Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:00:52.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The planar mosaic fails to account for spatially directed action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 October 2013

Roberta L. Klatzky
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA 15213. [email protected]://www.psy.cmu.edu/people/klatzky.html
Nicholas A. Giudice
Affiliation:
Spatial Informatics Program, School of Computing and Information Science, University of Maine, Orono, ME 04469-5711. [email protected]://spatial.umaine.edu/faculty/giudice/

Abstract

Humans' spatial representations enable navigation and reaching to targets above the ground plane, even without direct perceptual support. Such abilities are inconsistent with an impoverished representation of the third dimension. Features that differentiate humans from most terrestrial animals, including raised eye height and arms dedicated to manipulation rather than locomotion, have led to robust metric representations of volumetric space.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Avraamides, M., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M. & Golledge, R. G. (2004) Use of cognitive vs. perceptual heading during imagined locomotion depends on the response mode. Psychological Science 15:403408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, J. M. & Vishton, P. M. (1995) Perceiving layout and knowing distances: The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth. In: Handbook of perception and cognition, vol. 5: Perception of space and motion, ed. Epstein, W. & Rogers, S., pp. 69117. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Giudice, N. A., Klatzky, R. L., Bennett, C. & Loomis, J. M. (2013) Perception of 3-D location based on vision, touch, and extended touch. Experimental Brain Research 224:141–53.Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. L. (1998) Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In: Spatial cognition – An interdisciplinary approach to representation and processing of spatial knowledge (Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence, vol. 1404), ed. Freksa, C., Habel, C. & Wender, K. F., pp. 117. Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Loomis, J. L., Klatzky, R. L. & Giudice, N. A. (2013) Representing 3D space in working memory: Spatial images from vision, hearing, touch, and language. In: Multisensory imagery: Theory and applications, ed. Lacey, S. & Lawson, R., pp. 131–55. Springer.Google Scholar
Loomis, J. M., Lippa, Y., Klatzky, R. L. & Golledge, R. G. (2002) Spatial updating of locations specified by 3-D sound and spatial language. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning, Memory, and Cognition 28:335–45.Google Scholar