Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:25:46.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Revisiting the traditional conceptualizations of vocabulary knowledge as predictors of dual language learners’ English reading achievement in a new destination state

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2023

Min Hyun Oh*
Affiliation:
School of Education and Human Development, University of Virginia, 235 Ridley Hall, Charlottesville, VA 22903, USA
Jeannette Mancilla-Martinez
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University, Peabody College, 230 Appleton Place, Nashville, TN 37203, USA
Jin Kyoung Hwang
Affiliation:
School of Education, University of California, Irvine, 467 Social Sciences Tower, Irvine, CA 92697, USA
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The unprecedented growth of Spanish-English dual language learners (DLLs) in new destination states (e.g., Georgia, Indiana, South Carolina, Tennessee) calls for better understanding of the relation between their bilingual vocabulary skills and English reading achievement. The current study focused on school-age Spanish-English DLLs (N = 60) in Tennessee and explored how various vocabulary knowledge conceptualizations predict English reading comprehension achievement, controlling for English word reading skills and grade level. Vocabulary knowledge was assessed using monolingual (English-only and Spanish-only) and bilingual (conceptual and total) scoring methods. Results showed that, while DLLs performed below the national mean for English-only and Spanish-only vocabulary, they performed within the average to above-average range for bilingually scored conceptual vocabulary. More uniquely, the expressive vocabulary knowledge emerged as a robust predictor of English reading comprehension above and beyond the influence of English word reading skills. Findings suggest practical and theoretical value of bilingually driven vocabulary assessment approaches. As expected, bilingually scored vocabulary provided a more comprehensive understanding of DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge by accounting for vocabulary knowledge in both Spanish and English, compared to monolingually scored vocabulary. We discuss theoretical and instructional implications, with a focus on asset-driven and scientific assessment understandings for supporting DLLs’ vocabulary and reading achievement in new destination states.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Concerns about students’ reading comprehension (RC) achievement in the U.S. continue, given the far-reaching negative consequences of compromised RC (World Literacy Foundation, 2018). The challenge is particularly salient for school-age dual language learners (DLLs) who are simultaneously developing English proficiency and acquiring academic content in English (Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Mancilla-Martinez2020). In the U.S., DLLs comprise approximately 23% of the K-12 student population (Kids Count Data Center, 2020), and the majority are from Spanish-speaking homes (García, Reference García, Pianta, Barnett, Justice and Sheridan2012). To be clear, speaking more than one language itself is not a risk factor for compromised academic outcomes (Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Mancilla-Martinez2020). However and of concern, school-age Spanish-English DLLs often come from households living in or near poverty (Gennetian et al., Reference Gennetian, Guzman, Ramos-Olazagasti and Wildsmith2019; Lopez & Velasco, Reference Lopez and Velasco2011), and poverty compromises students’ academic experiences and trajectories, including RC (Heidlage et al., Reference Heidlage, Cunningham, Kaiser, Trivette, Barton, Frey and Roberts2020; Luo et al., Reference Luo, Tamis-LeMonda and Mendelsohn2020). The large and growing presence of Spanish-English DLLs across the nation necessitates that we revisit the traditional conceptualization of a well-known RC contributor, vocabulary knowledge. Even though DLLs are not monolinguals, the traditional conceptualization of DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge is based on the assessment of language-specific vocabulary, typically English-only vocabulary.

DLLs are not a new U.S. student population, but certain areas of the country—commonly known as new destination states (e.g., Georgia, Indiana, South Carolina, Tennessee)—have experienced rapid, unprecedented growth of DLLs in the last decade (Smolarek, Reference Smolarek2020). Mirroring national trends, DLLs in new destination states predominantly come from Spanish-speaking and low-income homes (Gándara & Mordechay, Reference Gándara and Mordechay2017). In other words, new destination states do not stand out as an anomaly relative to national DLL demographic trends. However, compared to traditional destination states (e.g., California, Texas), new destination states do stand out in that, by definition, they have more nascent experience educating DLLs. Consequently, schools and educators in these contexts tend to have comparatively less experience and fewer resources to support DLLs’ overall academic achievement (Lee & Hawkins, Reference Lee and Hawkins2015; Lowenhaupt & Reeves, Reference Lowenhaupt and Reeves2015; Umansky et al., Reference Umansky, Hopkins, Dabach, Porter, Thompson and Pompa2018).

In this study, we draw on Hoover and Gough’s (Reference Hoover and Gough1990) Simple View of Reading (SVR) model—a well-known, parsimonious model that frames RC as a product of word reading (i.e., the ability to read the printed words in a text) and language comprehension (i.e., the ability to automatically associate meaning to speech sounds). The SVR has been empirically supported across numerous scientific studies of reading involving both monolinguals and DLLs (e.g., Catts et al., Reference Catts, Herrera, Nielsen and Bridges2015; Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Hoff, Uchikoshi, Gillanders, Castro and Sandilos2014; Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and McClain2020). Within the SVR, vocabulary knowledge represents one important aspect of language comprehension (e.g., Braze et al., Reference Braze, Katz, Magnuson, Mencl, Tabor, Van Dyke, Gong, Johns and Shankweiler2016; Proctor et al., Reference Proctor, Uccelli, Dalton and Snow2009; Tunmer & Chapman, Reference Tunmer and Chapman2012; van Steensel et al., Reference van Steensel, Oostdam, van Gelderen and van Schooten2016), which is the component of interest in this study. Additionally, we draw on the shared (distributed) asymmetrical model as our theoretical framework for assessing DLLs’ vocabulary (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Gui and MacWhinney2005).

As U.S. student demographics continue to change and DLLs become less “new” across all contexts, a more nuanced and scientifically based understanding of the relation between Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary skills—which generally consist of vocabulary knowledge across English and Spanish—and their English RC achievement is both a pressing need and a timely matter of educational equity. To contribute to the literature on English RC among school-age DLLs and particularly students from Spanish-speaking homes, we conducted this exploratory study in Tennessee (i.e., a new immigrant destination state in the American South). Our goal was to compare the relation between monolingual and bilingual conceptualizations of DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge and their English RC outcomes. By studying DLLs in a new destination state (i.e., Tennessee), this study can offer theoretical and practical insight for better supporting English RC of English-Spanish DLLs broadly and specifically for those in new destination states where access to formal instruction and resources in Spanish tend to be relatively limited compared to traditional DLL-serving states.

Conceptualizing English reading comprehension

Decades of empirical and theoretical studies on RC have revealed a multitude of contributors to successful RC achievement (e.g., Gough & Tunmer, Reference Gough and Tunmer1986; Kendeou et al., Reference Kendeou, Papadopoulos and Kotzapoulou2013; Kirby & Savage, Reference Kirby and Savage2008; Lesaux et al., Reference Lesaux, Crosson, Kieffer and Pierce2010; Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and McClain2020; Perfetti & Stafura, Reference Perfetti and Stafura2014; RAND, 2002). We frame our exploration of English RC predictors in terms of the SVR (Gough & Tunmer, Reference Gough and Tunmer1986). The SVR posits that successful RC relies on the interrelation between language comprehension and word reading. Language comprehension encompasses the ability to process language and derive meaning, across word-, sentence-, or discourse-level oral language skills, while word reading refers to students’ ability to process printed text. Importantly, both components are multidimensional (Tunmer & Chapman, Reference Tunmer and Chapman2012), comprised of multiple subskills such as various top-down (e.g., vocabulary knowledge, syntactic knowledge, pragmatics) and bottom-up (e.g., sight word recognition, phonemic decoding, morphological awareness) processes. Proficiency in both language comprehension and word reading skills, and their cross-product, is essential to successful RC for monolingual English speakers and DLLs (e.g., Carver & David, Reference Carver and David2009; Catts et al., Reference Catts, Herrera, Nielsen and Bridges2015; Florit & Cain, Reference Florit and Cain2011; Mancilla-Martinez & Lesaux, Reference Mancilla-Martinez and Lesaux2017; Nakamoto et al., Reference Nakamoto, Lindsey and Manis2008; Silverman et al., Reference Silverman, Johnson, Keane and Khanna2020). In fact, Hoover and Gough’s (Reference Hoover and Gough1990) well-known longitudinal SVR study revealed that both language comprehension and word reading made independent, significant contributions to English RC. The two components were not only independent, but also highly inter-dependent, such that simply supporting language comprehension or word reading would be insufficient (Hoover & Gough, Reference Hoover and Gough1990). A less well-known fact is that this study was conducted with school-age Spanish-English DLLs.

A wealth of research points to the critical role of word reading for successful RC (Foorman et al., Reference Foorman, Petscher and Herrera2018; Lonigan et al., Reference Lonigan, Burgess and Schatschneider2018). Specifically, research with Spanish-English DLLs suggests a robust relation between RC and English and Spanish word reading skills (e.g., Cárdenas-Hagan et al., Reference Cárdenas-Hagan, Carlson and Pollard-Durodola2007; Durgunoğlu et al., Reference Durgunoğlu, Nagy and Hancin-Bhatt1993; Melby-Lervåg & Lervåg, Reference Melby-Lervåg and Lervåg2011). In this study, we examine DLLs’ language comprehension skills using DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge, which is a typical proxy of language comprehension within the SVR framework. In doing so, it is important to acknowledge that Spanish-English DLLs are not English monolinguals. Although DLLs come from homes where a language other than—or in addition to—English is used, not all DLLs are English learners (i.e., students formally identified and designated as limited English-proficient by their schools). In fact, DLLs’ language proficiency can vary widely. For example, some Spanish-English DLLs are more proficient in Spanish than in English, some more proficient in English than in Spanish, and others similarly proficient in both English and Spanish. This raises questions about how vocabulary skills have traditionally been assessed among this heterogenous and fast-growing group of learners. Indeed, although SVR studies on U.S. Spanish-English DLLs have expanded the field’s understanding of the reading process within and across languages (e.g., Cho et al., Reference Cho, Capin, Roberts, Roberts and Vaughn2019; Goodrich & Namkung, Reference Goodrich and Namkung2019; Gottardo & Mueller, Reference Gottardo and Mueller2009; Mancilla-Martinez & Lesaux, Reference Mancilla-Martinez and Lesaux2017; Nakamoto et al., Reference Nakamoto, Lindsey and Manis2008; Proctor et al., Reference Proctor, August, Carlo and Snow2006), research on the contribution of bilingually scored—compared to traditional, monolingually scored (e.g., English-only, Spanish-only)—vocabulary knowledge to Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC remains limited (Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and McClain2020).

Conceptualizing Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge

Substantial evidence underscores the central role of vocabulary knowledge for DLLs’ successful English RC (e.g., August et al., Reference August, Carlo, Dressler and Snow2005; Lesaux et al., Reference Lesaux, Crosson, Kieffer and Pierce2010; RAND, 2002; Snow & Kim, Reference Snow, Kim, Wagner, Muse and Tannenbaum2007). In fact, studies on Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge have reported significant effects of Spanish vocabulary knowledge on English RC (e.g., Goodrich & Namkung, Reference Goodrich and Namkung2019; Proctor et al., Reference Proctor, August, Carlo and Snow2006). Furthermore, it is important to recognize that DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge is distributed across multiple languages (Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Pan and Vagh2011; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Fernández and Oller1995), which necessitates attention to both English and Spanish, for example, to ensure comprehensive vocabulary assessment. Indeed, decades of research have cautioned against expecting DLLs to demonstrate vocabulary knowledge in each language on par with that of monolinguals (e.g., Grosjean, Reference Grosjean1989). Notwithstanding, the use of monolingual vocabulary assessments—typically English monolingual in the U.S. context—remains common practice with DLLs (Arias & Friberg, Reference Arias and Friberg2017; Caesar & Kohler, Reference Caesar and Kohler2007).

Reliance on monolingual vocabulary assessments (e.g., English-only vocabulary) when assessing DLLs could lead to a partial and potentially misguided understanding of their word knowledge (Bedore et al., Reference Bedore, Peña, Garcia and Cortez2005; Hammer et al., Reference Hammer, Hoff, Uchikoshi, Gillanders, Castro and Sandilos2014; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021). In fact, studies using monolingual vocabulary assessments have consistently found DLLs to score below their monolingual peers, as monolingual testing taps only a part of what DLLs know and underestimates their comprehensive word knowledge (Ehl et al., Reference Ehl, Bruns and Brosche2020; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021; Oller & Pearson, Reference Oller, Pearson, Oller and Eilers2002). Even when Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge is assessed using both English monolingual and Spanish monolingual tests, many studies report low vocabulary knowledge in both languages (Gross et al., Reference Gross, Buac and Kaushanskaya2014; Hoff, Reference Hoff2018). A central consideration is that the bulk of vocabulary assessments, whether in English or in Spanish, is designed for and normed on monolinguals. However, DLLs are not monolinguals. Given that high-stakes educational decisions (e.g., diagnoses for language-related difficulties, special education identification) are often made based on standardized assessment results, continued reliance on monolingual vocabulary assessments raises important educational equity concerns (Sugarman & Villegas, Reference Sugarman and Villegas2020). Hence, there is a pressing need to investigate the utility of bilingually scored vocabulary assessments with the growing population of Spanish-English DLLs.

Numerous models have been proposed to explain the bilingual mental lexicon. Given our focus on vocabulary knowledge as a proxy of language comprehension within the SVR framework, we draw on tenets of the shared (distributed) asymmetrical model proposed by Dong and colleagues (Reference Dong, Gui and MacWhinney2005), as this model incorporates features of the most prominent models of the bilingual mental lexicon (i.e., the distributed model, the revised hierarchical model, the separate storage model, the word-association model, and the conceptual mediation model). As the name implies, according to the shared (distributed) asymmetrical model, there is shared storage for conceptual representations of the bilingual’s two vocabularies (in this study, Spanish vocabulary and English vocabulary). However, there are also asymmetrical (i.e., distributed or separate) links between concepts and lexical names in the two languages. The shared (distributed) asymmetrical model thus offers a useful framework for exploring the distributed nature of school-age Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge. Specifically, this model suggests that bilingual, rather than monolingual, scoring methods may provide a more appropriate and scientifically grounded approach for assessing DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge. In other words, Spanish-English DLLs can be expected to produce lexical names (labels) in only English (e.g., cat), only Spanish (e.g., gato), both languages (e.g., gato and cat), or neither language (i.e., does not know the lexical name for the target concept) for one underlying concept. As such, vocabulary assessments that account for these varied response patterns among DLLs are needed.

The two most common bilingual scoring methods used to characterize DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge are conceptual vocabulary and total vocabulary (e.g., Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Fernández and Oller1995). Between these two bilingual scoring methods, conceptually scored vocabulary—which accounts for DLLs’ vocabulary in either language (e.g., English or Spanish) by giving credit for known concepts, rather than for correct responses in a single language (as is the case with monolingual assessments)—has been more widely studied. Conceptually scored vocabulary scores are typically derived by summing all the words a DLL knows in both languages, then subtracting translation equivalents. In other words, if DLLs know both cat and gato, they receive single credit for the overall concept and not double credit for knowing both labels. Conceptually scored vocabulary has been used as an indicator of DLLs’ language ability (e.g., Marchman et al., Reference Marchman, Fernald and Hurtado2010; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Fernández and Oller1993, Reference Pearson, Fernández and Oller1995), with growing evidence of its utility for understanding Spanish-English DLLs’ linguistic competence (e.g., Anaya et al., Reference Anaya, Peña and Bedore2018; Bedore et al., Reference Bedore, Peña, Garcia and Cortez2005; Peña & Halle, Reference Peña and Halle2011; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, McClain, Oh and Flores2020). Further, conceptual scoring mitigates not only score differences compared to monolingual scoring, but also widely observed vocabulary achievement differences between DLLs and their English monolingual peers (Bedore et al., Reference Bedore, Peña, Garcia and Cortez2005; Core et al., Reference Core, Hoff, Rumiche and Señor2013; Gross et al., Reference Gross, Buac and Kaushanskaya2014; Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Pan and Vagh2011; Pearson et al., Reference Pearson, Fernández and Oller1993; Peña et al., Reference Peña, Bedore and Kester2015).

Total scoring represents the second, albeit comparably rarely used, method of bilingual scoring for DLLs. Similar to conceptual scoring, total scoring allows the child to respond using both of their languages (e.g., English and Spanish). However, total scoring does give DLLs double credit. If DLLs know both cat and gato, they would receive double credit, unlike conceptual scoring that would give single credit. Thus, total vocabulary scores represent the sum of all the words DLLs know in English and Spanish, without subtracting translation equivalents. As such, total vocabulary has been described as a more comprehensive indicator of what DLLs know compared to conceptual vocabulary (Core et al., Reference Core, Hoff, Rumiche and Señor2013; Hoff & Core, Reference Hoff and Core2015; O’Toole et al., Reference O’Toole, Gatt, Hickey, Miękisz, Haman, Armon-Lotem, Rinker, Ohana, dos Santos and Kern2017). Studies to date find DLLs’ total vocabulary to be on par with or to exceed that of their monolingual peers (De Houwer et al., Reference De Houwer, Bornstein and Putnik2014; Hoff et al., Reference Hoff, Core, Place, Rumiche, Señor and Parra2012; Poulin-Dubois et al., Reference Poulin-Dubois, Bialystok, Blaye, Polonia and Yott2013). Further, compared to conceptual vocabulary, total vocabulary taps both word form (i.e., phonological representations) and meaning (i.e., concepts) to a greater extent, which are two of the key components of word knowledge (McGregor et al., Reference McGregor, Friedman, Reilly and Newman2002; Storkel, Reference Storkel2001). Core et al. (Reference Core, Hoff, Rumiche and Señor2013) assert that, while conceptual vocabulary puts word form at the peripheral (i.e., due to its focus on the conceptual aspect of word knowledge), total vocabulary attends to both dimensions by prompting both English and Spanish for each test item. By testing words known in both languages, total scoring helps measure the full scope of DLLs’ semantic and phonological knowledge—and thus more elaborate word knowledge (Pearson, Reference Pearson1998)—compared to conceptual scoring.

However, most studies on bilingual scoring of DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge are concentrated at the early childhood level. Only recently have studies started to use bilingual scoring with school-age Spanish-English DLLs and mostly focus on conceptually scored vocabulary (e.g., Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, McClain, Oh and Flores2020; Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and Pokowitz2020). Few studies have directly compared conceptual and total vocabulary scores among DLLs (e.g., Kern et al., Reference Kern, Valente and dos Santos2019; Legacy et al., Reference Legacy, Zesiger, Friend and Poulin-Dubois2016; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021). Studies suggest that DLLs have relatively larger total vocabulary than conceptual vocabulary (Kern et al., Reference Kern, Valente and dos Santos2019; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021) or report DLLs to have significantly larger total vocabulary—but not conceptual vocabulary—compared to their monolingual peers (Legacy et al., Reference Legacy, Zesiger, Friend and Poulin-Dubois2016). In particular, research on bilingually scored vocabulary among DLLs from Spanish-speaking homes—the most common language background among the school-aged DLLs in the U.S. (Gándara & Mordechay, Reference Gándara and Mordechay2017)—is limited (for exceptions, see Core et al., Reference Core, Hoff, Rumiche and Señor2013; Marchman et al., Reference Marchman, Fernald and Hurtado2010; Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021), especially at the elementary-school level when high-stakes educational decisions (e.g., identification for language-related difficulties) are often made. Therefore, studies that examine elementary-aged DLLs’ bilingually scored vocabulary as contributors to English RC are warranted for identifying equitable assessment practices for the growing population of DLLs across the nation.

Current study

The primary aim of this study is to gain an understanding of the extent to which vocabulary assessments designed for monolinguals (i.e., the typical scenario in most U.S. classrooms) compared to those designed for DLLs differentially predict English RC of school-age Spanish-English DLLs in a new immigrant destination state in the American South. Our study aims to address this measurement gap by using bilingual vocabulary assessments designed for and normed on Spanish-English DLLs to proxy conceptual and total vocabulary. To our knowledge, studies to date have not compared the predictive roles of conceptual and total vocabulary—the two most common bilingual scoring vocabulary approaches—on English RC among school-age Spanish-English DLLs.

Our study aims to address open questions about the relative utility of these related but distinct conceptualizations of bilingual vocabulary scoring for predicting English RC of the fastest-growing U.S. student population. The following research question guides this study: How do vocabulary assessments designed for use with monolinguals (i.e., English-only and Spanish-only) compare to those designed for use with DLLs (i.e., conceptual and total) in predicting school-age Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC, above and beyond the well-known influence of English word reading?

Method

Participants

The dataset used in the current study comes from a larger three-year longitudinal study on DLLs’ conceptual vocabulary knowledge development, for which three cohorts (i.e., kindergarten, first grade, or second grade in the first year of the study) of Spanish-English DLLs were recruited from three elementary schools in the same large urban school district in Tennessee. In the participating schools, approximately half of the student population consisted of English learners (i.e., DLLs actively receiving English language services; M = 51.02%; SD = 4.16%), followed by English-proficient DLLs (i.e., DLLs who formerly or never received English language services; M = 25.85%, SD = 5.74%) and native English speakers (M = 22.73%; SD = 2.02%). Further, on average, students were predominantly Hispanic (M = 65.10%; SD = 2.38%), White (M = 14.18%; SD = 2.80%), and Black (M = 13.83%; SD = 2.75%). Other racial and ethnic backgrounds made up less than 10% of the student population (i.e., Asian, Pacific Islander, Native American). As is often the case in classrooms in new destination states, students in our sample were instructed in an English-only context. The analytical sample was drawn from the last year (i.e., third year) of the project, when the kindergarten cohort was in second grade (n = 34) and the first-grade cohort was in fourth grade (n = 26). Specifically, we used student data from the last wave of the project when total vocabulary was assessed. To note, students who were in the second-grade cohort had transitioned into middle school and were no longer followed in the study.

Students were considered DLLs if their parents indicated that Spanish was spoken at home to some extent. Of students’ parents who reported their child’s language use at home in the first year of the larger study (n = 54), 33% (n = 18) used both English and Spanish, 5% (n = 3) used mostly English, 18% (n = 11) used mostly Spanish, and 35% (n = 21) used only Spanish. In addition, 63% of the students (n = 38) were formally identified as English learners by their schools, and the remaining 37% (n = 22) were English-proficient during the last wave. Only 8% of the students (n = 5) were identified for special education services. Family demographic information was collected as a part of the larger longitudinal project (n = 54), and the vast majority (90%) of DLLs were U.S.-born, and the rest were born outside the U.S. (6% in Honduras, 2% in Mexico, and 2% in Cuba).

Procedure

Students’ English RC scores come from a district-wide assessment administered in May, the end of the academic year. Additionally, students’ vocabulary and English word reading skills were individually assessed in May by Spanish-fluent graduate and undergraduate research assistants in a quiet area in each school.

Measures

English reading comprehension

English RC was measured with the reading test in the Measures of Academic Progress (MAP) Growth (Northwest Evaluation Association [NWEA], 2019). The MAP reading test is a multiple-choice, computer-adaptive assessment (i.e., the assessment presents students either harder or easier items based on their responses to the previous test items) that the school district of our participating schools used to monitor students’ academic progress. MAP reading scores are in the form of Rasch Unit (RIT) scores, which are equal-interval, vertically scaled scores that allow measurement of student improvement (Thum & Kuhfeld, Reference Thum and Kuhfeld2020). The types of passages and content structure of the MAP reading assessment slightly vary by grade levels (NWEA, 2019). From kindergarten to second grade, the content areas include the following: foundational skills (e.g., word recognition), language and writing (e.g., spelling, grammar), literature and informational (e.g., key ideas of informational and literary texts), and vocabulary use and functions (e.g., vocabulary use, context clues). From second grade to fifth grade, the content areas include the following: information text (e.g., inference, point of view), literacy text (e.g., figurative language, point of view, text structures), and vocabulary acquisition and use (e.g., context clues, word parts, academic vocabulary). The publisher reports marginal reliability coefficients of .96 for second graders and .94 for fourth graders (NWEA, 2011).

Monolingually scored vocabulary

English-only vocabulary

English-only vocabulary was measured both receptively and expressively. Receptive vocabulary was measured with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-4 (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, Reference Dunn and Dunn2007), a measure of a child’s ability to correctly point to a picture that corresponds to a target word provided by the examiner. The PPVT is normed on English-proficient population in the U.S. The internal consistency reliabilities are .96-.97 for ages 7 and 8 (average years of age for second graders) and .94-.96 for ages 9 and 10 (average years of age for fourth graders). Expressive vocabulary was measured with the Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey-III (WMLS-III) Picture Vocabulary subtest (Woodcock et al., Reference Woodcock, Alvarado and Ruef2017), a measure of a child’s ability to label the target item prompted by an examiner. The publisher reports Cronbach’s alphas of .77 to .79 for ages 7 to 10.

Spanish-only vocabulary

Spanish-only vocabulary was also measured both receptively and expressively. Receptive vocabulary was measured with the Test de Vocabulary en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP; Dunn et al., Reference Dunn, Padilla, Lugo and Dunn1986), the parallel test of PPVT-4 in Spanish. The TVIP is normed on monolingual Spanish speakers in Latin America. For ages 7–8 and 9–10, the publisher reports split-half reliability coefficients of .94 and .91-.94, respectively. Expressive vocabulary was measured with the WMLS-III Vocabulario sobre Dibujos subtest (Woodcock et al., Reference Woodcock, Alvarado and Ruef2017), the Spanish equivalent to the WMLS-III Picture Vocabulary subtest. The publisher reports Cronbach’s alphas of .77 to .79 for ages 7 to 10.

Spanish-English bilingually scored vocabulary

The Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-4: Spanish-Bilingual Edition (ROWPVT-4: SBE; Martin, Reference Martin2013a) and the Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary test-4: Spanish-Bilingual Edition (EOWPVT-4: SBE; Martin, Reference Martin2013b) were used to measure receptive and expressive bilingually scored vocabulary, respectively. Both ROWPVT-4: SBE and EOWPVT-4: SBE were normed on over 1,200 Spanish-English bilinguals (ages 2 through 70) in the U.S.

Conceptual vocabulary

Receptive and expressive conceptually scored vocabulary were measured by following the standardized protocol established by the ROWPVT-4: SBE (Martin, Reference Martin2013a) and EOWPVT-4: SBE (Martin, Reference Martin2013b). Both ROWPVT-4: SBE and EOWPVT-4: SBE administration started with practice items. During the receptive test, an examiner provided the target test item in the student’s dominant language (i.e., either English or Spanish), based on parent or teacher report. If the student could not respond or responded incorrectly at the first prompt (e.g., in English), the examiner prompted the student again in the other language (e.g., in Spanish). Similarly, during the expressive test, an examiner presented the child with a picture and asked them to label the test item in either English or Spanish (i.e., “What is this?” or “¿Qué es esto?”). As bilingual assessments of vocabulary knowledge, conceptual scoring accepts a response as correct regardless of the language of the response (i.e., English or Spanish) as long as the child accurately identifies the concept of the target item. For both tests, the publisher reports median internal consistency reliability coefficient as .95.

Total vocabulary

Receptive and expressive total vocabulary scores were researcher-generated using the two bilingual vocabulary assessments, the ROWPVT-4: SBE and the EOWPVT-4: SBE, due to the lack of a standardized protocol and assessment for total vocabulary scoring. To generate total vocabulary scores, we used a non-standard administration of the ROWPVT-4: SBE and the EOWPVT-4: SBE immediately after the standardized conceptual vocabulary administration using the two measures. In contrast to standardized conceptual scoring that began at the age-recommended start item, total scoring began at item 1 of the ROWPVT-4: SBE and the EOWPVT-4: SBE.

During total vocabulary assessment, an examiner prompted the child in both languages (i.e., English and Spanish) from item 1 to the test ceiling (i.e., where the conceptual testing ended under the standardized protocol). Specifically, the child was prompted in either English or Spanish for items that were not prompted in the other language during the conceptual vocabulary assessment (i.e., an examiner did not prompt the child in the other language if the correct response was given at the first prompting). In other words, all tested items were not administered twice. Rather, total vocabulary assessment ensured that the test items that were not prompted in both or either of the languages (i.e., English and Spanish) during conceptual scoring were probed. By doing so, we were able to record whether the child knew the labels in both languages (i.e., English and Spanish), in only one (i.e., English or Spanish) or in neither language (i.e., they did not know the label in neither English nor Spanish) for all tested items. If the child provided a correct answer in both English and Spanish for an item, two points were given for the item.

Because receptive and expressive total vocabulary scores were researcher-generated, there were no associated norms. Nonetheless, the sample-derived reliability values (i.e., Cronbach’s alphas) for total receptive and expressive total vocabulary were .81 and .73, respectively. While the values satisfy the acceptable threshold of Cronbach’s alpha, we underscore that total vocabulary scoring is not a standardized procedure, as total vocabulary scores were generated in our exploratory effort to understand bilingually driven scoring methods for DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge. Given that existing empirical data on total vocabulary is mostly focused on the early childhood level (e.g., Core et al., Reference Core, Hoff, Rumiche and Señor2013; Mancilla-Martinez & Vagh, Reference Mancilla-Martinez and Vagh2013), our exploration of total vocabulary as a contributor to school-age DLLs’ English RC achievement may offer valuable insight into bilingual vocabulary knowledge and its relation to reading achievement of this fast-growing student population.

English word reading

We used the Test of Word Reading Efficiency-2 (TOWRE-2; Torgesen et al., Reference Torgesen, Wagner and Rashotte2012) to measure students’ English word reading. TOWRE-2 is comprised of two subtests—the Sight Word Efficiency and Phonemic Decoding Efficiency—that tap real-world reading and non-word reading, respectively. In the current study, raw scores from the two subtests were combined to gauge DLLs’ overall English word reading skills. Reported reliability coefficients range from .95 to .96 for the average age ranges for second and fourth graders. To note, given the English-only instructional context of our participants and the well-documented robust and positive relation between English and Spanish word reading skills (e.g., Cárdenas-Hagan et al., Reference Cárdenas-Hagan, Carlson and Pollard-Durodola2007; Melby-Lervåg & Lervåg, Reference Melby-Lervåg and Lervåg2011), we did not assess students’ Spanish word reading skills.

Analytic plan

To answer our research question on the predictive roles of monolingually scored vocabulary and bilingually scored on Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC, we conducted ordinary least squares (OLS) regression analyses using SAS PROC MIXED with full maximum likelihood estimation to account for missing data. We created four OLS regression models for each vocabulary conceptualization (i.e., English-only, Spanish-only, conceptual, and total). In addition to grade level, we controlled for English word reading given the SVR framework (Hoover & Gough, Reference Hoover and Gough1990; Joshi & Aaron, Reference Joshi and Aaron2000).

Results

Descriptive analyses

Tables 1 and 2 present the descriptive findings of all student-level variables in our study. We identified two outliers in the English-only expressive vocabulary data (i.e., Picture Vocabulary subtest of the WMLS-III) and the Spanish-only expressive vocabulary data (i.e., Vocabulario de Dibujos subtest of the WMLS-III), which represented less than 4% of the data for both variables. Outliers for each variable were transformed using winsorization (Ludwig-Mayerhofer, Reference Ludwig-Mayerhofer2020). We then checked for normality of distribution for all variables using a Shapiro-Wilk test, which showed that all variables were normally distributed.

Table 1. Correlations among English reading comprehension, vocabulary conceptualizations, and English word reading

Note. Raw scores were used for correlations.

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

Table 2. Sample means on English reading comprehension, vocabulary conceptualizations, and English word reading by grade level

Note. English RC = MAP Growth Reading. Rasch unit scores are reported for English RC, which allows between-grade comparisons.

1 Spring-normed MAP Reading scores for second grade = 185.57 (15.49).

2 Spring-normed MAP Reading scores for fourth grade = 204.83 (16.31). English-only receptive vocabulary = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-4. English-only expressive vocabulary = Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey-III—Picture Vocabulary subtest. Spanish-only receptive vocabulary = Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. Spanish-only expressive vocabulary = Woodcock-Muñoz Language Survey-III—Vocabulario sobre Dibujos subtest. Receptive conceptual and total vocabulary = Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-4: Spanish-Bilingual Edition. Expressive conceptual and total vocabulary = Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test-4: Spanish-Bilingual Edition. English word reading = Test of Word Reading Efficiency-2 (TOWRE-2). Outliers of English-only and Spanish-only expressive vocabulary raw scores have been replaced using winsorization.

Table 1 presents the correlations among English RC, vocabulary (i.e., English-only, Spanish-only, conceptual, and total), and English word reading. There were positive and moderate to high correlations between English RC and all other measures. There were also positive within-language vocabulary correlations between the receptive and expressive domains. The bilingually scored vocabulary measures revealed positive and moderate to large correlations with the monolingual vocabulary measures, except for conceptually scored vocabulary and Spanish-only expressive vocabulary. Total vocabulary showed positive and moderate to large correlations with other vocabulary measures.

Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations of DLLs’ English RC, vocabulary, and English word reading scores for the total sample and by grade level. Raw scores are reported for all assessments, except for English RC, and standardized scores (e.g., standard scores, scaled scores, and Rasch unit scores) are also reported, except for total vocabulary. For each grade, DLLs’ English RC scores fell below the national average, compared to the MAP Reading national norms (NWEA, 2020). Using Cohen’s (Reference Cohen1992) conventions (i.e., .2 = small effect, .5 = medium effect, greater than .8 = large effect), we found a moderate difference for second graders (n = 34, Cohen’s d = .48) and a small difference for fourth graders (n = 25, Cohen’s d = .26) compared to the grade-specific national average.

Furthermore, DLLs in both grade levels performed below the standard mean of 100 on the monolingual vocabulary assessments (i.e., English-only and Spanish-only). On average, DLLs performed better receptively than expressively. Monolingual receptive vocabulary scores fell approximately one standard deviation below the national average, but their monolingual expressive vocabulary scores fell two to three standard deviations below the national norms. Specifically, DLLs performed well below the average on Spanish-only expressive vocabulary (i.e., over three standard deviations) and demonstrated slightly better, yet still low, English-only expressive vocabulary (i.e., within two standard deviations). In stark contrast, for both grade levels, DLLs’ performance on the receptive and expressive bilingually scored conceptual vocabulary was within the average to above-average range. Finally, on average, English word reading scores fell within the national average.

Predictors of DLLs’ English reading comprehension

Roles of vocabulary conceptualizations

To address our research question on how the various conceptualizations of DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge predict their English RC above and beyond overall English word reading, we developed four OLS regression models for each vocabulary conceptualization (i.e., English-only, Spanish-only, conceptual, and total; see Table 3). Again, raw scores were used to allow comparisons with the researcher-generated total vocabulary scores due to the absence of normative scores. In addition to English word reading, grade level was included as a control variable given the significant—and expected—difference in vocabulary achievement between grade levels (see Table 3). All reported coefficients are standardized estimates. That is, the coefficients for grade reflect its effect size on English RC, accounting for receptive vocabulary, expressive vocabulary, and English word reading scores under each vocabulary conceptualization. By accounting for English word reading and grade level across all four models, we aimed to specifically examine the extent to which receptive and expressive knowledge within each vocabulary conceptualization predicts DLLs’ English RC.

Table 3. OLS regression models predicting English reading comprehension by monolingually scored versus bilingually scored receptive and expressive vocabulary knowledge, accounting for English word reading and grade level

Note. N = 60 using multiple imputation. OLS = Ordinary least squares. Standard error is in parentheses.

* p < .05.

** p < .01.

*** p < .001.

As shown in Table 3, expressive, but not receptive, vocabulary consistently predicted English RC across Models 1, 3, and 4. The only exception was Model 2, for Spanish-only vocabulary, in which none of the vocabulary predictors (receptive and expressive) were significant. Given the robust role of expressive vocabulary, we then tested subsequent models utilizing only expressive vocabulary as a predictor of English RC (see Table 4, Models 1 and 2). Models 1 and 2 both included monolingually scored expressive vocabulary (i.e., English-only and Spanish-only) as predictors and controlled for English word reading and grade level, but they differed in their separate attention to either conceptual (Model 1) or total (Model 2) expressive vocabulary. This approach allowed us to contrast the predictive roles of the two bilingually scored approaches. English-only expressive vocabulary positively and significantly predicted English RC, for both the conceptual (β = .18, p = .02) and total vocabulary models (β = .17, p = .03), while Spanish-only expressive vocabulary did not.

Table 4. OLS regression models comparing expressive conceptual and expressive total vocabulary as predictors of English reading comprehension, accounting for monolingually scored vocabulary, English word reading, and grade level

Note. N = 60 using multiple imputation. OLS = Ordinary least squares. Standard error is in parentheses.

* p < .05.

** p < .001.

Interestingly, only total expressive vocabulary emerged as significant (β = .21, p = .03)—although with a small effect size—above the influence of English-only expressive vocabulary while conceptual expressive did not (see Table 4, Model 2). Across all models (in both Tables 3 and 4), the R 2 values ranged from .73 to .79, which indicates that 73–79% of the variance in DLLs’ English RC were explained by vocabulary, English word reading, and grade level. Furthermore, including total expressive vocabulary while controlling for English-only expressive vocabulary did not significantly explain additional variance. However, given that the total expressive vocabulary and English-only expressive vocabulary were moderately correlated (r = .55) and that total vocabulary does tap English vocabulary knowledge, this was not unexpected. Despite the lack of additional variance explained by total expressive vocabulary (Model 2 in Table 4), the results—total expressive vocabulary and English-only expressive vocabulary predicting English RC—are noteworthy given our small sample of participants.

Discussion

The large and growing representation of Spanish-English DLLs in schools across the nation underscores the need for accurate understanding of DLLs’ language and reading skills. We compared various conceptualizations of school-age Spanish-English DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge—attending to current understandings of the bilingual mental lexicon based on Dong and colleagues’ (Reference Dong, Gui and MacWhinney2005) shared (distributed) asymmetrical model—and explored the utility of bilingually scored vocabulary methods as predictors of English RC within the SVR framework (Gough & Tunmer, Reference Gough and Tunmer1986). Two key findings emerged. Aligned with growing research evidence (e.g., Goodrich & Namkung, Reference Goodrich and Namkung2019; Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, McClain, Oh and Flores2020), the expressive vocabulary domain emerged as a significant predictor of DLLs’ English RC. More uniquely, bilingually scored vocabulary indeed contributed to English RC. We discuss our findings from theoretical and practical perspectives below.

Bilingual vocabulary assessment approaches have theoretical and practical value

Our results show that bilingual approaches to vocabulary assessment provide a more comprehensive understanding of DLLs’ language skills, compared to monolingual approaches that remain ubiquitous in U.S. schools. Thus, our findings support the tenets of the shared (distributed) asymmetrical model (Dong et al., Reference Dong, Gui and MacWhinney2005), which suggests both shared storage for conceptual representations and asymmetrical links. Our results build on previous work that reveals school-age Spanish-English DLLs’ varied response patterns to vocabulary items (e.g., some vocabulary knowledge is shared between Spanish and English while some vocabulary knowledge is unique in each language) depending on whether monolingual or bilingual-scored approaches are used (Oh & Mancilla-Martinez, Reference Oh and Mancilla-Martinez2021). In fact, to a similar extent as English-only vocabulary scores, bilingually scored vocabulary scores were positive and significant predictors of English RC. This finding is noteworthy and supports theoretically based rationale for utilizing bilingually scored vocabulary assessment approaches with DLLs.

Results based on bilingual scoring of vocabulary knowledge reveal that the linguistic knowledge of DLLs in our study is on par with, and even above, national norms (see Table 2). These results noticeably differ from findings based on monolingual vocabulary measures (i.e., English-only and Spanish-only). The distinct vocabulary profiles from comparing DLLs’ monolingually scored versus bilingually scored vocabulary underscore the promise of the latter to describe DLLs’ vocabulary skills in a more comprehensive and accurate way. This information, in and of itself, can pave the way for instructional efforts aimed at preventing the watering-down of curriculum, which risks overly simplifying classroom content for DLLs (Kibler et al., Reference Kibler, Walqui and Bunch2015). In other words, DLLs’ academic capabilities might be underestimated if educators only have access to results from monolingual vocabulary measures. This may lead to reliance on simplified texts with high-frequency vocabulary words and instructional focus on basic English skills regardless of DLLs’ comprehensive vocabulary knowledge (as indicated by conceptual or total vocabulary results) and learning potential.

When working with DLLs, educators must be cognizant of the fact that reliance on monolingual measures will likely reveal only partial information of DLLs’ linguistic abilities. However, this does not imply that the instructional language must be changed and such a recommendation is beyond the scope of this study. The predominant profile of the U.S. teacher workforce remains English monolingual (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Garcia, Connally, Cook and Dancy2016), but our results do suggest the value of accounting for DLLs’ linguistic resources in both their home language and English, if feasible. At minimum, our results point to the importance of acknowledging the limitations of relying on monolingual measures. Teachers, including English monolingual teachers, can nonetheless facilitate Spanish-English DLLs’ word knowledge by providing English labels for known concepts in the home language, ensuring adequate instructional scaffolding (e.g., modeling language), or engaging students in metalinguistic activities that highlight similarities and/or differences between English and Spanish.

We also note that, although DLLs in this study were developing both English and Spanish, their performance on the Spanish-only vocabulary assessments suggested that their Spanish skills were not at age-based norms. This finding may be partially attributable to their English-only instructional environment, a context that may have limited the extent to which their Spanish skills could influence their English RC outcomes (Proctor et al., Reference Proctor, Carlo, August and Snow2005). Indeed, DLLs’ Spanish-only receptive and expressive vocabulary skills did not predict English RC. This finding not only converges with previous evidence on the inconclusive influence of Spanish-only vocabulary on Spanish-English DLLs’ English outcomes (e.g., Mancilla-Martinez & Lesaux, Reference Mancilla-Martinez and Lesaux2017; Proctor et al., Reference Proctor, Silverman, Harring and Montecillo2012) but also underscores the value of bilingually scored assessments. That is, reliance on Spanish-only assessments might have led to the erroneous conclusion that it is unnecessary to account for Spanish-English DLLs’ home language. As DLLs’ performance on the bilingually driven vocabulary assessments revealed, this was not the case. An important consideration is that Spanish-only vocabulary assessments are designed for and normed on Spanish monolinguals, while Spanish-English DLLs in the U.S. are not Spanish monolinguals. The bilingually scored measures we used, in contrast, were designed for and normed on Spanish-English bilinguals in the U.S.

Expressive vocabulary as a predictor of English reading comprehension

Consistent with the SVR framework (Gough & Tunmer, Reference Gough and Tunmer1986), both vocabulary and word reading predicted school-age Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC. While DLLs’ vocabulary knowledge was the focus of our study, as expected, English word reading emerged as a robust predictor of English RC (Mancilla-Martinez & Lesaux, Reference Mancilla-Martinez and Lesaux2017; Vellutino et al., Reference Vellutino, Tunmer, Jaccard and Chen2007). For vocabulary knowledge, we tested whether one or both vocabulary domains (i.e., receptive, expressive) predict DLLs’ English RC. In contrast to previous studies that have found both domains to be predictive (e.g., Catts et al., Reference Catts, Nielsen, Bridges and Liu2014; Foorman et al., Reference Foorman, Petscher and Herrera2018; Ouellette, Reference Ouellette2006), we found a differential influence that has implications for informing future studies that seek parsimonious models of English RC for DLLs. Our sample of DLLs performed better receptively than expressively on all vocabulary measures, a finding that is consistent with a recent longitudinal study (Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and Pokowitz2020). However, only expressive vocabulary (except for Spanish-only vocabulary) emerged as a significant predictor of DLLs’ English RC, similar to recent research on the predictive role of expressive vocabulary on Spanish-English DLLs’ language and literacy outcomes (Mancilla-Martinez et al., Reference Mancilla-Martinez, Hwang, Oh and Pokowitz2020; Ribot et al., Reference Ribot, Hoff and Burridge2018). We speculate that when receptive vocabulary knowledge is in competition with expressive vocabulary knowledge in predicting English RC, the former may become a weaker predictor. Expressive vocabulary knowledge requires students to retrieve semantic knowledge (i.e., by accessing the meaning of the target word) and phonological knowledge (i.e., by pronouncing the retrieved target word), which are two elements that have long been identified as key components of word learning and significant predictors of language and literacy outcomes (Carlo et al., Reference Carlo, August, McLaughlin, Snow, Dressler, Lippman, Lively and White2004; Wise et al., Reference Wise, Sevcik, Morris, Lovett and Wolf2007). Existing evidence likewise points to a differential influence of receptive and expressive vocabulary on reading skills (Wise et al., Reference Wise, Sevcik, Morris, Lovett and Wolf2007). Thus, attending to DLLs’ expressive vocabulary may have practical value given the time constraints of testing in real school settings and its strong relation to English RC. The prominent role of expressive vocabulary knowledge on DLLs’ English RC—despite relatively lower scores in expressive vocabulary than receptive vocabulary—underscores the importance for DLLs to have ample explicit opportunities to use language in the classroom.

Finally, our findings based on use of bilingually scored expressive vocabulary knowledge contribute to and expand how the field might conceptualize the language comprehension component of the SVR framework (Hoover & Gough, Reference Hoover and Gough1990) for DLL students. The seminal study on the SVR by Hoover and Gough was effectively conducted with a sample of elementary-age Spanish-English DLLs in Texas, which is a lesser-known fact about the beginnings of the SVR framework. Our findings not only support the validity of the SVR as a model for understanding RC contributors among DLLs but also specify how the language comprehension component within the SVR framework could be assessed and conceptualized for this growing student population.

Given previous findings on the predictive role of conceptual vocabulary on Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC (e.g., Hwang et al., Reference Hwang, Mancilla-Martinez, McClain, Oh and Flores2020), it was somewhat unexpected to find that expressive conceptual vocabulary was not a significant predictor of English RC. However, expressive total vocabulary was a significant predictor, even though both bilingually scored approaches accounted for Spanish and English. To understand these intriguing results, we turn to empirical work on word learning and development and offer three potential explanations. First, while both expressive conceptual and total vocabulary reflect the concept (i.e., semantic knowledge) and form (i.e., phonological knowledge) of a given word, expressive total vocabulary does so to a greater extent, by prompting students in both languages for every item of the bilingually normed assessments. Because expressive total vocabulary taps the ability to access both semantic knowledge and phonological representations to a greater extent than conceptual total vocabulary does, it may be more strongly related to English RC achievement. Second, the predictive role of expressive total vocabulary highlights the link between word retrieval (i.e., production) and lexical quality representation. Existing research suggests that efficient word retrieval (i.e., skilled ability to produce words in response during an assessment) might be associated with a higher quality of linguistic representations that learners have (Newman & German, Reference Newman and German2002; Ouellette, Reference Ouellette2006). Likewise, Oller et al. (Reference Oller, Pearson and Cobo-Lewis2007) describe receptive vocabulary as a passive skill compared to expressive vocabulary, given that expressive vocabulary knowledge places an increased demand to not only comprehend a word but to comprehend and retrieve it during the assessment. Theoretical evidence similarly links language production (i.e., expressive domain) to language knowledge construction and consequently RC (Swain, Reference Swain, Gass and Madden1985). This relation between word production to overall language knowledge leads to the last explanation for our findings. Given that vocabulary development involves conceptual development that builds and expands background knowledge (Mancilla-Martinez & McClain, Reference Mancilla-Martinez, McClain, Moje, Afflerbach, Enciso and Lesaux2018), RC research finds vocabulary knowledge as an adequate proxy for readers’ background knowledge (Perfetti, Reference Perfetti, Reitsma and Verhoeven1998), another well-documented predictor of RC (Kintsch & van Dijk, Reference Kintsch and van Dijk1978). Therefore, it is possible that expressive total vocabulary elicits even more background knowledge (i.e., by prompting in both languages for every item) compared to expressive conceptual vocabulary, thus providing a more comprehensive reflection of DLLs’ language knowledge.

Limitations and future directions

There are limitations of this study worth noting. First, given the exploratory nature of the study, our sample size was small (N = 60) and bilingual vocabulary scoring was limited to a sample of Spanish-English DLLs using the ROWPVT-4: SBE and the EOWPVT-4: SBE. Due to the small sample size, we could not test for interactions with English word reading and vocabulary knowledge—as hypothesized under the SVR (Gough & Tunmer, Reference Gough and Tunmer1986). Although we did control for grade level to account for potential developmental differences, a larger sample would help detect more detailed effects given the developmental realities of the student population. Hence, our findings on the predictive role of expressive vocabulary on English RC should be interpreted with recognition of these limitations. Our participants were also heterogeneous in that they were from different grade levels, had varying levels of English proficiency (e.g., some were identified as English learners, some were reclassified as being English-proficient), and a small percentage of our participants were receiving special education services. Hence, our findings and practical implications should be interpreted with caution, and this study would need to be replicated with larger samples of DLLs.

Despite limitations regarding our sample, we were able to examine the differential roles of various vocabulary conceptualizations on Spanish-English DLLs’ English RC. Our findings suggest that the use of bilingual scoring can be beneficial for understanding DLLs’ linguistic knowledge. Second, our study participants were all recruited from English-only instruction public schools. This likely influenced our results, particularly on students’ Spanish-only vocabulary scores and their relation to English RC. Future studies that include a more diverse sample of Spanish-English DLLs in different instructional contexts (e.g., bilingual programs, dual language immersion programs) would be needed to better understand the predictors of English RC for this student population. Third, we employed total scoring using standardized conceptual vocabulary measures as, to our knowledge, there is no standardized vocabulary measure that can be used to assess Spanish-English DLLs’ total vocabulary knowledge. Therefore, standard or scaled scores that would allow us to compare our participants’ scores to the national norm were not available. Designing and developing a systematic assessment to measure DLLs’ bilingual vocabulary knowledge comprehensively and equitably is warranted. This would allow researchers and educators to examine DLLs’ total vocabulary knowledge and track lexical growth of DLLs in a way that is less limited by relying on monolingual or conceptual vocabulary assessments.

Despite these limitations, our study contributes to the limited body of research on Spanish-English DLLs in new immigration destination states. Given that school is one of the primary sources of English linguistic input for DLLs (Bowers & Vasilyeva, Reference Bowers and Vasilyeva2011), our findings underscore the importance of linguistically rich instruction (e.g., opportunities to use language). Finally, results also demonstrate the benefits of bilingual scoring. It helps capture DLLs’ comprehensive linguistic knowledge that could be used in classroom instruction and to build asset-driven perspectives towards helping DLLs grow into successful and independent readers.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare none.

References

Anaya, J. B., Peña, E. D., & Bedore, L. M. (2018). Conceptual scoring and classification accuracy of vocabulary testing in bilingual children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 8597. https://doi.org/10.1044/2017_LSHSS-16-0081 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arias, G., & Friberg, J. (2017). Bilingual language assessment: Contemporary versus recommended practice in American schools. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 48, 115. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_LSHSS-15-0900 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary development for English language learners. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21(1), 5057. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2005.00120.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Garcia, M., & Cortez, C. (2005). Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 188200. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2005/020)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, E. P., & Vasilyeva, M. (2011). The relation between teacher input and lexical growth of preschoolers. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 221241. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000354 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braze, D., Katz, L., Magnuson, J. S., Mencl, W. E., Tabor, W., Van Dyke, J. A., Gong, T., Johns, C. L., & Shankweiler, D. P. (2016). Vocabulary does not complicate the simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 29, 435451. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9608-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caesar, L. G., & Kohler, P. D. (2007). The state of school-based bilingual assessment: Actual practice versus recommended guidelines. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(3), 190200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cárdenas-Hagan, E., Carlson, C. D., & Pollard-Durodola, S. D. (2007). The cross-linguistic transfer of early literacy skills: The role of initial L1 and L2 skills and language of instruction. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 249259. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/026) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D. M., Lively, T. J., & White, C. E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(2), 188215. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.39.2.3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, R. P., & David, A. H. (2009). Investigating reading achievement using a causal model. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(2), 107140. https://doi.org/10.1207/S1532799Xssr0502_1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Herrera, S., Nielsen, D. C., & Bridges, M. S. (2015). Early prediction of reading comprehension within the simple view framework. Reading and Writing, 28, 14071425. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9576-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W., Nielsen, D. C., Bridges, M. S., & Liu, Y. (2014). Early identification of reading comprehension difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 115. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219414556121 Google ScholarPubMed
Cho, E., Capin, P., Roberts, G., Roberts, G. J., & Vaughn, S. (2019). Examining sources and mechanisms of reading comprehension difficulties: Comparing English learners and non-English learners within the simple view of reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 111(6), 9821000. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000332 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Core, C., Hoff, E., Rumiche, R., & Señor, M. (2013). Total and conceptual vocabulary in Spanish-English bilinguals from 22 to 30 months: Implications for assessment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 5. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/11-0044)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Putnik, D. L. (2014). A bilingual-monolingual comparison of young children’s vocabulary size: Evidence from comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 11891211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000744 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dong, Y., Gui, S., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Shared and separate meanings in the bilingual mental lexicon. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 8, 221238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L. (1986). Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody. American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test–4. NCS Pearson.Google Scholar
Durgunoğlu, Y., Nagy, W. & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Crosslanguage transfer of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 453465. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.3.453 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehl, B., Bruns, G., & Brosche, M. (2020). Differentiated bilingual vocabulary assessment reveals similarities and differences compared to monolinguals: Conceptual versus single-language scoring and the relation with home language and literacy activities. International Journal of Bilingualism, 24(4), 715728. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006919876994 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Florit, E., & Cain, K. (2011). The simple view of reading: Is it valid for different types of alphabetic orthographies? Educational Psychology Review, 23(4), 553576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-011-9175-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foorman, B., Petscher, Y., & Herrera, S. (2018). Unique and common effects of decoding and language factors in predicting reading comprehension in grades 1-10. Learning and Individual Differences, 63, 1223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2018.02.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gándara, P., & Mordechay, K. (2017). Demographic change and the new (and not so new) challenges for Latino education. The Educational Forum, 81(2), 148159. https://doi.org/10.1080/00131725.2017.1280755 Google Scholar
García, E. E. (2012). Language, culture, and early education in the United States. In Pianta, R. C., Barnett, W. S., Justice, L. M., & Sheridan, S. M. (Eds.). Handbook of early childhood education (pp. 137157). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gennetian, L., Guzman, L., Ramos-Olazagasti, M., & Wildsmith, E. (2019). An economic portrait of low-income Hispanic families: Key findings from the first five years of studies from the national research center on Hispanic children & families. National Research Center on Hispanic Children and Families. https://www.hispanicresearchcenter.org/research-resources/an-economic-portrait-of-low-income-hispanic-families-key-findings-from-the-first-five-years-of-studies-from-the-national-research-center-on-hispanic-children-families Google Scholar
Goodrich, J. M., & Namkung, J. M. (2019). Correlates of reading comprehension and word-problem solving skills of Spanish-speaking dual language learners. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48, 256266. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.04.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottardo, A., & Mueller, J. (2009). Are first- and second-language factors related in predicting second-language reading comprehension? A study of Spanish-speaking children acquiring English as a second language from first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 330344. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014320 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gough, P., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 610. https://doi.org/10.1177/074193258600700104 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grosjean, F. (1989). Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and Language, 36, 315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, M., Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2014). Conceptual scoring of receptive and expressive vocabulary measures in simultaneous and sequential bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23, 574586. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, C. S., Hoff, E., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D., & Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 29, 715733. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.05.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heidlage, J. K., Cunningham, J. E., Kaiser, A. P., Trivette, C. M., Barton, E. E., Frey, J. R., & Roberts, M. Y. (2020). The effects of parent-implemented language interventions on child linguistic outcomes: A meta-analysis. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50(1), 623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoff, E. (2018). Lessons from the study of input effects on bilingual development. The International Journal of Bilingualism. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006918768370 Google Scholar
Hoff, E., & Core, C. (2015). What clinicians need to know about bilingual development. Seminars in Speech and Language, 36(2), 8999. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0035-1549104 Google ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoover, W. A., Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2, 127160. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00401799 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hwang, J. K., Mancilla-Martinez, J., McClain, J. B., Oh, M., & Flores, I. (2020). Spanish-speaking English learners’ English language and literacy skills: The predictive role of conceptually-scored vocabulary. Applied Psycholinguistics, 41(1), 124. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716419000365 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21(2), 8597. https://doi.org/10.1080/02702710050084428 Google Scholar
Kendeou, P., Papadopoulos, T.C., & Kotzapoulou, M. (2013). Evidence for the early emergence of the simple view of reading in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 26, 189204. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9361-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kern, S., Valente, D., & dos Santos, C. (2019). Lexical development in bilingual French/Portuguese speaking toddlers: Vocabulary size and language dominance. Journal of Monolingual and Bilingual Speech, 1.2, 206224. https://doi.org/10.1558/jmbs.v1i2.11880 Google Scholar
Kibler, A. K., Walqui, A., & Bunch, G. C. (2015). Transformational opportunities: Language and literacy instruction for English language learners in the common core era in the United States. TESOL Journal, 6(1), 935. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesj.133 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kids Count Data Center. (2020). Children who speak a language other than English at home. https://datacenter.kidscount.org/data/tables/81-children-who-speak-a-language-other-than-english-at-home#detailed/1/any/false/1729,37,871,870,573,869,36,868,867,133/any/396,397Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Towards a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42(2), 7582. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S. J. & Hawkins, M. R. (2015). Policy, context and schooling: The education of English learners in rural new destinations. Global Education Review, 2(4), 4059.Google Scholar
Legacy, J., Zesiger, P., Friend, M., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). Vocabulary size, translation equivalents, and efficiency in word recognition in very young bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 43, 760783. https://doi.org/10.1017/S03050000915000252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesaux, N. K., Crosson, A. C., Kieffer, M. J., Pierce, M. (2010). Uneven profiles: Language minority learners’ word reading, vocabulary, and reading comprehension skills. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 31(6), 475483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2010.09.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Schatschneider, C. (2018). Examining the simple view of reading with elementary school children: Still simple after all these years. Remedial and Special Education, 39(5), 260273. https://doi.org/10.1177/0741932518764833 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez, M. H., & Velasco, G. (2011). The toll of the great recession: Childhood poverty among Hispanics sets record, leads nation. Pew Research Center. https://assets.pewresearch.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/7/2011/10/147.pdf Google Scholar
Lowenhaupt, R., & Reeves, T. (2015). Toward a theory of school capacity in new immigrant destinations: Instructional and organizational considerations. Leadership and Policy in Schools, 14(3), 308340. https://doi.org/10.1080/15700763.2015.1021052 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludwig-Mayerhofer, W. (2020). Winsorizing and trimming. STATA Guide. https://wlm.userweb.mwn.de/Stata/wstatwin.htm Google Scholar
Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2020). Children’s literacy experiences in low-income families: The content of books matters. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(2), 213233. https://doi.org/10.1002/rr1.263 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J. (2020). Understanding and supporting literacy development among English learners: A deep dive into the role of language comprehension. AERA Open, 6(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858420912198 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., Hwang, J. K., Oh, M., & McClain, J. B. (2020). Early elementary grade dual language learners from Spanish-speaking homes struggling with English reading comprehension: The Dormant role of language skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112(5), 880894. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000402 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancilla-Martinez, J., Hwang, J. K., Oh, M., & Pokowitz, E. L. (2020). Patterns of development in Spanish-English conceptually-scored vocabulary among elementary-age dual language learners. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(9), 30843099. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20_00056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2017). Early indicators of later English reading comprehension outcomes among Children From Spanish-Speaking homes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 21(5), 428448. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2017.1320402 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & McClain, J. (2018). What do we know today about the complexity of vocabulary gaps and what do we not know? In Moje, E. B., Afflerbach, P., Enciso, P., & Lesaux, N. K. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (5th ed., pp. 216236). Abington, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., Pan, B. A., & Vagh, S. B. (2011). Assessing the productive vocabulary of Spanish–English bilingual toddlers from low-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32(3), 333357. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000433 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Vagh, S. B. (2013). Growth in toddlers’ Spanish, English, and conceptual vocabulary knowledge. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 555567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.03.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Fernald, A., & Hurtado, N. (2010). How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish-English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 817840. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990055 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, N. (2013a). Receptive one-word picture vocabulary-4: Spanish-Bilingual edition (ROWPVT-4: SBE). Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
Martin, N. (2013b). Expressive one-word picture vocabulary-4: Spanish-Bilingual Edition (EOWPVT-4: SBE). Academic Therapy Publications.Google Scholar
McGregor, K. K., Friedman, R. M., Reilly, R. M., & Newman, R. M. (2002). Semantic representation and naming in young children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45(2), 332346. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/026) CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A. (2011). Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Journal of Research in Reading, 34, 114135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2010.01477.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamoto, J., Lindsey, K. A., & Manis, F. R. (2008). A cross-linguistic investigation of English language learners’ reading comprehension in English and Spanish. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 351371. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430802378526 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, R. S., & German, D. J. (2002). Effects of lexical factors on lexical access among typical language-learning children and children with word finding difficulties. Language and Speech, 45, 285317. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309020450030401 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
The Northwest Evaluation Association. (2011). Technical manual: For Measures of Academic Progress (MAP) and Measures of Academic Progress for Primary Grades (MPG). https://www.richland2.org/RichlandDistrict/media/Richland-District/AdvancED/Standard%205/5.1/5-1-NWEA-Technical-Manual-for-MAP-and-MPG.pdf Google Scholar
The Northwest Evaluation Association. (2020). 2020 NWEA MAP Growth normative data overview. https://teach.mapnwea.org/impl/MAPGrowthNormativeDataOverview.pdf Google Scholar
Oh, M., & Mancilla-Martinez, J. (2021). Comparing vocabulary knowledge conceptualizations among Spanish-English dual language learners in a new destination state. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 52, 1. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_LSHSS-20-00031 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., & Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Assessing the effects of bilingualism: A background. In Oller, D. K. & Eilers, R. E. (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 321). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oller, D. K., Pearson, B. Z., & Cobo-Lewis, A. B. (2007). Profile effects in early bilingual language and literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 191230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Toole, C., Gatt, D., Hickey, T. M., Miękisz, A., Haman, E., Armon-Lotem, S., Rinker, T., Ohana, O., dos Santos, C., & Kern, S. (2017). Parent report of early lexical production in bilingual children: A cross-linguistic CDI comparison. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 20(2), 124245. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1179258 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(3), 554566. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.3.554 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z. (1998). Assessing lexical development in bilingual babies and toddlers. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 2, 347372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43(1), 93120. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K. (1995). Cross-language synonyms in the lexicons of bilingual infants: One language or two? Journal of Child Language, 22, 345368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Kester, E. S. (2015). Discriminant accuracy of a semantics measure with Latino English-speaking, Spanish-speaking, and English-Spanish bilingual children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 53, 3041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peña, E. D., & Halle, T. G. (2011). Assessing preschool dual language learners: Traveling a multiforked road. Child Development Perspectives, 5(1), 2832. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00143.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1998). Two basic questions about reading and learning to read. In Reitsma, P. & Verhoeven, L. (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 1548). Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 2237. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2013.827687 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(1), 5770. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911431198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M. S., & Snow, C. (2006). The intriguing role of Spanish language vocabulary knowledge in predicting English reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(1), 159169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.159 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 246256. https://doi.org./10.1037/0022-0663.97/2.246 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Silverman, R. D., Harring, J. R., & Montecillo, C. (2012). The role of vocabulary depth in predicting reading comprehension among English monolingual and Spanish–English bilingual children in elementary school. Reading and Writing, 25, 16351664. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9336-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Proctor, C. P., Uccelli, P., Dalton, B., & Snow, C. E. (2009). Understanding depth of vocabulary online with bilingual and monolingual children. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 25(4), 311333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. RAND.Google Scholar
Ribot, K. M., Hoff, E., & Burridge, A. (2018). Language use contributes to expressive language growth: Evidence from bilingual children. Child Development, 89(3), 929940. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12770 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverman, R. B., Johnson, E., Keane, K., & Khanna, S. (2020). Beyond decoding: A meta-analysis of the effects of language comprehension interventions on K-5 students’ language and literacy outcomes. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(S1), S207S233. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.346 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smolarek, B. B. (2020). Feeling the stress and strain—Race, economics, and the educational experiences of Latinx emergent bilinguals in a ‘new’ destination schools. Race Ethnicity and Education, 23(6), 800819. https://doi.org/10.1080/13613324.2018.1511534 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. E., & Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Large problem spaces: The challenge of vocabulary for English language learners . In Wagner, R. K., Muse, A. E., & Tannenbaum, K. R. (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 123139). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(6), 13211337. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/103).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugarman, J., & Villegas, L. (2020). Native language assessments for K-12 English learners: Policy considerations and state practices. Migration Policy Institute. https://www.migrationpolicy.org/research/native-language-assessments-english-learners Google Scholar
Swain, M. (1985). Communicative competence: Some roles of comprehensible input and comprehensible output in its development. In Gass, S. & Madden, C. (Eds.), Input in second language acquisition (pp. 165179). Newbury House.Google Scholar
Thum, Y. M., & Kuhfeld, M. (2020). NWEA 2020 MAP Growth Achievement Status and Growth Norms for Students and Schools. NWEA Research Report. https://teach.mapnwea.org/impl/normsResearchStudy.pdf Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2012). Test of word reading efficiency—Second Edition (TOWRE-2). Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2012). The simple view of reading Redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 45(5), 453466. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219311432685 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umansky, I., Hopkins, M., Dabach, D. B., Porter, L., Thompson, K., & Pompa, D. (2018). Understanding and supporting the educational needs of recently arrived immigrant English learner students: Lessons for state and local education agencies. Council of Chief State School Officers. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED586975.pdf Google Scholar
van Steensel, R., Oostdam, R., van Gelderen, A., & van Schooten, E. (2016). The role of word decoding, vocabulary knowledge and meta-cognitive knowledge in monolingual and bilingual low-achieving adolescents’ reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 39(3), 312329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. S. (2007) Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(1), 332. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888430709336632 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, C. P., Garcia, A., Connally, K., Cook, S., & Dancy, K. (2016). Pays to prosperity: Growing multilingual teacher workforce: Multilingual paraprofessionals, an untapped resource for supporting American pluralism. https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED570876 Google Scholar
Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007). The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 10931109. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/076)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodcock, R. W., Alvarado, C. G., & Ruef, M. L. (2017). Woodcock-Muñoz language survey III. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
World Literacy Foundation. (2018). The economic & social cost of illiteracy: A white paper by the World Literacy Foundation. https://worldliteracyfoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/06/TheEconomicSocialCostofIlliteracy-2.pdf Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Correlations among English reading comprehension, vocabulary conceptualizations, and English word reading

Figure 1

Table 2. Sample means on English reading comprehension, vocabulary conceptualizations, and English word reading by grade level

Figure 2

Table 3. OLS regression models predicting English reading comprehension by monolingually scored versus bilingually scored receptive and expressive vocabulary knowledge, accounting for English word reading and grade level

Figure 3

Table 4. OLS regression models comparing expressive conceptual and expressive total vocabulary as predictors of English reading comprehension, accounting for monolingually scored vocabulary, English word reading, and grade level