Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T05:01:54.835Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Knowledge of context sensitive spellings as a component of spelling competence: Evidence from Danish

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2005

HOLGER JUUL
Affiliation:
University of Copenhagen

Abstract

Spelling performances in 104 Danish children from Grades 4 to 6 were compared across three levels of orthographic transparency. At the first level all spellings that were plausible at the level of the single phoneme were accepted. At the second level spellings were accepted only if they were plausible when the phonological context was considered (context sensitive spellings). At the third level word-specific spelling accuracy was required. There were 16 word items per level, matched for structure and frequency. Scores for context sensitive spellings were intermediate between scores at the phonemic level and the word-specific level, both for vowel and consonant spellings. Scores for context sensitive vowels and consonants were significantly interrelated even when performances at the phonemic and word-specific levels were controlled. The results demonstrate that Danish spellers beyond the initial phases of literacy development rely on phonological entities larger than the single phoneme. They extend similar findings from English in suggesting that knowledge of context sensitive spellings is a separate component of spelling competence.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2005 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aronoff M., & Koch E. 1996. Context-sensitive regularities in English vowel spelling. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 251265.Google Scholar
Barry C. 1994. Spelling routes (or roots of rutes). In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 2749). Chichester: Wiley.
Basbøll H., & Wagner J. 1985. Kontrastive phonologie des Deutschen und Dänischen. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.
Becker–Christensen C. 1988. Bogstav og lyd. Dansk retskrivning og rigsmålsudtale [Letter and sound. Danish orthography and standard pronunciation]. Copenhagen: Gyldendal.
Brown G. D. A., & Loosemore R. P. W. 1994. Computational approaches to normal and impaired spelling. In G. D. A. Brown & N. C. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of spelling: Theory, process and intervention (pp. 319335). Chichester: Wiley.
Bruck M., & Waters G. 1988. An analysis of the spelling errors of children who differ in their reading and spelling skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 7792.Google Scholar
Campbell R. 1983. Writing nonwords to dictation. Brain and Language, 19, 153178.Google Scholar
Caravolas M., Bruck M., & Genesee F. 2003. Similarities and differences between English- and French-speaking poor spellers. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages. London: Whurr.
Caravolas M., Hulme C., & Snowling M. J. 2001. The foundations of spelling ability: Evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 751774.Google Scholar
Carlisle J. F. 1991. Questioning the psychological reality of onset-rime as a level of phonological awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carney E. 1994. A survey of English spelling. London: Routledge.
Cassar M., & Treiman R. 1997. The beginnings of orthographic knowledge: Children's knowledge of double letters in words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 631644.Google Scholar
Fischer F. W., Shankweiler D., & Liberman I. Y. 1985. Spelling proficiency and sensitivity to word structure. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 423441.Google Scholar
Goswami U. 1988. Children's use of analogy in learning to spell. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 2133.Google Scholar
Goswami U., & East M. 2000. Rhyme and analogy in beginning reading: Conceptual and methodological issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 6393.Google Scholar
Goswami U., Ziegler J. C., Dalton L., & Schneider W. 2003. Nonword reading across orthographies: How flexible is the choice of reading units? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 235247.Google Scholar
Gough P. B., Juel C., & Griffith P. L. 1992. Reading, spelling and the orthographic cipher. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 3548). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kessler B., & Treiman R. 2001. Relationships between sounds and letters in English monosyllables. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 592617.Google Scholar
Kreiner D. S., & Gough P. B. 1990. Two ideas about spelling: Rules and word-specific memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 103118.Google Scholar
Lennox C., & Siegel L. S. 1996. The development of phonological rules and visual strategies in average and poor spellers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 6083.Google Scholar
Marsh G., Friedman M., Welch V., & Desberg P. 1980. The development of strategies in spelling. In U. Frith (Ed.), Cognitive processes in spelling (pp. 339353). London: Academic Press.
Martensen H., Maris E., & Dijkstra T. 2000. When does inconsistency hurt? On the relation between phonological consistency effects and the reliability of sublexical units. Memory & Cognition, 28, 648656.Google Scholar
Nation K. 1997. Children's sensitivity to rime unit frequency when spelling words and nonwords. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 321338.Google Scholar
Notenboom A., & Reitsma P. (in press). Investigating the dimensions of spelling ability. Educational and Psychological Measurements.
Perry C., Ziegler J. C., & Coltheart M. 2002. A dissociation between orthographic awareness and spelling production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 4373.Google Scholar
Sampson G. 1985. Writing systems. London: Hutchinson.
Steffler D. J. 2001. Implicit cognition and spelling development. Developmental Review, 21, 168204.Google Scholar
Søegård A., & Petersen P. B. 1968. OS 400 Ordstillelæsningssprøve. Copenhagen: Dansk Psykologisk Forlag.
Treiman R., Kessler B., & Bick S. 2002. Context sensitivity in the spelling of English vowels. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 448468.Google Scholar
Treiman R., Kessler B., & Bick S. 2003. Influence of consonantal context on the pronunciation of vowels: A comparison of human readers and computational models. Cognition, 88, 4978.Google Scholar
Treiman R., Mullennix J., Bijeljac–Babic R., & Richmond–Welty E. D. 1995. The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107136.Google Scholar
Treiman R., & Zukowski A. 1988. Units in reading and spelling. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 466477.Google Scholar
Varnhagen C. K. 2003, June. Brain activation during spelling. Paper presented at the SSSR Conference, Boulder, CO.
Varnhagen C. K., Boechler P. M., & Steffler D. J. 1999. Phonological and orthographic influences on children's vowel spelling. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 363379.Google Scholar
Varnhagen C. K., McCallum M., & Burstow M. 1997. Is children's spelling naturally stage-like? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 9, 451481.Google Scholar
Venezky R. L. 1970. The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton.
Zinna D. R., Liberman I. Y., & Shankweiler D. 1986. Children's sensitivity to factors influencing vowel reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 465480.Google Scholar