Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T08:16:11.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective facial expression processing in young children who have experienced maltreatment during the first year of life: An event-related potential study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 April 2011

W. John Curtis*
Affiliation:
University of Kentucky
Dante Cicchetti
Affiliation:
Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester University of Minnesota
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: W. John Curtis, Department of Psychology, University of Kentucky, 207C Kastle Hall, Lexington, KY 40506; E-mail: [email protected].

Abstract

This study employed event-related potentials (ERPs) to investigate the neural correlates of facial affect processing in maltreated and nonmaltreated children at 42 months of age. ERPs elicited while children passively viewed standardized pictures of female models posing angry, happy, and neutral facial expressions were examined, and differences between maltreated (N = 46) and nonmaltreated (N = 25) children were reported. Three occipital components (P1, N170, and P400) and four frontal–central components (N150, P240, Nc, and a positive slow wave [PSW]) were identified. Findings revealed that maltreated children had greater P1 and P400 amplitude in response to angry facial affect compared to other emotions, and compared to nonmaltreated children. N170 amplitude was greater in response to happy compared to angry in the maltreated group. For the P240 component, maltreated children had greater amplitude in response to angry facial expressions relative to happy, whereas children in the nonmaltreated group had greater P240 amplitude in response to happy relative to angry facial affect. Further, the nonmaltreated group had greater PSW amplitude in response to angry facial affect, whereas those in the maltreated group showed greater PSW amplitude to happy facial expressions. The results provided further support for the hypothesis that the experience of maltreatment and the predominantly negative emotional tone in maltreating families alters the functioning of neural systems associated with the identification and processing of facial emotion. These results exemplify the importance of early preventive interventions focused on emotion for children who have experienced maltreatment early in life.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barnett, D., Manly, J. T., & Cicchetti, D. (1993). Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. L. (Eds.), Child abuse, child development, and social policy (pp. 773). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2003). Early processing of the six basic facial emotional expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batty, M., & Taylor, M. J. (2006). The development of emotional face processing during childhood. Developmental Science, 9, 207220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (1993). The Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Beteleva, T. G. (1998). Changes in the event-related potentials in a picture classification task. Human Physiology, 24, 6470.Google Scholar
Black, J., Jones, T. A., Nelson, C. A., & Greenough, W. T. (1998). Neuronal plasticity and the developing brain. In Alessi, N. E., Coyle, J. T., Harrison, S. I., & Eth, S. (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent psychiatry (pp. 3153). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bousha, D. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1984). Mother–child interactional style in abuse, neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 106114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camras, L., Grow, J. G., & Ribordy, S. (1983). Recognition of emotional expression by abused children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 12, 325328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Sachs, V., Spaccarelli, S., et al. (1990). Maternal facial behavior and the recognition and production of emotional expression by maltreated and nonmaltreated children. Developmental Psychology, 26, 304312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. A., Ribordy, S., Hill, J., Martino, S., Spaccarelli, S., & Stefani, R. (1988). Recognition and posing of emotional expressions by abused children and their mothers. Developmental Psychology, 24, 776781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caron, A. J., Caron, R. F., & MacLean, D. J. (1988). Infant discrimination of naturalistic emotional expressions: The role of face and voice. Child Development, 59, 604616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cassidy, J. (1994). Emotion regulation: Influences of attachment relationships. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 228283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B., & Izard, C. (1995). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1991). Toward the development of a scientific nosology of child maltreatment. In Grove, W. & Cicchetti, D., (Eds.) Thinking clearly about psychology: Essays in honor of Paul E. Meehl: Vol. 2. Personality and psychopathology (pp. 346377). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Cannon, T. D. (1999). Neurodevelopmental processes in the ontogenesis and epigenesis of psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 375393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2005). An event-related potential (ERP) study of processing of affective facial expressions in young children who have experienced maltreatment during the first year of life. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 641677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Curtis, W. J. (2006). The developing brain and neural plasticity: Implications for normality, psychopathology, and resilience. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 2. Developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 164). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Gunnar, M. R. (2008). Integrating biological processes into the design and evaluation of preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 737743.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Izard, C. (Eds.). (1995). Emotions in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 1226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 623649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., & Schneider-Rosen, K. (1984). Theoretical and empirical considerations in the investigation of the relationship between affect and cognition in atypical populations of infants: Contributions to the formulation of an integrative theory of development. In Izard, C., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 366406). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S. L., & Manly, J. T. (2003). Maternal Maltreatment Interview. Unpublished manuscript, University of Rochester.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and self-regulatory structures of the mind. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 533549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Development, 72, 317333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connell-Carrick, K., & Scannapieco, M. (2006). Ecological correlates of neglect in infants and toddlers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 299316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courchesne, E., Chisum, H., & Townsend, J. (1994). Neural activity-dependent brain changes in development: Implications for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 697722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, P. M. (1981). Abusing, neglecting, problematic, and adequate dyads: Differentiating by patterns of interaction. Merrill–Palmer Quarterly, 27, 201208.Google Scholar
Curtis, W. J., & Cicchetti, D. (2003). Moving research on resilience into the 21st century: Theoretical and methodological considerations in examining the biological contributors to resilience. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 773810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H., & McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neutral facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Science, 7, 340359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeBoer, T., Scott, L. S., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). ERPs in developmental populations. In Handy, T. C. (Ed.), Event-related potentials: A methods handbook (pp. 263297). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., & Halit, H. (2007). Development of face-sensitive event-related potentials during infancy. In de Haan, M. (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-related potentials (pp. 7799). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1999). Brain activity differentiates face and object processing by 6-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 34, 11141121.Google Scholar
Dennis, T. A., Malone, M. M., & Chen, C-C. (2009). Emotional face processing and emotion regulation in children: An ERP study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 85102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1990). Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 16781683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donchin, E., Karis, D., Bashore, T. R., Coles, M. G. H., & Gratton, G. (1986). Cognitive psychophysiology and human information processing. In Coles, M. G. H., Donchin, E., & Porges, S. W. (Eds.), Psychophysiology (pp. 244267). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Durand, K., Gallay, M., Seigneuric, A., Robichon, F., & Baudouin, J.-Y. (2007). The development of facial emotion recognition: The role of configural information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 1427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Gaudin, J. M., Polansky, N. A., Kilpatrick, A. C., & Shilton, P. (1996). Family functioning in neglectful families. Child Abuse & Neglect, 20, 363377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Vaituzis, A. C., Hamburger, S. D., Lange, N., Rajapakse, J. C., Kaysen, D., et al. (1996). Quantitative MRI of the temporal lobe, amygdala, and hippocampus in normal human development: Ages 4–18 years. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 366, 223230.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroencephalography and Clinical neurophysiology, 55, 468484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guyer, A. E., McClure, E. B., Adler, A. D., Brotman, M. A., Rich, B. A., Kimes, A. S., et al. (2007). Specificity of facial expression labeling deficits in childhood psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 863871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrenkohl, R. C., Herrenkohl, E. C., Egolf, B. P., & Wu, P. (1991). The developmental consequences of child abuse: The Lehigh longitudinal study. In Starr, R. H. & Wolfe, D. A. (Eds.), The effects of child abuse and neglect: Issues and research (pp. 5781). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hesse, P., & Cicchetti, D. (1982). Perspectives on an integrated theory of emotional development. New Directions for Child Development, 16, 348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillyard, S. A., & Picton, T. W. (1987). Electrophysiology of cognition. In Mountcastle, V. (Ed.), Handbook of physiology: Higher functions of the brain (Vol. 5, pp. 519583). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
Hoehl, S., & Striano, T. (2008). Neural processing of eye gaze and threat-related emotional facial expressions in infancy. Child Development, 79, 17521760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. (2002). Neural plasticity: The effects of environment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E., Fine, S., Mostow, A., Trentacosta, C., & Campbell, J. (2002). Emotion processes in normal and abnormal development and preventive intervention. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 761787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E., & Harris, P. (1995). Emotional development and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (pp. 467503). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E., Youngstrom, E. A., Fine, S. E., Mostow, A. J., & Trentacosta, C. J. (2006). Emotions and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and methods (2nd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Jasper, H. H. (1958). The 10-20 electrode system of the international federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371375.Google Scholar
Kats-Gold, I., Besser, A., & Priel, B. (2007). The role of simple emotion recognition skills among school aged boys at risk of ADHD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 363378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanaugh, K. A., Youngblade, L., Reid, J. B., & Fagot, B. I. (1988). Interactions between children and abusive versus control parents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 17, 137142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Key, A. P. F., Dove, G. O., & Maguire, M. J. (2005). Linking brainwaves to the brain: An ERP primer. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 183215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kobiella, A., Grossman, T., Reid, V. M., & Striano, T. (2008). The discrimination of angry and fearful facial expressions in 7-month-old infants: An event-related potential study. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 134146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutas, M., & Dale, A. (1997). Electrical and magnetic readings of mental functions. In Rugg, M. (Ed.), Cognitive neuroscience (pp. 197242). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Leist, T., & Dadds, M. R. (2009) Adolescents' ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14, 237250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, J. M., Moulson, M. C., Vogel-Farley, V. K., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). An ERP study of emotional face processing in the adult and infant brain. Child Development, 78, 232245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D., Zoll, D., & Stahl, J. (1987). Infants at social risk: Relationships among infant maltreatment, maternal behavior, and infant attachment behavior. Developmental Psychology, 23, 223232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masten, C. L., Guyer, A. E., Hodgdon, H. B., McClure, E. B., Charney, D. S., Ernst, M., et al. (2008). Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 139153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondloch, C. J., Geldart, S., Maurer, D., & Le Grand, R. (2003). Developmental changes in face processing skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 6784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulson, M. C., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moulson, M. C., Westerlund, A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). The effects of early experience on face recognition: An event-related potential study of institutionalized children in Romania. Child Development, 80, 10391056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A. (1987). The recognition of facial expressions in the first two years of life: Mechanisms of development. Child Development, 58, 889909.Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., & Monk, C. M. (2001). The use of event related potentials in the study of cognitive development. In Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 125136). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Parker, S. W., Nelson, C. A., & The Bucharest Early Intervention Core Group. (2005). The impact of early institutional rearing on the ability to discriminate facial expressions of emotion: An event-related potential study. Child Development, 76, 5472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D. (2008). Mechanisms linking early experience and the emergence of emotions: Illustrations from the study of maltreated children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 370375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Hornung, K., & Reed, A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36, 679688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., Cicchetti, D., Klorman, R., & Brumaghim, J. (1997). Cognitive brain event-related potentials and emotion processing in maltreated children. Child Development, 68, 773787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., & Kistler, D. J. (2002). Early experience alters categorical representation for facial expressions of emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 90729076.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., Klorman, R., Thatcher, J. E., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). P3b reflects maltreated children's reaction to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology, 38, 267274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., Messner, M., Kistler, D. J., & Cohn, J. F. (2009). Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition, 110, 242247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., & Sinha, P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children's recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38, 784791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollak, S. D., & Tolley-Schell, S. A. (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 323338.Google ScholarPubMed
Rossion, B., Campanella, S., Gomez, C. M., Delinte, A., Debatisse, D., Liard, L., et al. (1999). Task modulation of brain activity related to familiar and unfamiliar face processing: An ERP study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 449462.Google ScholarPubMed
Serrano, J. M., Iglesias, J., & Loeches, A. (1992). Visual discrimination and recognition of facial expressions of anger, fear, and surprise in 4- to 6-month old infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 25, 411425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shackman, J. E., Shackman, A. J., & Pollak, S. D. (2007). Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion, 7, 838852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stiles, J. (2008). The fundamentals of brain development: Integrating nature and nurture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M. J., McCarthy, G., Saliba, E., & Degiovanni, E. (1999). ERP evidence of developmental changes in processing faces. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 910915.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 10, 547558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, R. M., Lewis, M. D., Meusel, L. A., & Zelazo, P. D. (2008). The time course of social–emotional processing in early childhood: ERP responses to facial affect and personal familiarity in a go–nogo task. Neuropsychologia, 46, 595613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonks, J., Williams, W. H., Frampton, I., Yates, P., & Salter, A. (2007). Assessing emotion recognition in 9–15 year olds: Preliminary analysis of abilities in reading emotion from faces, voices and eyes. Brain Injury, 21, 623629.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Maughan, A., Manly, J. T., Spagnola, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2002). The relative efficacy of two interventions in altering maltreated preschool children's representational models: Implications for attachment theory. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 777808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toth, S. L., Pianta, R. C., & Erickson, M. F. (in press). From research to practice: Developmental contributions to the field of prevention science. In Cicchetti, D. & Roisman, G. I. (Eds.), Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology: Vol. 36. The Origins and Organization of Adaptation and Maladaptation. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Vasey, M. W., & Thayer, J. F. (1987). The continuing problem of false positives in repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: A multivariate solution. Psychophysiology, 24, 479486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogel, E. K., & Luck, S. J. (2000). The visual N1 component as an index of a discrimination process. Psychophysiology, 37, 190203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walden, T. A., & Field, T. M. (1982). Discrimination of facial expressions by preschool children. Child Development, 53, 13121319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker-Andrews, A. S. (1997). Infants' perception of expressive behaviors: Differentiation of multimodal information. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 437456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, S. J., Long, J. D., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). A longitudinal investigation of visual event-related potentials in the first year of life. Developmental Science, 8, 605616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed