Article contents
Toward a unified account of comprehension and production in language development
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 24 June 2013
Abstract
Although Pickering & Garrod (P&G) argue convincingly for a unified system for language comprehension and production, they fail to explain how such a system might develop. Using a recent computational model of language acquisition as an example, we sketch a developmental perspective on the integration of comprehension and production. We conclude that only through development can we fully understand the intertwined nature of comprehension and production in adult processing.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013
References
Arnon, I. & Snider, N. (2010) More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language
62:67–82.Google Scholar
Bannard, C. & Matthews, D. (2008) Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children's repetition of four-word combinations. Psychological Science
19:241–48.Google Scholar
Borensztajn, G., Zuidema, J. & Bod, R. (2009) Children's grammars grow more abstract with age. Topics in Cognitive Science
1:175–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, F., Dell, G. S. & Bock, K. (2006) Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review
113(2):234–272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, F., Ferraro, V. & Bailey, K. G. D. (2002) Good enough representations in language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science
11:11–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, C., Bellugi, U. & Brown, R. (1963) Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior
2:121–35.Google Scholar
Freudenthal, D., Pine, J. M., Aguado-Orea, J. & Gobet, F. (2007) Modelling the developmental pattern of finiteness marking in English, Dutch, German, and Spanish using MOSAIC. Cognitive Science
31:311–41.Google Scholar
Gertner, Y. & Fisher, C. (2012) Predicted errors in children's early sentence comprehension. Cognition
124:85–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, A., Muller, A., Hamann, C. & Ruigendijk, E. (2011) Production-comprehension asymmetries in child language. De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janssen, N. & Barber, H. A. (2012) Phrase frequency effects in language production. PLoS ONE7:e33202.Google ScholarPubMed
Mani, N. & Huettig, F. (2012) Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake – but only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance
38: 843–47.Google Scholar
McCauley, S. M. & Christiansen, M. H. (2011) Learning simple statistics for language comprehension and production: The CAPPUCCINO model. In: Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, ed. Carlson, L., Hölscher, C. & Shipley, T., pp. 1619–24. Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
McCauley, S. M. & Christiansen, M. H. (submitted) Language learning as language use: A computational model of children's comprehension and production of language. Manuscript in preparation, Cornell University.Google Scholar
Pelucchi, B., Hay, J. F. & Saffran, J. R. (2009) Learning in reverse: Eight-month-old infants track backward transitional probabilities. Cognition
113:244–47.Google Scholar
Reali, F. & Christiansen, M. H. (2007) Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. Journal of Memory and Language, 57:1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanford, A. J. & Sturt, P. (2002) Depth of processing in language comprehension: Not noticing the evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences
6:382–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
- 7
- Cited by
Target article
An integrated theory of language production and comprehension
Related commentaries (32)
A developmental perspective on the integration of language production and comprehension
An ecological alternative to a “sad response”: Public language use transcends the boundaries of the skin
Are forward models enough to explain self-monitoring? Insights from patients and eye movements
Cascading and feedback in interactive models of production: A reflection of forward modeling?
Communicative intentions can modulate the linguistic perception-action link
Does what you hear predict what you will do and say?
Evidence for, and predictions from, forward modeling in language production
Forward modelling requires intention recognition and non-impoverished predictions
How do forward models work? And why would you want them?
Inner speech as a forward model?
Integrate, yes, but what and how? A computational approach of sensorimotor fusion in speech
Intentional strategies that make co-actors more predictable: The case of signaling
Intermediate representations exclude embodiment
Is there any evidence for forward modeling in language production?
It ain't what you do (it's the way that you do it)
Memory and cognitive control in an integrated theory of language processing
Prediction in processing is a by-product of language learning
Prediction is no panacea: The key to language is in the unexpected
Prediction plays a key role in language development as well as processing
Predictive coding? Yes, but from what source?
Preparing to be punched: Prediction may not always require inference of intentions
Seeking predictions from a predictive framework
The complexity-cost factor in bilingualism
The neurobiology of receptive-expressive language interdependence
The poor helping the rich: How can incomplete representations monitor complete ones?
The role of action in verbal communication and shared reality
Toward a unified account of comprehension and production in language development
Towards a complete multiple-mechanism account of predictive language processing
What does it mean to predict one's own utterances?
What is the context of prediction?
When to simulate and when to associate? Accounting for inter-talker variability in the speech signal
“Well, that's one way”: Interactivity in parsing and production
Author response
Forward models and their implications for production, comprehension, and dialogue