Livio Provenzi
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy Developmental Psychobiology Lab, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy
*
Corresponding author: Sarah Nazzari; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Infant sleep quality is increasingly regarded as an important factor for children long-term functioning and adaptation. The early roots of sleep disturbances are still poorly understood and likely involve a complex interplay between prenatal and postnatal factors. This study investigated whether exclusive breastfeeding during the first 6 months moderated the association between maternal prenatal pandemic-related stress (PRS) and sleep problems in 24-months children born during the COVID-19 pandemic. We also explored the potential contribution of maternal postnatal anxiety in these relations. Seventy-eight infants (50% males) and their mothers provided complete data from birth to 24 months. Between 12 and 48 h from birth, maternal PRS during pregnancy was retrospectively reported as well as maternal anxiety and exclusive breastfeeding. Maternal anxiety and exclusive breastfeeding were also reported at 3 and 6 months after childbirth. Children sleep disturbances were reported at 24 months. Bayesian analyses revealed that maternal PRS was positively associated with sleep problems in children who were not exclusively breastfed from birth to 6 months. Findings add to the growing literature on the lasting impact of early pre- and postnatal experiences on child well-being and development.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Background

Sleep quality plays a crucial role in brain development and adaptation from early in life to adulthood. Children sleep disruptions might increase the risk for a range of adverse outcomes, including cognitive and socio-emotional domains (Astill et al., Reference Astill, Van der Heijden, Van Ijzendoorn and Van Someren2012) as well as undermine academic achievements (Dewald et al., Reference Dewald, Meijer, Oort, Kerkhof and Bögels2010). Noteworthy, sleep disruptions in childhood have been found to predict mental health outcomes later in life, including behavioral and emotional problems in adolescence (Sadeh et al., Reference Sadeh, Tikotzky and Kahn2014). Understanding the factors that underlie the early onset of sleep problems in childhood is critical for developing prevention and intervention strategies that might support healthy sleep patterns and enhance long-term developmental outcomes.

The etiology of sleep disturbances is complex and poorly understood. Besides genetic factors, mounting evidence suggests the role of pre- and postnatal environmental exposures. For example, prenatal exposure to maternal stress might impact infant bio-behavioral regulation, including sleep behaviors (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den, Bea, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020). Likewise, exclusive breastfeeding and postnatal maternal mood are often regarded as non-negligible contributors to the onset of sleep disturbances in the youngests (Galbally et al., Reference Galbally, Lewis, McEgan, Scalzo and Islam2013). However, how prenatal and postnatal environmental exposures act together to shape infant sleep behaviors in humans is still unknown. This study investigates, for the first time, whether exclusive breastfeeding from birth to six months moderates the association between maternal prenatal COVID-19 pandemic-related stress (PRS) exposure and infant sleep regulation at 24 months, taking also into account the potential contribution of maternal postnatal anxiety. Delving into the complex interplay between pre- and postnatal environmental influences of infant sleep quality becomes a paramount endeavor in unraveling the multifaceted determinants of healthy child development.

Prenatal maternal stress and child sleep behaviors

The impact of prenatal exposure to maternal stress on child development is an area of growing interest and research. Maternal stress during pregnancy exceeds the confines of gestation, influencing various aspects of a child’s growth and development (for a review see Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van den, Bea, van den Heuvel, Lahti, Braeken, de Rooij, Entringer, Hoyer, Roseboom, Räikkönen, King and Schwab2020). Amassing evidence indicates that prenatal exposure to maternal stress is associated with cognitive and emotional developmental outcomes in the offspring, such as poorer cognitive development (e.g., Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Fearon, Rice, Ciceri, Molteni and Frigerio2020), altered emotional regulation (e.g., Nolvi et al., Reference Nolvi, Karlsson, Bridgett, Korja, Huizink, Kataja and Karlsson2016), and more “difficult” temperament (e.g., Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Giorda, Bordoni, Pansarasa, Borgatti, Grumi, Mambretti, Villa, Giorda, Bordoni, Pansarasa, Borgatti and Provenzi2023b). Additionally, maternal stress during pregnancy has been shown to be related to a number of biological alterations early in life, encompassing structural (e.g., Buss et al., Reference Buss, Davis, Muftuler, Head and Sandman2010; Lehtola et al., Reference Lehtola, Tuulari, Scheinin, Karlsson, Parkkola, Merisaari, Lewis, Fonov, Louis Collins, Evans, Saunavaara, Hashempour, Lähdesmäki, Acosta and Karlsson2020) and functional (e.g., van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Hect, Smarr, Qawasmeh, Kriegsfeld, Barcelona, Hijazi and Thomason2021) brain alterations, stress regulation (e.g., Davis et al., Reference Davis, Glynn, Waffarn and Sandman2011), immune function (e.g., Veru et al., Reference Veru, Laplante, Luheshi and King2014), and DNA methylation patterns (e.g., Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Mambretti, Villa, Grumi, Citterio, Bertazzoli, Biasucci, Decembrino, Falcone, Gardella, Longo, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Orcesi, Scelsa, Giorda and Borgatti2021; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Cagliero, Grumi, Pisoni, Mallucci, Bergamaschi, Maccarini, Giorda and Provenzi2023a) with possible long-term implications for children health and development (Van Den Bergh et al., Reference Van Den Bergh, Dahnke and Mennes2018). An intriguing area of exploration is its potential effect on the sleep patterns of infants. Higher maternal prenatal stress has been found to be associated with greater sleep difficulties at 3 months (Morales-Muñoz et al., Reference Morales-Muñoz, Saarenpää-Heikkilä, Kylliäinen, Pölkki, Porkka-Heiskanen, Paunio and Paavonen2018), increased sleep disturbances at 18 and 30 months (O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Caprariello, Blackmore, Gregory, Glover and Fleming2007), greater parents’ sleep concerns at 3 years of age and disturbed sleep physiology at 5 years (van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Hect, Smarr, Qawasmeh, Kriegsfeld, Barcelona, Hijazi and Thomason2021). Furthermore, it was recently showed in a large population-based cohort that greater prenatal stressful life events predicted not only higher rates of sleep problems at 1.5 years of age, but also their persistence over time (at 3, 5, 7 and 11 years) (Ksinan Jiskrova et al., Reference Ksinan Jiskrova, Pikhart, Bobák, Klanova and Stepanikova2022). Interestingly, Simcock and colleagues (2019) investigated the prospective association between prenatal exposure to a natural disaster and children’s sleep. They reported more severe objective flood-related hardship, but not maternal subjective stress, to be associated with greater children’s sleep problem scores at 2.5 years of age. However, few studies suggested that prenatal maternal stress may not be associated with sleep characteristics in 4–6 years old children (e.g., Chatterjee et al., Reference Chatterjee, Thompson, Svensson, Tamayo y Ortiz, Wright, Wright, Tellez-Rojo, Baccarelli, Cantoral, Schnaas and Oken2018; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ji, Wang, Li, Leung and Pinto-Martin2020). The lack of consistency in findings across studies may partly be due to methodological heterogeneity in sleep assessments and to an inconsistent consideration of relevant postnatal maternal factors. For example, findings from Morales-Muñoz et al. (Reference Morales-Muñoz, Saarenpää-Heikkilä, Kylliäinen, Pölkki, Porkka-Heiskanen, Paunio and Paavonen2018) were adjusted for breastfeeding, whereas some studies accounted for postnatal maternal mood (e.g., O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Caprariello, Blackmore, Gregory, Glover and Fleming2007; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Hect, Smarr, Qawasmeh, Kriegsfeld, Barcelona, Hijazi and Thomason2021; Simcock et al., Reference Simcock, Cobham, Laplante, Elgbeili, Gruber, Kildea and King2019). In contrast, few studies did not include postnatal confounders (e.g., Chatterjee et al., Reference Chatterjee, Thompson, Svensson, Tamayo y Ortiz, Wright, Wright, Tellez-Rojo, Baccarelli, Cantoral, Schnaas and Oken2018; Ksinan Jiskrova et al., Reference Ksinan Jiskrova, Pikhart, Bobák, Klanova and Stepanikova2022). Thus, further research is clearly needed to explore these relationships with a more comprehensive approach to postnatal factors.

Exclusive breastfeeding: a buffer for child sleep?

Exclusive breastfeeding, defined as feeding an infant solely with breast milk without any other liquids or solids, has well-known multifaceted benefits, acting as a cornerstone of early childhood nutrition and immunological protection (Victora et al., Reference Victora, Bahl, Barros, França, Horton, Krasevec, Murch, Sankar, Walker, Rollins, Allen, Dharmage, Lodge, Peres, Bhandari, Chowdhury, Sinha, Taneja and Giugliani2016). Beyond its advantages in bolstering the infant’s immune system and promoting optimal growth, recent research illuminates a potential link between breastfeeding and enhanced sleep patterns. The rich composition of breast milk, containing a host of bioactive compounds including tryptophan and melatonin precursors, suggests it may function as a natural sedative, potentially augmenting the infant’s capacity for consolidated and restorative sleep (e.g., Engler et al., Reference Engler, Hadash, Shehadeh and Pillar2012). On another account, qualitative research indicated that new mothers often perceive breastfeeding as being associated with more night-waking and poorer sleep quality in infancy, due to the faster digestion of breast milk (Cloherty et al., Reference Cloherty, Alexander and Holloway2004; Rosenbaum et al., Reference Rosenbaum, Gillen and Markey2022). In contrast, they perceive formula feeding as offering more opportunity to rest. As maternal perception of the relationship between breastfeeding and infant sleep might influence the decision to initiate/terminate breastfeeding (Bay et al., Reference Bay, Eksioglu, Soğukpınar and Turfan2023), more empirical evidence is needed. While several studies reported that exclusive breastfeeding is associated with increased sleep disturbances in the first year of life (e.g., Galbally et al., Reference Galbally, Lewis, McEgan, Scalzo and Islam2013; Nakagawa et al., Reference Nakagawa, Ohta, Shimabukuro, Asaka, Nakazawa, Oishi, Hirata, Ando, Ikeda and Yoshimura2021), null associations has also been found (Demirci et al., Reference Demirci, Sereika and Bogen2013; Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto and Field2017a). Furthermore, conflicting evidence exists of longer sleep duration in breastfed infants as compared to formula-fed infants (e.g., Engler et al., Reference Engler, Hadash, Shehadeh and Pillar2012). A recent systematic review concluded that although more fragmented sleep and more waking are reported in breastfed infants, the total sleep duration does not differ between breastfed and formula-fed infants (Manková et al., Reference Manková, Švancarová and Štenclová2023). Interestingly, while few studies reported different results according to the infant’s age (Figueiredo et al., Reference Figueiredo, Dias, Pinto and Field2017; Jafar et al., Reference Jafar, Tham, Pang, Fok, Chua, Teoh, Goh, Shek, Yap and Tan2021; Rudzik et al., Reference Rudzik, Robinson-Smith and Ball2018), more night awakenings and similar or longer night sleep duration in breastfed infants were found in the majority of works regardless of infant’s age (Mankova et al., Reference Manková, Švancarová and Štenclová2023). Noteworthy, beyond the nutritional and hormonal content of breast milk, several psychosocial factors have been hypothesized to underline the association between breastfeeding and infant’s sleep, including sleep environments (Quante et al., Reference Quante, McGee, Yu, von Ash, Luo, Kaplan, Rueschman, Haneuse, Davison and Redline2022), bed arrangements (Mankova et al., Reference Manková, Švancarová and Štenclová2023) or the quality of the dyadic relationship between the mother and child (for a review see Dias & Figueiredo, Reference Dias and Figueiredo2019). For example, a secure attachment has been associated with more regulated sleep-wake behavior, characterized by longer night sleep duration and less night wakings (Pennestri et al., Reference Pennestri, Moss, O’Donnell, Lecompte, Bouvette-Turcot, Atkinson, Minde, Gruber, Fleming and Meaney2015; Zentall et al., Reference Zentall, Braungart-Rieker, Ekas and Lickenbrock2012). Longer duration of breastfeeding, in turn, has been associated with the security of attachment in some studies (reviewed in Linde et al., Reference Linde, Lehnig, Nagl and Kersting2020), thus possibly highlighting an additional pathway for the effects of breastfeeding on sleep behaviors.

As exclusive breastfeeding is often regarded as a protective factor for many domains of child growth and development, an important aspect that is yet to be determined is whether it can act as a buffer for the effect of prenatal stress on infant’s sleep behaviors.

Postnatal maternal anxiety: a risk factor for child sleep?

Alongside, postnatal maternal anxiety is an emerging risk factor for sleep problems in infants (Goldberg et al., Reference Goldberg, Lucas-Thompson, Germo, Keller, Davis and Sandman2013). The pervasive influence of maternal anxiety on the mother-infant dyad has the potential to disrupt the establishment of healthy sleep routines and patterns. Heightened maternal anxiety may result in increased infants’ nocturnal awakenings and fragmented sleep, ultimately compromising the quality and duration of restorative sleep (Tikotzky et al., Reference Tikotzky, Volkovich and Meiri2021). Notwithstanding, the association between maternal postnatal anxiety and infants’ sleep is likely to be bi-directional with insufficient or fragmented sleep negatively affecting maternal mood and potentially increase the risk for maternal postnatal anxiety symptoms (Okun et al., Reference Okun, Mancuso, Hobel, Schetter and Coussons-Read2018). To further complicate the picture, more anxious mothers are more likely to misinterpret their infants’ behaviors, possibly providing biased perceptions of infant sleep behaviors (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Todd-Leonida, Fallon and Silverio2022). This intricate interplay underscores the needs to account for maternal postnatal mood when investigating factors underpinning the early establishment of infant sleep disturbances.

The COVID-19 pandemic: an unprecedented emergency and opportunity

In this context, the recent COVID-19 pandemic, an unparalleled global crisis, provides a framework for examining these complex dynamics. Pregnant women, already navigating the complexities of gestation, found themselves contending with an unprecedented level of uncertainty and stress. The pandemic, with its far-reaching influence on daily life, constitutes a critical period during which prenatal stressors may have been amplified (Tomfohr-Madsen et al., Reference Tomfohr-Madsen, Racine, Giesbrecht, Lebel and Madigan2021), potentially heightening the risk or prevalence of sleep problems in offspring. Early reports suggest that maternal prenatal PRS during pregnancy, understood as the emotional and psychological stress response related to the COVID-19 emergency, is associated with several early infants’ socio-emotional developmental outcomes (Buthmann et al., Reference Buthmann, Miller and Gotlib2022; López-Morales et al., Reference López-Morales, Gelpi Trudo, del-Valle, Canet-Juric, Biota, Andrés and Urquijo2022; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Grumi, Biasucci, Decembrino, Fazzi, Giacchero, Magnani, Nacinovich, Scelsa, Spinillo, Capelli, Roberti and Provenzi2023c, Reference Nazzari, Pili, Günay and Provenzi2024; Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Mambretti, Villa, Grumi, Citterio, Bertazzoli, Biasucci, Decembrino, Falcone, Gardella, Longo, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Orcesi, Scelsa, Giorda and Borgatti2021), including sleep difficulties (Maccarini et al., Reference Maccarini, Nazzari, Grumi and Provenzi2024). To the best of our knowledge, nearly nothing is known for what pertains the possible moderating role of postnatal factors, such as breastfeeding or anxiety, in the observed associations. Understanding the nuanced interplay between maternal prenatal PRS, postnatal influences and infant sleep outcomes is crucial to elucidate the potential intergenerational impact of the pandemic.

The present study

Based on these premises, this study aimed at investigating whether exclusive breastfeeding from birth to 6 months of age may act as a potential moderator of the association between prenatal maternal PRS and the risk of emerging sleep problems in 24-months children born during the COVID-19 healthcare emergency in Italy. Given the extensive changes in sleep development during the first two years of life, including the maturation of sleep-wake rhythms, the development of self-regulation skills, and the reduction in sleep needs, with significant inter-individual variability (e.g., Paavonen et al., Reference Paavonen, Saarenpää-Heikkilä, Morales-Munoz, Virta, Häkälä, Pölkki, Kylliäinen, Karlsson, Paunio and Karlsson2020), we assessed infant sleep behaviors at 24 months of age. This age was chosen to provide a clearer picture of infants who establish patterns of sleep difficulties. Due to the constraints imposed by the pandemic, maternal stress experiences related to the pandemic during pregnancy were reported retrospectively soon after childbirth (i.e., between 12 and 48 hr postpartum), following previous studies (e.g., Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Mambretti, Villa, Grumi, Citterio, Bertazzoli, Biasucci, Decembrino, Falcone, Gardella, Longo, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Orcesi, Scelsa, Giorda and Borgatti2021). Continuous exclusive breastfeeding, defined as no other forms of food or liquids being introduced, was assessed from birth to 6 months of age, in line with World Health Organization recommendations (WHO, 2021). In line with emerging literature on the buffering role of the postnatal environment in the association between maternal stress and infant outcomes (Grande et al., Reference Grande, Swales, Sandman, Glynn and Davis2021; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Fearon, Rice, Molteni and Frigerio2022a), we tested both the independent and interactive effects of maternal PRS and exclusive breastfeeding on infant sleep disturbances. We hypothesized that continuous exclusive breastfeeding at 6 months of age would buffer the magnitude of the association between maternal PRS and infant’s sleep problems, reducing the effect of prenatal stress exposure in breastfed infants. In contrast, we anticipated greater sleep problems to occur in infants prenatally exposed to greater maternal stress and not exclusively breastfed in the first 6 months of life. Additionally, as maternal anxiety might further confound the observed associations, we further explored whether maternal postnatal anxiety would also modify the relations between prenatal maternal PRS, exclusive breastfeeding, and children sleep problems. To this aim, maternal postnatal anxiety was assessed soon after childbirth, at 3 and 6 months of age to track the trajectory of maternal mental health across the early perinatal period. A three-way interaction among maternal PRS, breastfeeding and postnatal anxiety was included in our exploratory model. Due to the exploratory nature of this latter analysis, no specific a-priori hypotheses were made.

Methods

Participants and procedures

Mother-infant dyads were recruited from May 2020 to February 2021 in ten neonatal units in Northern Italy as an extended follow-up of a longitudinal multi-centric research project, the MOM-COPE Study (for full details see Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Grumi, Giorda, Biasucci, Bonini, Cavallini, Decembrino, Drera, Falcone, Fazzi, Gardella, Giacchero, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Scelsa, Spartà, Veggiotti, Orcesi and Borgatti2020). Inclusion criteria were: maternal age over 18 years, absence of prenatal and perinatal diseases or injuries, and term delivery (i.e., from 37 + 0 to 41 + 6 weeks of gestation). As the data collection occurred during the COVID-19 health emergency, a negative PCR test for SARS-CoV-2 at delivery was an additional inclusion criterion. All infants (50% males, mostly White) were born full-term (37–42 weeks’ gestation) and had normal birth weight (> 2500 g). Most women were well-educated (averaged years of education = 15.79, SD = 2.9) and employed at the time of the data collection. For more information on sample characteristics see Table 1. Within 48 hr from infant birth, mother-infant dyads were recruited from a diverse urban environment including the metropolitan and suburban areas of major cities of Northern Italy such as Pavia, Lodi, Piacenza, Brescia, Milano and Monza. Socio-demographic (i.e., age, educational level, occupational status) and neonatal (i.e., gestational age, birth weight, head circumference, length, Apgar scores, breastfeeding at birth, and mode of delivery) data were obtained from medical records. Between 12 and 48 h after childbirth (T0), women retrospectively reported on their PRS experience during pregnancy, as well as on their current anxiety levels and breastfeeding practices. Maternal anxiety and breastfeeding were also assessed at 3 (T1) and 6 (T2) months after childbirth. Infants’ sleep disturbances were reported 24 months after childbirth (T3). An initial sample of 320 mother-infant dyads were recruited at birth. From this sample, 220 dyads (around 69%) completed the T1 assessment, whereas 85 mothers (around 26,5%) provided data at the 24-months follow-up assessment (T3). Given the presence of missing-data for some of the study measures in the T3 sample, we decided to use a listwise deletion approach (i.e., dropping observations in the dataset for which the values of at least one variable are missing) resulting in a final analytic sample of 78 participants. While sample attrition at the postnatal follow-up phases was a significant issue, it is noteworthy that we did not observe any meaningful difference between participants and those who withdrew from the postnatal phases in demographic, PRS, anxiety and depression scores, suggesting no systematic patterns of attrition (Section 3 in Supplementary Material).

Table 1. Descriptives of the included subjects

The study was approved by the Ethics Committees of the IRCCS Mondino Foundation and the participating hospitals. All the procedures were performed in accordance with the 2018 Declaration of Helsinki for studies conducted with human participants. All parents provided written informed consent to participate to the study.

Measures

Maternal PRS. At delivery (T0) mothers retrospectively reported on their PRS during pregnancy through an ad hoc questionnaire (Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Grumi, Giorda, Biasucci, Bonini, Cavallini, Decembrino, Drera, Falcone, Fazzi, Gardella, Giacchero, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Scelsa, Spartà, Veggiotti, Orcesi and Borgatti2020; Section 2 in Supplementary Material). The questionnaire included six 5-point Likert scale items (1, not at all; 5, very much) on the emotional stress response to the COVID-19 emergency during pregnancy. An average maternal antenatal PRS score was obtained, ranging from 1 (low) to 5 (high). The Cronbach’s α of this scale was .88, 95% CI [0.62, 0.98].

Maternal postnatal anxiety. The 20-item Italian version of the State anxiety subscale (STAI-S) of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Pedrabissi & Santinello, Reference Pedrabissi and Santinello1989) was employed to assess maternal postnatal anxiety symptoms at delivery (T0), 3 (T1) and 6 (T2) months postpartum. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale with the total continuous score ranging from 20 (low) to 80 (high). The Cronbach’s α of this scale was .98, 95% CI [0.96, 0.99] at T0 and T1 and .97, 95% CI [0.94, 0.99] at T2.

Exclusive breastfeeding. Mothers were asked to complete an ad hoc form to assess whether they were using exclusive breastfeeding, maternal milk from a bottle, formula or mixed feeding methods at delivery (T0), 3 (T1) and 6 (T2) months of infants’ age.

Children sleep behavior. Mothers filled in the Italian adaptation of the 33 items Children’s Sleep Habits Questionnaire (Borrelli et al., Reference Borrelli, Scala, Festa, Bruzzese, Michelotti, Cantone, Corcione, Fragnito, Miranda and Santamaria2021) at 24 months after childbirth (T3). The frequency of sleep-related behaviors in the last previous week was rated on a 3-point scale as “usually” (5 to 7 times per week, scored as 3 points), “sometimes” (2 to 4 times per week, scored as 2 points) or “rarely” (0 to 1 time per week, scored as 1 point). The total Sleep Disturbance Scale results from the sum of the responses obtained on each item, with higher scores indicating greater sleep disturbances. The Cronbach’s α of this scale was .77, 95% CI [0.63, 0.87].

Data analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R software (R Core Team, 2018) including brms (Bürkner, Reference Bürkner2017) and ggplot2 (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016) packages, and using STAN to implement MCMC sampling (Stan Development Team, 2018). In preliminary analyses, we explored data distribution. Then, we computed descriptive statistics (Table 1) and bivariate correlations among our study variables (Figure S4 and S5). We subsequently evaluated our hypothesis by fitting and comparing a series of linear models (Table 2) using a full Bayesian approach for estimating parameters. Model comparison allows for the selection of the most plausible models given the data and a set of candidate models. Specifically, we compared the following models: model 0 (M00), i.e., a model assuming that there were no associations among the study variables; model 1 (M01), including only exclusive breastfeeding as predictor; model 2 (M02), including only maternal PRS; model 3 (M03), including both exclusive breastfeeding and maternal PRS main effects and, lastly, model 4 (M04), testing the interactive effect between maternal PRS and exclusive breastfeeding. Models were compared in terms of statistical evidence (i.e. support by the data) using information criteria, which enables the evaluation of models considering the tradeoff between parsimony and goodness-of-fit. Here, models were compared using the following criteria: Bayesian R2 (Gelman et al., Reference Gelman, Goodrich, Gabry and Vehtari2019), Leave-one-out Cross Validation information criterion (Vehtari et al., Reference Vehtari, Gelman and Gabry2017), where models with lower values indicate higher predictive capability, and model weights, with high values indicating a better model. Considering that the sample size is relatively small, we used the following weak informative priors (for prior specifications see Section 5 Supplementary Materials): for exclusive breastfeeding effect Student’s t (3, –5, 5), for maternal PRS effect Student’s t (3, 0, 1), and for interaction effect between the two predictors Student’s t (3, –2, 3). For all models, estimates were based on 4000 samples extracted from the posteriors with 4 chains. MCMC convergences were assessed by means of the Potential Scale Reduction Statistic (PSRF or Rhat; Gelman & Rubin, Reference Gelman and Rubin1992). We also checked model diagnostics for residuals. Once identified, the best model was analyzed in detail. We analyzed the model predictions based on the parameters’ posterior distributions and the Posterior Predictive Distribution (i.e. the distribution of possible unobserved values conditional on observed data and model parameters, PPD). In order to obtain further information on the extent to which infants’ sleep problems may differ in relation to the interaction between maternal PRS and breastfeeding, we computed the degree of overlap of the posterior distribution of parameters (eta; Pastore & Calcagnì, Reference Pastore and Calcagnì2019). The eta index measures the degree of overlap between two empirical densities and ranges between 0 (when the distributions are completely disjoint) and 1 (when the distributions completely overlap). Values within this range quantify the similarity/difference of the values in the two groups (no-exclusive breastfeeding, exclusive breastfeeding). Finally, we addressed our exploratory research question by including maternal anxiety factor score (i.e., estimated values for maternal anxiety assessed at 0, 3 and 6 months of infant age) in our statistical model. Again, to obtain information on the extent to which infants’ sleep problems may differ in relation to the interaction between maternal PRS, breastfeeding and anxiety, we computed the degree of overlap of the posterior distribution of parameters. In this case, the eta index allows to quantify the similarity/difference of the values in the two groups concerning maternal anxiety level.

Table 2. Linear models definition

Note. Infants’ sleep problems: infants’ sleep problems assessed at 24 months of age, breastfeeding groups: exclusive breastfed vs. no- exclusive breastfed infants; maternal PRS: maternal pandemic-related stress.

Results

Preliminary analysis

In Table 1 are reported the univariate statistics for the quantitative study variables. Exclusive breastfeeding was reported by 65.4% of the sample at T0, 55.1% at T1, and 48.7% at T2. At six months 38.5% of the sample had continuative exclusive breastfeeding since birth, while 61.5% had discontinued.

Figure S4 represents bivariate associations between variables of interest while Figure S5 represents distributions and bivariate associations between participants descriptives (i.e., the descriptives reported in Table 1) in exclusive breastfeeding groups.

Main analyses

Table 3 shows the results of the Bayesian model comparison. The best performing model was model M04 (i.e., the model with the interaction between exclusive breastfeeding and maternal PRS). Importantly, continuous exclusive breastfeeding from birth up to 6 months of age interacted with maternal PRS in predicting infants sleep problems at 24 months (B = 2.46, with a 90% Highest posterior density interval (HPDI) [0.35; 4.54], corresponding to an η2 of 0.03 with a 90% HPDI [.00; .10]).

Table 3. Model comparison

Note. LOO, leave-one-out cross-validation information criterion; SE, standard error; W, model weight; R2, Bayesian R2. M00 is the null model, M01 is the model with breastfeeding group comparison, M02 is the model with the effect of maternal PRS, M03 is the model with the additive effect of breastfeeding groups and PRS, M04 is the model with the interaction effect between breastfeeding groups and PRS.

Specifically, as illustrated in Figure 1 [B], infants who do not received continuous exclusive breastfeeding from birth to 6 months showed higher sleep problems at 24 months of age at higher (+1SD) levels of maternal PRS. Figure 2 represents the overlapping of PPD of the expected values of infants’ sleep problems assessed at 24 months of age as a function of maternal PRS (x-axis) and exclusive breastfeeding (colors). Between non-continuosly breastfed and continuosly breastfed infants, those with mothers with lower (1) PRS scores showed a very small overlap (0.05), indicating a large difference in means, while those with PRS scores equal to 3 showed a higher overlap (0.84). When mothers reported the maximum level of PRS (5), the overlapping index between breastfed and non-breastfed infants was very small (0.16), indicating a large difference in means.

Figure 1. Model M04 posterior predictive check [a] and expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal PRS assessed at T0 (x-axis) and exclusive breastfeeding groups [b].

Figure 2. Overlapping index for M04. Expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis) and exclusive breastfeeding groups represented in yellow and green respectively.

Exploratory analysis

To address our exploratory research question concerning the potential role of maternal anxiety in the observed associations, we included maternal anxiety factor score to our best model identified from the analyses described above. We computed 2 different levels of maternal anxiety corresponding to its mean ± the standard deviation (i.e., –7.58, 7.58). For each maternal anxiety level * maternal PRS * group (breastfed vs non-breastfed) interaction, we sampled 4000 posterior draws of the expected values from PPD of the model (Figure 3 [A]). We represented the obtained posteriors in Figure 4 and for each pair (–7.58 vs 7.58 and breastfed vs. non-breastfed) we computed the eta. Figure 3 [A] represents the posterior predictive check while Figure 3 [B] represents the expected values of the model testing the triple interaction between group (i.e., breastfed vs. non-breastfed), maternal PRS and maternal anxiety. Figure 4 represents the PPD of the expected values in interaction model as a function of maternal PRS (x-axis), breastfeeding groups (panels), and maternal anxiety (colors). Among breastfed infants, those with mothers with lower (1) PRS scores showed an high overlap (0.84), while those with higher (5) PRS showed a small overlap (0.24), indicating a large difference in means. Among non-breastfed infants, those with lower (1) PRS showed a small overlap (0.34) which becomes lower and lower until it is equal to 0.19 for those with higher (5) PRS scores. This highlight that the effect of maternal anxiety (i.e., f1) on the association between PRS and infants’ sleep problems at 24 months of age seems to impact similarly in breastfed and non-breastfed infants. It should also be noted that the triple interaction is rather weak and poorly supported by the data.

Figure 3. Triple interaction model posterior predictive check [a] and expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis), breastfeeding groups and different levels of maternal anxiety [b].

Figure 4. Triple interaction model overlapping index. Expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis), exclusive breastfeeding groups (panels) and maternal anxiety level (colours).

Discussion

With amassing evidence of an association between maternal prenatal stress and infants’ developmental outcomes (Van den Bergh et al., Reference Van Den Bergh, Dahnke and Mennes2020), there is now a growing focus on identifying relevant postnatal factors that might buffer these effects. One potential candidate is breastfeeding, considering its beneficial effects that extend beyond maternal and infant health, encompassing maternal sensitivity and mother-infant bonding (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Feldman, Mayes, Eicher, Thompson, Leckman and Swain2011; Tharner et al., Reference Tharner, Luijk, Raat, IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Moll, Jaddoe, Hofman, Verhulst and Tiemeier2012). The current study provides novel evidence suggesting that exclusive breastfeeding might buffer the association between antenatal exposure to maternal PRS and sleep disturbances in infants born during the COVID-19 pandemic. Specifically, in line with our broad hypothesis, higher levels of maternal PRS during pregnancy were associated with greater children’s sleep problems at 24 months of age only among infants that were not exclusively continuously breastfed until 6 months of age, while the association became non-significant among infants that were exclusively continuously breastfed until 6 months of age. Interestingly, exploratory analyses indicate that maternal postnatal anxiety might not represent a substantial modifier of these effects.

The COVID-19 pandemic, along with the related restriction measures, has been an extraordinary and highly stressful situation for many families on a global scale, with a carry-over negative impact on people’s mental well-being, particularly in vulnerable groups like expectant mothers (Tomfohr-Madsen et al., Reference Tomfohr-Madsen, Racine, Giesbrecht, Lebel and Madigan2021). There is a growing literature showing that maternal stress experience related to the pandemic (i.e., PRS) might have significant implications for both maternal and fetal outcomes. Elevations in maternal PRS during pregnancy have been found to be associated with early infant temperament (e.g., Buthmann et al., Reference Buthmann, Miller and Gotlib2022; López-Morales et al., Reference López-Morales, Gelpi Trudo, del-Valle, Canet-Juric, Biota, Andrés and Urquijo2022), socio-cognitive outcomes (Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Grumi, Biasucci, Decembrino, Fazzi, Giacchero, Magnani, Nacinovich, Scelsa, Spinillo, Capelli, Roberti and Provenzi2023c) and sleep problems (Maccarini et al., Reference Maccarini, Nazzari, Grumi and Provenzi2024). Furthermore, prenatal exposure to the lockdown has been linked with patterns of DNA methylation of gene involved in stress regulations in the newborns (Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Grumi, Mambretti, Villa, Giorda, Provenzi, Borgatti, Biasucci, Decembrino, Giacchero, Magnani, Nacinovich, Prefumo, Spinillo and Veggiotti2022b, Reference Nazzari, Cagliero, Grumi, Pisoni, Mallucci, Bergamaschi, Maccarini, Giorda and Provenzi2023a; Provenzi et al., Reference Provenzi, Mambretti, Villa, Grumi, Citterio, Bertazzoli, Biasucci, Decembrino, Falcone, Gardella, Longo, Nacinovich, Pisoni, Prefumo, Orcesi, Scelsa, Giorda and Borgatti2021), thus suggesting that maternal prenatal PRS might have a broad impact on infant’s bio-behavioral development. The current findings critically extend available literature by showing that continuous exclusive breastfeeding from birth to 6 months of age might be able to mitigate some of these effects. These results align with mounting evidence showing that the association between maternal prenatal stress and infants’ bio-behavioral outcomes might be at least partly over-ridden by the quality of the postnatal rearing environment (Frigerio & Nazzari, Reference Frigerio and Nazzari2021; Nazzari et al., Reference Nazzari, Fearon, Rice, Molteni and Frigerio2022a), raising important conceptual and clinical implications.

It is acknowledged that an altered prenatal environment can have enduring impacts on the functioning of physiological systems, including the regulation of circadian rhythms (Palagini et al., Reference Palagini, Drake, Gehrman, Meerlo and Riemann2015). For example, prenatal exposure to stress-related elevations in maternal cortisol might alter the development of fetal stress response systems leading to disturbances in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis functioning (Nazzari et al., Reference Maccarini, Nazzari, Grumi and Provenzi2019), such as a hyper-responsivity of the HPA axis, which in turn have been associated with several sleep problems, such as chronic insomnia (Bonnet & Arand, Reference Bonnet and Arand2010). Interestingly, maternal emotional distress during pregnancy have been found to be associated with fetal activity and sleep patterns. For example, foetuses of depressed women spent a greater percentage of time being active, as compared to the offspring of non-depressed women (Dieter et al., Reference Dieter, Emory, Johnson and Raynor2008). In contrast, fetuses of anxious mothers were found to spend more time in “quiet sleep” and to be less active in “active sleep” than fetuses of mothers without high anxiety (Groome et al., Reference Groome, Swiber, Bentz, Holland and Atterbury1995). Furthermore, growing research is showing an association between maternal prenatal stress and altered fetal brain activity in areas that might be involved in sleep regulation (e.g., van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Hect, Smarr, Qawasmeh, Kriegsfeld, Barcelona, Hijazi and Thomason2021), possibly suggesting a mediating role of these alterations in the onset of early sleep problems. While the mechanisms underlying the association between maternal prenatal stress and infant’s sleep disturbances required further investigation, the current work suggests that exclusive breastfeeding can reverse these effects. Specifically, we showed that the association between maternal PRS and 24-month sleep problems was not significant in dyads that reported continuous exclusive breastfeeding until 6 months of life.

Exclusive breastfeeding is widely acknowledged as an important bio-behavioral protective factor, exerting widespread beneficial effects on infant health and development (Victora et al., Reference Victora, Bahl, Barros, França, Horton, Krasevec, Murch, Sankar, Walker, Rollins, Allen, Dharmage, Lodge, Peres, Bhandari, Chowdhury, Sinha, Taneja and Giugliani2016). The current findings align with initial human’s evidence showing that exclusive breastfeeding can buffer the association between maternal prenatal or postnatal distress and infants’ outcomes. For example, Miller-Graff & Scheid (Reference Miller-Graff and Scheid2020) recently showed that breastfeeding mitigated the effects of prenatal exposure to intimate partner violence on infant temperament. Likewise, the typical frontal asymmetry patterns often found in infants of depressed mothers has not been observed in infants of depressed mothers who breastfed (Jones et al., Reference Jones, McFall and Diego2004). Biological and/or psychosocial mechanisms might underline these effects. Maternal milk has important nutritional properties, including high levels of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, which are known to directly impact on neural and cognitive development in offspring (Larque et al., Reference Larque, Demmelmair and Koletzko2002). In addition, maternal milk contains a broad range of biologically active hormones, such as glucocorticoids, which might provide important biochemical signals to the offspring about the quality of the postnatal environment. Interestingly, a positive association has been reported between milk cortisol levels and an enhanced performance on the Autonomic Stability cluster of the Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale in one-week-old newborns, indicating a better homeostatic adjustment of the central nervous system (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Boylan, Border, Carroll, McGunegle and Lampe2004). Furthermore, aspects of maternal microbiome might be transmitted through breastmilk and further influence infant’s health and development (Kordy et al., Reference Kordy, Gaufin, Mwangi, Li, Cerini, Lee, Adisetiyo, Woodward, Pannaraj and Tobin2020). Besides purely biological factors, breastfeeding has been found to have a beneficial impact on maternal mood, sensitivity and mother-infant bonding (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Feldman, Mayes, Eicher, Thompson, Leckman and Swain2011; Tharner et al., Reference Tharner, Luijk, Raat, IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Moll, Jaddoe, Hofman, Verhulst and Tiemeier2012), promoting close physical contact and emotional connection. In this light, the moderating effects of maternal breastfeeding on the association between maternal PRS during pregnancy and children sleep might be mediated by maternal postnatal behaviors, such as greater maternal sensitivity. However, this hypothesis needs to be explicitly addressed in future studies including observative direct measure of the quality of maternal caregiving. While the precise mechanisms of the observed effects deserve further investigation, the current results underscore the holistic benefits of breastfeeding, which not only provide essential nutrients but also extend to positively influencing the sleep architecture of infants. Recognizing the potential role of exclusive breastfeeding in mitigating the intergenerational transmission of stress-related disturbances provide further empirical leverage for supporting and promoting this practice as part of pre- and postnatal care systems. Importantly, current findings suggest that extra efforts to promote breastfeeding should be directed toward mothers experiencing stressful situations or higher levels of distress across pregnancy as this may be especially beneficial in promoting long-term outcomes in the offspring.

Lastly, current findings suggest that the observed associations between maternal PRS, exclusive breastfeeding and children sleep are not substantially modified by maternal postnatal anxiety. It is important to acknowledge that the limited sample size might have reduced the chance to observe a significant 3-way interaction in the current sample, thus further replication in larger samples is required. Nevertheless, the lack of a substantial effect of maternal postnatal anxiety, if true, suggests that the observed effect of breastfeeding on the association between maternal prenatal stress and infant sleep is not confounded by postnatal maternal mood so that it is fairly comparable among mothers with high versus low levels of anxiety across the first months of life. Maternal anxiety can negative impact breastfeeding duration (Fallon et al., Reference Fallon, Groves, Halford, Bennett and Harrold2016) and is bidirectionally linked with children’s sleep, with greater anxiety either influencing or resulting from early difficulties in children’s establishment of healthy sleep patterns (Goldberg et al., Reference Goldberg, Lucas-Thompson, Germo, Keller, Davis and Sandman2013; Okun et al., Reference Okun, Mancuso, Hobel, Schetter and Coussons-Read2018). Furthermore, maternal anxiety might influence the accurate reporting of children sleep behaviors (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Todd-Leonida, Fallon and Silverio2022). Although caution is needed in interpreting the current results due to the limited statistical power, our exploratory analyses seem to further corroborate the current findings showing that the observed interaction between maternal PRS during pregnancy and breastfeeding in influencing children sleep problems is not significantly modified by maternal postnatal mood.

Some limitations are noteworthy. First, results are based on a small middle-high SES community sample of healthy women and infants, and assessments were made during an extraordinary stressful period, thus limiting generalizability to different time and high-risk populations. Secondly, the longitudinal and observational nature of the project, especially during the pandemic time, resulted in remarkable sample attrition. Nevertheless, potential systematic patterns of missingness were not detected in the current study. Third, we selected specific time points for assessing infant sleep, exclusive breastfeeding and maternal stress, which may not fully capture the dynamic changes and variability in these parameters over the early years of life. While these time points were chosen based on theoretical and practical considerations, including constraints imposed by the pandemic, this may limit the generalizability of the findings to other developmental periods and contexts. Fourth, the prenatal stress questionnaire was developed ad hoc for this study, prioritizing sensitivity to the specific and unprecedented nature of COVID-19 emergency over measure standardization. Lastly, the reliance on maternal reports for measures of maternal PRS, breastfeeding status and infant sleep produces shared method variance and potentially leads to an overestimation of the effects of maternal predictors on infant sleep outcomes. Further replication of these findings is needed using more objective and structured assessment of infant sleep problems. Future work would also benefit from the inclusion of direct measures of breastmilk cortisol as well as of maternal sensitivity to elucidate the biological and/or relational mechanisms that might underlying the buffering effect of breastfeeding. Lastly, although findings of a prospective association between maternal prenatal stress, breastfeeding until 6 months of age and offspring sleep at 24 months were in the expected direction and it is tempting to interpret them as suggestive of causative pathways, they are based on correlational analyses and no causal conclusions should be drawn.

Conclusions

The present study suggests that continuous exclusive breastfeeding from birth to 6 months of age might be able to reverse the effects of prenatal exposure to maternal PRS on children sleep problems at 24 months in a sample of infants born during the pandemic. Around 40% of mothers reported continuous exclusive breastfeeding from birth to 6 months in the current sample, generally in line with the global trend observed in high-income countries (Cai et al., Reference Cai, Wardlaw and Brown2012). As empirical support accumulates for a critical protective role of breastfeeding in promoting infant and children’s developmental trajectories, a call for action is needed to enhance the rates of exclusive breastfeeding during the first 6 months of age. Going beyond the well-known “breast is best” motto, this overarching goal need to be accomplished by spreading a nurturing culture that prioritize the mother-infant dyad well-being. Instead of leaving women who do not wish or are unable to breastfeed navigating the stigma and guilt, new mothers should be empowered to make informed choices and receive the necessary physical, psychological, social and economic support. Together these factors may not only increase the duration of breastfeeding, but ultimately strengthen mother-infant relationship, setting the stage for healthier generations to come.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001627.

References

Astill, R. G., Van der Heijden, K. B., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Van Someren, E. J. W. (2012). Sleep, cognition, and behavioral problems in school-age children: A century of research meta-analyzed. Psychological Bulletin, 138(6), 11091138. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028204 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bay, H., Eksioglu, A., Soğukpınar, N., & Turfan, E. C. (2023). The effect of postpartum sleep quality on mothers’ breastfeeding self-efficacy level. Early Child Development and Care, 193(2), 235246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnet, M. H., & Arand, D. L. (2010). Hyperarousal and insomnia: State of the science. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14(1), 915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borrelli, M., Scala, I., Festa, P., Bruzzese, D., Michelotti, A., Cantone, E., Corcione, A., Fragnito, M., Miranda, V., & Santamaria, F. (2021). Linguistic adaptation and psychometric evaluation of Italian version of children’s sleep habits questionnaire. Italian Journal of Pediatrics, 47(1), 15.Google ScholarPubMed
Bürkner, P.-C. (2017). Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R package brms. ArXiv Preprint ArXiv:1705.11123.Google Scholar
Buss, C., Davis, E. P., Muftuler, L. T., Head, K., & Sandman, C. A. (2010). High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6-9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(1), 141153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buthmann, J. L., Miller, J. G., & Gotlib, I. H. (2022). Maternal-prenatal stress and depression predict infant temperament during the COVID-19 pandemic. Development and Psychopathology, 36, 19. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579422001055 Google ScholarPubMed
Cai, X., Wardlaw, T., & Brown, D. W. (2012). Global trends in exclusive breastfeeding. International Breastfeeding Journal, 7(1), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chatterjee, A., Thompson, J. W., Svensson, K., Tamayo y Ortiz, M., Wright, R., Wright, R., Tellez-Rojo, M., Baccarelli, A., Cantoral, A., Schnaas, L., & Oken, E. (2018). Maternal antenatal stress has little impact on child sleep: Results from a prebirth cohort in Mexico city. Sleep Health, 4(5), 397404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.07.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloherty, M., Alexander, J., & Holloway, I. (2004). Supplementing breast-fed babies in the UK to protect their mothers from tiredness or distress. Midwifery, 20(2), 194204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, S. M., Todd-Leonida, B. F., Fallon, V. M., & Silverio, S. A. (2022). Exclusive breastfeeding duration and perceptions of infant sleep: The mediating role of postpartum anxiety. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(8), 4494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, E. P., Glynn, L. M., Waffarn, F., & Sandman, C. A. (2011). Prenatal maternal stress programs infant stress regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 52(2), 119129. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02314.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demirci, J. R., Sereika, S. M., & Bogen, D. (2013). Prevalence and predictors of early breastfeeding among late preterm mother-infant dyads. Breastfeeding Medicine, 8(3), 277285. https://doi.org/10.1089/bfm.2012.0075 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewald, J. F., Meijer, A. M., Oort, F. J., Kerkhof, G. A., & Bögels, S. M. (2010). The influence of sleep quality, sleep duration and sleepiness on school performance in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 14(3), 179189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2009.10.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dias, C. C., & Figueiredo, B. (2019). Sleep-wake behaviour during the first 12 months of life and associated factors: A systematic review. Early Child Development and Care, 190, 23332365. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2019.1582034 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dieter, J. N. I., Emory, E. K., Johnson, K. C., & Raynor, B. D. (2008). Maternal depression and anxiety effects on the human fetus: Preliminary findings and clinical implications. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 29(5), 420441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engler, A. C., Hadash, A., Shehadeh, N., & Pillar, G. (2012). Breastfeeding may improve nocturnal sleep and reduce infantile colic: Potential role of breast milk melatonin. European Journal of Pediatrics, 171(4), 729732. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-011-1659-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fallon, V., Groves, R., Halford, J. C. G., Bennett, K. M., & Harrold, J. A. (2016). Postpartum anxiety and infant-feeding outcomes: A systematic review. Journal of Human Lactation, 32(4), 740758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figueiredo, B., Dias, C. C., Pinto, T. M., & Field, T. (2017a). Exclusive breastfeeding at three months and infant sleep-wake behaviors at two weeks, three and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 6269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.06.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frigerio, A., & Nazzari, S. (2021). Antenatal maternal anxiety, maternal sensitivity and toddlers’ behavioral problems: An investigation of possible pathways. Early Human Development, 157,105364. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2021.105364 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galbally, M., Lewis, A. J., McEgan, K., Scalzo, K., & Islam, F. A. (2013). Breastfeeding and infant sleep patterns: An Australian population study. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 49(2), 147152. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpc.12089 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, A., Goodrich, B., Gabry, J., & Vehtari, A. (2019). R-squared for Bayesian regression models. The American Statistician, 73(3), 307309. https://doi.org/10.1080/00031305.2018.1549100 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science, 7(4), 457472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, W. A., Lucas-Thompson, R. G., Germo, G. R., Keller, M. A., Davis, E. P., & Sandman, C. A. (2013). Eye of the beholder? Maternal mental health and the quality of infant sleep. Social Science and Medicine, 79(1), 101108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2012.07.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grande, L. A., Swales, D. A., Sandman, C. A., Glynn, L. M., & Davis, E. P. (2021). Maternal caregiving ameliorates the consequences of prenatal maternal psychological distress on child development. Development and Psychopathology, 34(4), 13761385. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000286 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groome, L. J., Swiber, M. J., Bentz, L. S., Holland, S. B., & Atterbury, J. L. (1995). Maternal anxiety during pregnancy: Effect on fetal behavior at 38 to 40 weeks of gestation. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 16(6), 391396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, S., Boylan, L. M., Border, B., Carroll, S. R., McGunegle, D., & Lampe, R. M. (2004). Breast milk levels of cortisol and Secretory Immunoglobulin A (SIgA) differ with maternal mood and infant neuro-behavioral functioning. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(1), 101106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.06.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jafar, N. K. A., Tham, E. K. H., Pang, W. W., Fok, D., Chua, M. C., Teoh, O.-H., Goh, D. Y. T., Shek, L. P. C., Yap, F., & Tan, K. H. (2021). Association between breastfeeding and sleep patterns in infants and preschool children. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 114(6), 19861996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, N. A., McFall, B. A., & Diego, M. A. (2004). Patterns of brain electrical activity in infants of depressed mothers who breastfeed and bottle feed: The mediating role of infant temperament. Biological Psychology, 67(1-2), 103124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, P., Feldman, R., Mayes, L. C., Eicher, V., Thompson, N., Leckman, J. F., & Swain, J. E. (2011). Breastfeeding, brain activation to own infant cry, and maternal sensitivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(8), 907915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kordy, K., Gaufin, T., Mwangi, M., Li, F., Cerini, C., Lee, D. J., Adisetiyo, H., Woodward, C., Pannaraj, P. S., & Tobin, N. H. (2020). Contributions to human breast milk microbiome and enteromammary transfer of Bifidobacterium breve . PLOS ONE, 15(1), e0219633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ksinan Jiskrova, G., Pikhart, H., Bobák, M., Klanova, J., & Stepanikova, I. (2022). Prenatal psychosocial stress and children’s sleep problems: Evidence from the ELSPAC-CZ study. Journal of Sleep Research, 31(4), 110. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13531 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larque, E., Demmelmair, H., & Koletzko, B. (2002). Perinatal supply and metabolism of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: Importance for the early development of the nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 967(1), 299310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehtola, S. J., Tuulari, J. J., Scheinin, N. M., Karlsson, L., Parkkola, R., Merisaari, H., Lewis, J. D., Fonov, V. S., Louis Collins, D., Evans, A., Saunavaara, J., Hashempour, N., Lähdesmäki, T., Acosta, H., & Karlsson, H. (2020). Newborn amygdalar volumes are associated with maternal prenatal psychological distress in a sex-dependent way. NeuroImage: Clinical, 28, 102380. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102380.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linde, K., Lehnig, F., Nagl, M., & Kersting, A. (2020). The association between breastfeeding and attachment: A systematic review. Midwifery, 81, 102592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Ji, X., Wang, G., Li, Y., Leung, P. W., & Pinto-Martin, J. (2020). Maternal emotions during the pre/postnatal periods and children’s sleep behaviors: The mediating role of children’s behavior. Journal of Affective Disorders, 273, 138145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.178 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López-Morales, H., Gelpi Trudo, R., del-Valle, M. V., Canet-Juric, L., Biota, M., Andrés, M. L., & Urquijo, S. (2022). The pandemial babies: Effects of maternal stress on temperament of babies gestated and born during the pandemic. Current Psychology, 43(16), 1488114893. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03976-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccarini, J., Nazzari, S., Grumi, S., Provenzi, L., & MOM-COPE Study Group. (2024). Prenatal maternal pandemic-related stress was associated with a greater risk of children having disturbed sleep at 24 months of age. Acta Paediatrica, 113(2), 256258. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.17042 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manková, D., Švancarová, S., & Štenclová, E. (2023). Does the feeding method affect the quality of infant and maternal sleep? A systematic review. Infant Behavior and Development, 73, 101868. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2023.101868 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller-Graff, L., & Scheid, C. R. (2020). Breastfeeding continuation at 6 weeks postpartum remediates the negative effects of prenatal intimate partner violence on infant temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 32(2), 503510. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000245 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales-Muñoz, I., Saarenpää-Heikkilä, O., Kylliäinen, A., Pölkki, P., Porkka-Heiskanen, T., Paunio, T., & Paavonen, E. J. (2018). The effects of maternal risk factors during pregnancy on the onset of sleep difficulties in infants at 3 months old. Journal of Sleep Research, 27(5), 18. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12696 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, M., Ohta, H., Shimabukuro, R., Asaka, Y., Nakazawa, T., Oishi, Y., Hirata, M., Ando, A., Ikeda, T., & Yoshimura, Y. (2021). Daytime nap and nighttime breastfeeding are associated with toddlers’ nighttime sleep. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 3028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzari, S., Cagliero, L., Grumi, S., Pisoni, E., Mallucci, G., Bergamaschi, R., Maccarini, J., Giorda, R., & Provenzi, L. (2023a). Prenatal exposure to environmental air pollution and psychosocial stress jointly contribute to the epigenetic regulation of the serotonin transporter gene in newborns. Molecular Psychiatry, 28(8), 35033511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02206-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzari, S., Fearon, P., Rice, F., Ciceri, F., Molteni, M., & Frigerio, A. (2020). Neuroendocrine and immune markers of maternal stress during pregnancy and infant cognitive development. Developmental Psychobiology, 62(8), 11001110. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21967 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzari, S., Fearon, P., Rice, F., Molteni, M., & Frigerio, A. (2022a). Maternal caregiving moderates the impact of antenatal maternal cortisol on infant stress regulation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 63(8), 871880. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13532 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzari, S., Giorda, R., Bordoni, M., Pansarasa, O., Borgatti, R., Grumi, S., Mambretti, F., Villa, M., Giorda, R., Bordoni, M., Pansarasa, O., Borgatti, R., & Provenzi, L. (2023b). Sex-dimorphic pathways in the associations between maternal trait anxiety, infant BDNF methylation, and negative emotionality. Development and Psychopathology, 36(2), 908918. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000172 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzari, S., Grumi, S., Biasucci, G., Decembrino, L., Fazzi, E., Giacchero, R., Magnani, M. L., Nacinovich, R., Scelsa, B., Spinillo, A., Capelli, E., Roberti, E., & Provenzi, L. (2023c). Maternal pandemic-related stress during pregnancy associates with infants’ socio-cognitive development at 12 months: A longitudinal multi-centric study. PLOS ONE, 18(4), 115. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0284578 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazzari, S., Grumi, S., Mambretti, F., Villa, M., Giorda, R., Provenzi, L., Borgatti, R., Biasucci, G., Decembrino, L., Giacchero, R., Magnani, M. L., Nacinovich, R., Prefumo, F., Spinillo, A., & Veggiotti, P. (2022b). Maternal and infant NR3C1 and SLC6A4 epigenetic signatures of the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown: When timing matters. Translational Psychiatry, 12(1), 386. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-02160-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzari, S., Pili, M. P., Günay, Y., & Provenzi, L. (2024). Pandemic babies: A systematic review of the association between maternal pandemic-related stress during pregnancy and infant development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 162,105723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105723 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolvi, S., Karlsson, L., Bridgett, D. J., Korja, R., Huizink, A. C., Kataja, E.-L., & Karlsson, H. (2016). Maternal prenatal stress and infant emotional reactivity six months postpartum. Journal of Affective Disorders, 199, 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, T. G., Caprariello, P., Blackmore, E. R., Gregory, A. M., Glover, V., & Fleming, P. (2007). Prenatal mood disturbance predicts sleep problems in infancy and toddlerhood. Early Human Development, 83(7), 451458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okun, M. L., Mancuso, R. A., Hobel, C. J., Schetter, C. D., & Coussons-Read, M. (2018). Poor sleep quality increases symptoms of depression and anxiety in postpartum women. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 41(5), 703710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paavonen, E. J., Saarenpää-Heikkilä, O., Morales-Munoz, I., Virta, M., Häkälä, N., Pölkki, P., Kylliäinen, A., Karlsson, H., Paunio, T., & Karlsson, L. (2020). Normal sleep development in infants: Findings from two large birth cohorts. Sleep Medicine, 69, 145154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palagini, L., Drake, C. L., Gehrman, P., Meerlo, P., & Riemann, D. (2015). Early-life origin of adult insomnia: Does prenatal-early-life stress play a role? Sleep Medicine, 16(4), 446456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pastore, M., & Calcagnì, A. (2019). Measuring distribution similarities between samples: A distribution-free overlapping index. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedrabissi, L., & Santinello, M. (1989). Inventario per l’ansia di «Stato» e di «Tratto»: Nuova versione italiana dello STAI Forma Y: Manuale. Organizzazioni Speciali.Google Scholar
Pennestri, M.-H., Moss, E., O’Donnell, K., Lecompte, V., Bouvette-Turcot, A.-A., Atkinson, L., Minde, K., Gruber, R., Fleming, A. S., & Meaney, M. J. (2015). Establishment and consolidation of the sleep-wake cycle as a function of attachment pattern. Attachment & Human Development, 17(1), 2342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provenzi, L., Grumi, S., Giorda, R., Biasucci, G., Bonini, R., Cavallini, A., Decembrino, L., Drera, B., Falcone, R., Fazzi, E., Gardella, B., Giacchero, R., Nacinovich, R., Pisoni, C., Prefumo, F., Scelsa, B., Spartà, M. V., Veggiotti, P., Orcesi, S., & Borgatti, R. (2020). Measuring the Outcomes of Maternal COVID-19-related Prenatal Exposure (MOM-COPE): Study protocol for a multicentric longitudinal project. BMJ Open, 10(12), e044585. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-044585 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provenzi, L., Mambretti, F., Villa, M., Grumi, S., Citterio, A., Bertazzoli, E., Biasucci, G., Decembrino, L., Falcone, R., Gardella, B., Longo, M. R., Nacinovich, R., Pisoni, C., Prefumo, F., Orcesi, S., Scelsa, B., Giorda, R., & Borgatti, R. (2021). Hidden pandemic: COVID-19-related stress, SLC6A4 methylation, and infants’ temperament at 3 months. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-95053-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quante, M., McGee, G. W., Yu, X., von Ash, T., Luo, M., Kaplan, E. R., Rueschman, M., Haneuse, S., Davison, K. K., & Redline, S. (2022). Associations of sleep-related behaviors and the sleep environment at infant age one month with sleep patterns in infants five months later. Sleep Medicine, 94, 3137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenbaum, D. L., Gillen, M. M., & Markey, C. H. (2022). The importance of sleep and parity in understanding changes in weight and breastfeeding behavior among postpartum women. Appetite, 170, 105889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudzik, A. E. F., Robinson-Smith, L., & Ball, H. L. (2018). Discrepancies in maternal reports of infant sleep vs. actigraphy by mode of feeding. Sleep Medicine, 49, 9098.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sadeh, A., Tikotzky, L., & Kahn, M. (2014). Sleep in infancy and childhood: Implications for emotional and behavioral difficulties in adolescence and beyond. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 27(6), 453459. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simcock, G., Cobham, V. E., Laplante, D. P., Elgbeili, G., Gruber, R., Kildea, S., & King, S. (2019). A cross-lagged panel analysis of children’s sleep, attention, and mood in a prenatally stressed cohort: The QF2011 Queensland flood study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 255, 96104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.05.041 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stan Development Team (2018). Stan modeling language users guide and reference manual, version 2.18. 0. Stan Development Team.Google Scholar
Tharner, A., Luijk, M. P. C. M., Raat, H., IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Moll, H. A., Jaddoe, V. W. V., Hofman, A., Verhulst, F. C., & Tiemeier, H. (2012). Breastfeeding and its relation to maternal sensitivity and infant attachment. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(5), 396404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tikotzky, L., Volkovich, E., & Meiri, G. (2021). Maternal emotional distress and infant sleep: A longitudinal study from pregnancy Through 18 Months. Developmental Psychology, 57(7), 11111123. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001081 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomfohr-Madsen, L. M., Racine, N., Giesbrecht, G. F., Lebel, C., & Madigan, S. (2021). Depression and anxiety in pregnancy during COVID-19: A rapid review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 300, 113912.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den, Bergh, Bea, R. H., van den Heuvel, M. I., Lahti, M., Braeken, M., de Rooij, S. R., Entringer, S., Hoyer, D., Roseboom, T., Räikkönen, K., King, S., & Schwab, M. (2020). Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: The influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 117, 2664. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Den Bergh, B. R. H., Dahnke, R., & Mennes, M. (2018). Prenatal stress and the developing brain: Risks for neurodevelopmental disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 30, 743762. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000342 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Heuvel, M. I., Hect, J. L., Smarr, B. L., Qawasmeh, T., Kriegsfeld, L. J., Barcelona, J., Hijazi, K. E., & Thomason, M. E. (2021). Maternal stress during pregnancy alters fetal cortico-cerebellar connectivity in utero and increases child sleep problems after birth. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-81681-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vehtari, A., Gelman, A., & Gabry, J. (2017). Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC. Statistics and Computing, 27(5), 14131432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veru, F., Laplante, D. P., Luheshi, G., & King, S. (2014). Prenatal maternal stress exposure and immune function in the offspring. Stress-the International Journal On the Biology of Stress, 17(2), 133148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Victora, C. G., Bahl, R., Barros, A. J. D., França, G. V. A., Horton, S., Krasevec, J., Murch, S., Sankar, M. J., Walker, N., Rollins, N. C., Allen, K., Dharmage, S., Lodge, C., Peres, K. G., Bhandari, N., Chowdhury, R., Sinha, B., Taneja, S., Giugliani, E.et al. (2016). Breastfeeding in the 21st century: epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. The Lancet, 387(10017), 475490. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(15)01024-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis (pp. 1131). Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, S. R., Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Ekas, N. V., & Lickenbrock, D. M. (2012). Longitudinal assessment of sleep-wake regulation and attachment security with parents. Infant and Child Development, 21(5), 443457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptives of the included subjects

Figure 1

Table 2. Linear models definition

Figure 2

Table 3. Model comparison

Figure 3

Figure 1. Model M04 posterior predictive check [a] and expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal PRS assessed at T0 (x-axis) and exclusive breastfeeding groups [b].

Figure 4

Figure 2. Overlapping index for M04. Expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis) and exclusive breastfeeding groups represented in yellow and green respectively.

Figure 5

Figure 3. Triple interaction model posterior predictive check [a] and expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis), breastfeeding groups and different levels of maternal anxiety [b].

Figure 6

Figure 4. Triple interaction model overlapping index. Expected values of infants sleep problems at T3 (y-axis) as a function of maternal pandemic-related stress assessed at T0 (x-axis), exclusive breastfeeding groups (panels) and maternal anxiety level (colours).

Supplementary material: File

Mariani Wigley et al. supplementary material

Mariani Wigley et al. supplementary material
Download Mariani Wigley et al. supplementary material(File)
File 787.1 KB