Hb is an Fe-containing metal protein found in the erythrocytes, and is responsible for O2 transportation in vertebrates. In addition to being bound to Hb, Fe is found in cells throughout the human body, and is crucial for multiple metabolic processes, which range from DNA synthesis to electron transportation(Reference Conrad, Umbreit and Moore1, Reference Beard2). As defined by the WHO, Fe deficiency is the most common nutritional disorder and the leading cause of anaemia in the world. A quarter of the world's population, i.e. 1·6 billion people, are estimated to be anaemic, with pregnant women and children being the most vulnerable to this deficiency(3). Anaemia is commonly defined as Hb levels below 138 g/l in men, 121 g/l in women, and 110 g/l in pregnant women and in children.
Through its complex role in oxidative metabolism and in the synthesis of neurotransmitters and myelin, the importance of Fe in normal neurological functions is increasingly being recognised(Reference Beard4). However, with 41·5 % of the global population of pregnant women (European 18·7 % and African 55·8 %) being anaemic(Reference McLean5), there is still a shortage of studies examining the association between Fe deficiency and early brain development.
Studies in rodents have shown that pregnant rats fed on an Fe-deficient diet during both early gestation and lactation give birth to offspring with lower Fe concentrations in the brain(Reference Felt and Lozoff6, Reference Kwik-Uribe, Golub and Keen7). Fe supplementation in mid-gestation or immediately following delivery did not raise the brain Fe concentration as expected, with dams and pups exhibiting significantly poorer cognitive performance. Human studies also suggest an association of Fe deficiency and supplementation, particularly in early life, with long-term cognitive development(Reference Grantham-McGregor and Ani8–Reference Sachdev, Gera and Nestel10). However, there are no studies examining the relationship of maternal Fe status (as a marker for pre-natal exposure) with offspring's long-term cognitive development. Using data obtained from the 1966 Northern Finnish Birth Cohort (NFBC 1966) study, we examined the association between maternal Hb levels during pregnancy and later educational achievement to obtain further insights into the possible long-term influences of Fe status in utero.
Methods
Population
The NFBC 1966 dataset comprises data pertaining to the births in the two northernmost provinces of Finland (i.e. Oulu and Lapland) during 1966 (live births n 12 058, coverage 96 %)(Reference Rantakallio11). Data on mothers were collected through questionnaires/interviews and records at communal maternal welfare centres during routine antenatal visits; mothers were recruited for the study between the twenty-fourth and the twenty-eighth gestational weeks. Of these, 9·7 % of the data were completed in later pregnancy or after birth. Children were followed into adulthood, and data were collected at birth and at 14 and 31 years of age using hospital records/official national registers and self-administered questionnaires/interviews. In the present study, we analysed data on individuals with written informed consent and born from singleton pregnancies. Data pertaining to socio-demographic status, behaviour, whether pregnancy was wanted or not, mother's educational status, mother's emotional status and smoking were extracted from this dataset for analysis(Reference Jarvelin, Sovio and King12, Reference Rantakallio13).
Measures
Maternal Hb levels were measured at the third, seventh and ninth gestational months during antenatal visits at health centres or hospitals. Seven different methods available at that time were used, i.e. Tallqvists table, Ljungberg, Sicca, Sahli, Vatatron-Fotom, Erka and Linson Junior. In order to make the results comparable, they were standardised and scaled to grams per litre by clinical and laboratory experts and by the research team. Information on maternal Fe supplementation was not available.
Data on school performance were acquired at 14 and 16 years of age through scores given by teachers. Scores ranged from 4 to 10 on theory subjects such as physics, mathematics, chemistry, the first and second national languages, foreign languages, biology, religion/ethics, social studies, history, geography and natural sciences (given as separate scores/subject and as an overall performance score), and overall score of all subjects, which was calculated as the average of scores given on theory subjects and practical subjects (i.e. music, visual art, health education, crafts, physical education and household economics, available at both 14 and 16 years of age). Information on school performance at the age of 14 years was obtained through self-administered questionnaires sent to the adolescents or to the parents, if the adolescents did not respond (total response n 9983, 86 % of the study sample). Information on school achievement at the age of 16 years was extracted directly from school reports (n 10 474, 90 % of the study sample). School scores at the ages of 14 (self-reported) and 16 (reported by the school) years were highly correlated (intraclass correlation coefficient = 0·9), suggesting a good reliability for the self-reported scores. Information on higher educational status, with details of university degree if present, was acquired through a postal questionnaire at the age of 31 years (n 8427, 72 % of the study sample). The present study was conducted in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all procedures involving human subjects were approved by the University of Oulu ethics committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all the subjects.
Statistical analysis
The ANOVA or Student's t test was used to run the statistical analysis to examine the association between background variables and maternal Hb levels during pregnancy and school scores of the offspring (Table 1). The χ2 test was used to analyse the association between background covariates and the level of education at the age of 31 years. The association of persistent anaemia (Hb < 110 g/l measured at all points, n 39) and intermittent anaemia (n 1440, Hb < 110 g/l in either the third (n 823), seventh (n 869) or ninth (n 376) month of gestation) with educational achievements was also examined by fitting separate models. Multiple linear regressions were run for continuous outcomes (school scores) by assuming a linear relationship between the variable and its predictors. Residual plots were checked for the model assumptions. Logistic and polytomous regressions were run for binomial and categorical variables. Analyses were first performed using linear or logistic regression to examine the sex-adjusted association between the predictors and the outcomes. Among the available information (i.e. sex, gestational age, birth weight and length, child's calendar month of birth, mother's educational status, whether pregnancy was wanted or not, social class, parity, mother's marital status at pregnancy, mother's smoking status during pregnancy, mother's mental status, father's social status, mother's weight, mother's height and child's Fe supplementation during infancy), factors were selected as covariates in the final model if a significant improvement in the goodness of fit of the model was achieved. The proportion of participants with missing data varied by covariate, ranging from 0 % for sex to 27·7 % for information on higher education at the age of 31 years. We imputed (five times) the data to replace the missing values using the multivariate imputation by chained equations package(Reference Van Buuren and Oudshoorn14). Statistical analysis was run by R (2.3.0) package(Reference Everitt and Hothorn15).
NA, not available.
* Based on the information provided by mothers (self-reported). Unknown group was not included in the statistical tests.
Results
Table 1 shows the associations between confounding factors, exposure and educational outcomes. Children from higher social class, married families and wanted pregnancies, and female offspring and first or second born had significantly better school performance at the age of 16 years (P < 0·001 for each comparison), and were more likely to have completed a university degree by the age of 31 years (P < 0·001).
Data on Hb levels were available for 9470 expectant mothers; of these, 84·79 % (n 8030) did not have any form of anaemia at any stage during pregnancy. Thirty-nine expectant mothers (0·41 %) had persistent anaemia, and 1440 (15·21 %) had intermittent anaemia at third, seventh and/or ninth month of gestation (but were not anaemic at all points).
Adjusted for sex, maternal Hb at all stages of pregnancy was associated with school scores at the ages of 14 and 16 years (P < 0·01 for all comparisons), while associations with higher education were observed for concentrations at the seventh and ninth months only. After full adjustment for potential confounders (sex, birth weight, calendar month of birth, mother's educational status, whether pregnancy was wanted or not, social class, parity, mother's marital status at pregnancy, mother's smoking status during pregnancy and mother's mental status), the associations between higher maternal Hb levels and school performance persisted for measures taken at 9 months, but not for measures taken at earlier stages of pregnancy (Table 2). Some suggestion for an association with school performance at the age of 16 years was also observed for maternal concentrations at 3 months in the imputed case analyses (P = 0·04), but not in the complete case analyses. After full adjustment, maternal Hb at any single time point was not associated with the higher final education. However, offspring of mothers with anaemia at least in some stage of pregnancy (Hb < 110 g/l at one or more points) appeared to have lower school scores, and they were also less likely to have completed a higher education degree at the age of 31 years (Table 2). The associations between maternal Hb and school achievement were similar in the subgroup of mothers who were non-anaemic throughout pregnancy compared with those observed in the full cohort (data not shown). There was no evidence for an interaction between Hb levels at different points of measurement and sex in relation to school scores or higher educational achievement (P>0·15 for all comparisons).
* Adjusted for sex, birth weight, mother's educational status, whether pregnancy was wanted or not, mother's social class, number of children, mother's marital status at pregnancy, mother's smoking status during pregnancy, mother's mental status and child's calendar month of birth.
† Models with multiple imputation on missing information, adjusted as described above.
‡ Comparison for anaemia ( < 110 g/l) at one or more occasions v. no anaemia at any stage.
Discussion
Using data obtained from a large prospective cohort study, we found evidence for a direct association between maternal Hb levels during pregnancy and the educational achievement of the offspring. These data suggest long-term influences of pre-natal Fe metabolism, supporting earlier observations from animal experiments(Reference Beard16–Reference Felt20). Previous studies in human subjects are sparse, although earlier data have suggested reduced cognitive performance after Fe deficiency during infancy/early childhood(Reference Algarin, Peirano and Garrido21–Reference Walter26).
It is unclear what individual pathways are involved in Fe deficiencies thought to affect brain development. Plausible routes described in previous human/animal studies include the following: alteration in neurotransmitter and/or brain energy metabolism and/or decreased myelination(Reference Lozoff, Bear and Connor9, Reference Youdim19, Reference Beard27). In the present study, maternal Hb at the final trimester (but not at the earlier stages of pregnancy) was associated with the offspring's school performance, which indicates that there could be a sensitive period for the final stages of development(Reference Ben-Shlomo and Kuh28). This would be biologically plausible as during the final trimester of pregnancy and early infancy, myelination can be altered as a result of Fe deficiency(Reference Roncagliolo, Garrido and Walter29, Reference Walter30). It is also possible that the association could have been mediated by maternal influences on Hb concentrations in early infancy, which would provide an alternative explanation for the lack of effect for the first two trimesters. Lack of association for the earlier stages of pregnancy can also be explained by the initiation of Fe supplementation to mothers diagnosed as anaemic.
Unfortunately, information on infant concentrations or on maternal Fe supplementation during pregnancy was not available from the present study. Nevertheless, based on standard clinical procedures(Reference Milman31), it is reasonable to assume that Fe supplementation would have been initiated for all women diagnosed as anaemic.
Longitudinal studies in human subjects examining motor development and cognitive function through to adulthood have indicated their negative relation with Fe deficiency anaemia in infancy(Reference Beard32–Reference Lozoff34). Randomised control trials examining Fe status and school scores also reported a relationship between the two, with improvement in scores after Fe treatment(Reference Pollitt, Hathiral and Kotchabhakdi35, Reference Soemantri, Pollitt and Kim36). Though these studies had included Fe status in addition to Hb levels in their analysis, they had failed to examine the effects of maternal Fe status during pregnancy on offspring. In addition to underlining the importance of maternal Hb level in the cognitive performance of the offspring, the present study suggested that the effect of Fe deficiency during pregnancy is long lasting. This is important, because despite previous suggestions for programming of cognitive function by maternal Fe status from animal experiments(Reference Beard16–Reference Felt20), there are very little data obtained from studies in human subjects. The effect sizes appear to be fairly modest in the present study, although the suggested 14 % lower proportion of offspring achieving higher education for anaemic mothers (compared with others) may be important. Furthermore, due to the active monitoring (and treatment) of pregnant women in Finland at the time of the study, there were very few mothers with severe anaemia, and many of the women classified here as anaemic would have received treatment. Hence, extrapolation of effect sizes to other populations should be done with caution, and it is possible that associations between maternal anaemia and offspring's school performance might be stronger for the more extreme cases of deficiency.
Some methodological issues also need to be considered in the context of these findings. It should be noted that although in most cases anaemia can reasonably be assumed to be due to Fe deficiency, we did not have Fe status measures to confirm this. Some of the reduction in maternal Hb during the mid-pregnancy is likely to be due to haemodilution (expansion of blood volume), which typically leads to an approximately 10 g/l reduction in circulating maternal concentrations. However, this would provide further support for our suggestive observation for a key role of late pregnancy, as the lowest Hb concentrations are typically observed about 28–32 weeks, after which, concentrations naturally raise again towards the end of the pregnancy in most mothers(Reference Koller37). Missing data are a problem for all these types of longitudinal studies with follow-up lasting over several decades, although, on average, the participation rates in the NFBC 1966 study were fairly good (with 75 % of the target population participating at the age of 31 years). Multiple imputation was used for missing data in the main analyses, and similar results that were obtained were compared to complete case analyses in most analyses. Exception was the borderline association between maternal Hb at 3 months and school performance, which was observed for the imputed models, but not in the complete case series. This suggests that reduced power due to missing information could have contributed to non-significant findings. Multiple imputation is based on the assumption that the data are missing at random, and although there is no reason to believe that this assumption is violated in the NFBC 1966 study, possible bias due to missing information cannot be ruled out. Information on the school scores at the age of 14 years was self-reported, and is therefore subject to reporting bias. However, school-reported and self-reported scores were strongly correlated in the present study, suggesting that self-reported scores were reliable. Furthermore, level of education was the only outcome measured at the age of 31 years, and it can be argued that more detailed measures are required for reliable estimation of potential influences on adult cognitive performance.
Conclusion
The data suggest that there may be long-term influences of maternal Hb during the final stages of pregnancy on the cognitive development of offspring, a finding in urgent need of replication. If true, this observation is important for the clinical care of pregnant women, as it suggests that Fe prophylaxis even at fairly late stages of pregnancy may be beneficial for the subsequent development of the offspring. The association between maternal Hb level during pregnancy and offspring's school performance is potentially of great public health importance as Fe deficiency is highly prevalent in pregnant women (40 % worldwide). Further investigations with a wider range of mental performance indicators are needed to evaluate the relationships and mechanisms of maternal Fe and offspring's mental performance.
Acknowledgements
We thank Professor Paula Rantakallio (launch of the NFBC 1966 study) and Mr Markku Koiranen (data management and advice) from the Institute of Health Sciences, University of Oulu, Finland. The Academy of Finland has financially supported NFBC 1966 study. Dr Ian Brown (Department of Epidemiology and Public Health, Imperial College London, UK) and Ms Diane Berry (UCL Institute of Child Health, London) are acknowledged for advice and help on data analysis. Support for the present research was provided by a PhD grant from the Yasuj University of Medical Sciences (a non-profit organisation). E. H. is funded by the Department of Health (UK) Public Health Career Scientist Award. U. S. is funded by the Medical Research Council, UK. The Centre for Epidemiology of Child Health is funded by the Medical Research Council. GOSH/UCL Institute of Child Health receives a proportion of funding from the Department of Health's NIHR Biomedical Research Centres funding scheme. The authors' contributions are as follows: M. F. carried out the data analysis and wrote the article along with E. H. and M.-R. J. Data were managed by M. F. and U. S. The work was supervised by E. H., M.-R. J, J. W. and C. R. Data were collected by A. R., A. P., A.-L. H. and M.-R. J. All authors participated in the evaluation of the results, revised the manuscript and approved the final version. The authors report no conflicts of interest with the present paper.