Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T05:00:03.074Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceiving numerosity from birth

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 July 2017

Maria Dolores de Hevia
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 75006 Paris, [email protected] CNRS UMR 8242, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, 75006 Paris, [email protected]@gmail.comhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=LolaDhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=ArletteShttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=VeroniqueI
Elisa Castaldi
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/I2BM, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin Center, 91191 Gif/Yvette, France. [email protected]@gmail.comhttp://www.pisavisionlab.org/index.php/people/postdocs/elisa-castaldi
Arlette Streri
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 75006 Paris, [email protected] CNRS UMR 8242, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, 75006 Paris, [email protected]@gmail.comhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=LolaDhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=ArletteShttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=VeroniqueI
Evelyn Eger
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroimaging Unit, CEA DRF/I2BM, INSERM, Université Paris-Sud, Université Paris-Saclay, NeuroSpin Center, 91191 Gif/Yvette, France. [email protected]@gmail.comhttp://www.pisavisionlab.org/index.php/people/postdocs/elisa-castaldi
Véronique Izard
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes, Sorbonne Paris Cité, 75006 Paris, [email protected] CNRS UMR 8242, Laboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, 75006 Paris, [email protected]@gmail.comhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=LolaDhttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=ArletteShttp://lpp.psycho.univ-paris5.fr/person.php?name=VeroniqueI

Abstract

Leibovich et al. opened up an important discussion on the nature and origins of numerosity perception. The authors rightly point out that non-numerical features of stimuli influence this ability. Despite these biases, there is evidence that from birth, humans perceive and represent numerosities, and not just non-numerical quantitative features such as item size, density, and convex hull.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anobile, G., Castaldi, E., Turi, M., Tinelli, F. & Burr, D. C. (2016b) Numerosity but not texture-density discrimination correlates with math ability in children. Developmental Psychology 52(8):1206–16. doi: 10.1037/dev0000155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antell, S. E. & Caron, A. J. (1985) Neonatal perception of spatial relationships. Infant Behavior and Development 8(1):1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brannon, E. M., Abbott, S. & Lutz, D. J. (2004) Number bias for the discrimination of large visual sets in infancy. Cognition 93(2):B5968. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.01.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castaldi, E., Aagten-Murphy, D., Tosetti, M., Burr, D. & Morrone, M. C. (2016) Effects of adaptation on numerosity decoding in the human brain. NeuroImage 143:364–77.Google Scholar
Cordes, S. & Brannon, E. M. (2008) The difficulties of representing continuous extent in infancy: Using number is just easier. Child Development 79(2):476–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01137.x.Google Scholar
Cordes, S. & Brannon, E. M. (2011) Attending to one of many: When infants are surprisingly poor at discriminating an item's size. Frontiers in Psychology 2:65. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Hevia, M. D., Izard, V., Coubart, A., Spelke, E. S. & Streri, A. (2014) Representations of space, time, and number in neonates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(13):4809–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eger, E., Michel, V., Thirion, B., Amadon, A., Dehaene, S. & Kleinschmidt, A. (2009) Deciphering cortical number coding from human brain activity patterns. Current Biology 19(19):1608–15.Google Scholar
Farroni, T., Valenza, E., Simion, F. & Umiltà, C. (2000) Configural processing at birth: Evidence for perceptual organisation. Perception 29(3):355–72.Google Scholar
Guellai, B. & Streri, A. (2011) Cues for early social skills: Direct gaze modulates newborns' recognition of talking faces. PLoS ONE 6(4):e18610.Google Scholar
Harvey, B. M., Fracasso, A., Petridou, N. & Dumoulin, S. O. (2015) Topographic representations of object size and relationships with numerosity reveal generalized quantity processing in human parietal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112(44):13525–30. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1515414112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, B. M., Klein, B. P., Petridou, N. & Dumoulin, S. O. (2013) Topographic representation of numerosity in the human parietal cortex. Science 341(6150):1123–26. doi: 10.1126/science.1239052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, V., Dehaene-Lambertz, G. & Dehaene, S. (2008) Distinct cerebral pathways for object identity and number in human infants. PLoS Biology 6(2):e11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, V., Sann, C., Spelke, E. S. & Steri, A. (2009) Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(25):10382–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libertus, M. E., Starr, A. & Brannon, E. M. (2014) Number trumps area for 7-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 50(1):108–12. doi: 10.1037/a0032986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipton, J. S. & Spelke, E. S. (2003) Origins of number sense large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science 14(5):396401. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J., DeWind, N. K., Woldorff, M. G. & Brannon, E. M. (2016b) Rapid and direct encoding of numerosity in the visual stream. Cerebral Cortex 26(2):748–63. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv017.Google ScholarPubMed
Piazza, M., Izard, V., Pinel, P., Le Bihan, D. & Dehaene, S. (2004) Tuning curves for approximate numerosity in the human intraparietal sulcus. Neuron 44(3):547–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.014.Google Scholar
Turati, C., Gava, L., Valenza, E. & Ghirardi, V. (2013) Number versus extent in newborns' spontaneous preference for collections of dots. Cognitive Development 28(1):1020.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenza, E., Leo, I., Gava, L. & Simion, F. (2006) Perceptual completion in newborn human infants. Child Development 77(6):1810–21.Google Scholar
Xu, F. (2003) Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations. Cognition 89(1):B1525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, F. & Spelke, E. S. (2000) Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74(1):B111. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00066-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar