Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T14:32:33.716Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Song development in birds: the role of early experience and its potential effect on rehabilitation success

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2023

KA Spencer*
Affiliation:
Division of Environmental and Evolutionary Biology, Graham Kerr Building, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8QQ, Scotland
S Harris
Affiliation:
School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1UG, UK
PJ Baker
Affiliation:
School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1UG, UK
IC Cuthill
Affiliation:
School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol, BS8 1UG, UK
*
* Contact for correspondence and request for reprints: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Environmental conditions during the early life stages of birds can have significant effects on the quality of sexual signals in adulthood, especially song, and these ultimately have consequences for breeding success and fitness. This has wide-ranging implications for the rehabilitation protocols undertaken in wildlife hospitals which aim to return captive-reared animals to their natural habitat. Here we review the current literature on bird song development and learning in order to determine the potential impact that the rearing of juvenile songbirds in captivity can have on rehabilitation success. We quantify the effects of reduced learning on song structure and relate this to the possible effects on an individual's ability to defend a territory or attract a mate. We show the importance of providing a conspecific auditory model for birds to learn from in the early stages post-fledging, either via live- or tape-tutoring and provide suggestions for tutoring regimes. We also highlight the historical focus on learning in a few model species that has left an information gap in our knowledge for most species reared at wildlife hospitals.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

References

Adret, P 1993 Operant conditioning, song learning and imprinting to taped song in the zebra finch. Animal Behaviour 46: 149159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adret, P 2004 Vocal imitation in blindfolded zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) is facilitated in the presence of a non-singing conspecific female. Journal of Ethology 22: 2935CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adret-Hausberger, M, Guttinger, H-R and Merkel, FW 1990 Individual life history and song repertoire changes in a colony of starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Ethology 84: 265280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aitken, G 2004 A new approach to conservation: the importance of the individual through wildlife rehabilitation Ashgate Publishing: Aldershot, Hampshire, UKGoogle Scholar
Alvarez-Buylla, A and Kirn, JR 1997 Birth, migration, incorporation, and death of vocal control neurons in adult songbirds. Journal of Neurobiology 33: 5856013.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aubin, T and Bremond, J-C 1983 The process of species-specific song recognition in the skylark Alauda arvensis. An experimental study by means of synthesis. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 61: 141152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, MC 1986 Sexual selection and size of repertoire in songbirds. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 19: 13581365Google Scholar
Baker, MC, Bjerke, TK, Lampe, H and Espmark, Y 1986 Sexual response of female great tits Parus major to variation in size of males' song repertoires. American Naturalist 128: 491498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, MC, Bjerke, TK, Lampe, HU and Espmark, YO 1987 Sexual response of female yellowhammers to differences in regional song dialects and repertoire sizes. Animal Behaviour 35: 395401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baptista, LF 1988 Imitations of white-crowned sparrow songs by a song sparrow. Condor 90: 486489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baptista, LF and Morton, ML 1981 Interspecific song acquisition by a white-crowned sparrow. Auk 98: 383385Google Scholar
Baptista, LF and Morton, ML 1982 Song dialects and mate selection in montane white-crowned sparrows. Auk 99: 537547Google Scholar
Baptista, LF and Petrinovich, L 1984 Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song template hypothesis in the white-crowned sparrow. Animal Behaviour 32: 172181CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baptista, LF and Petrinovich, L 1986 Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: social factors and sex differences. Animal Behaviour 34: 13591371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baptista, LF, Morton, ML and Pereyra, ME 1981 Interspecific song mimesis by a Lincoln sparrow. Wilson Bulletin 93: 265267Google Scholar
Becker, PH 1976 Artkennzeichnende Gesangsmerkmale bei Winter- und Sommergoldhähnchen (Regulus regulus, R. ignicapillus). Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 42: 411437. [Title translation: Species-specific song characteristics of the goldcrest and firecrest]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, PH 1982 The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In: Kroodsma, DE and Miller, EH (eds) Acoustic communication in birds pp 213252. Academic Press: New York, USACrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beecher, MD and Brenowitz, EA 2005 Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20: 143149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Best, D 2003 Small birds. In: Mullineaux, E, Best, D and Cooper, JE (eds) BSAVA Manual of Wildlife Casualties pp 260269. British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Gloucester, UKGoogle Scholar
Böhner, J, Chaiken, ML, Ball, GF and Marler, P 1990 Song acquisition in photosensitive and photorefractory male European starlings. Hormones and Behavior 24: 582594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolhuis, JJ, Van Mil, DP and Houx, BB 1999 Song learning with audiovisual compound stimuli in zebra finches. Animal Behaviour 58: 12851292CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottjer, SW 2002 Neural strategies for learning during sensitive periods of development. Journal of Comparative Physiology 188A: 917-928Google Scholar
Bottjer, SW and Johnson, F 1997 Circuits, hormones, and learning: vocal behavior in songbirds. Journal of Neurobiology 33: 6026183.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainard, MS and Doupe, AJ 2001 Postlearning consolidation of birdsong: stabilizing effects of age and anterior forebrain lesions. Journal of Neuroscience 21: 25012517CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brainard, MS and Doupe, AJ 2002 What songbirds teach us about learning. Nature 417: 351358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bremond, J-C 1968 Recherche sur la sémantique et les éléments vecteurs d'information dans les signaux acoustiques du rougegorge Erithacus rubecula. Terre Vie 2: 109220. [Title translation: Research on the song structure and information giving elements in the acoustic signals of the robin]Google Scholar
Bremond, J-C 1976a The role of rise time in the recognition of acoustic elements in European robin's song (Erithacus rubecula). Experientia 32: 460462Google Scholar
Bremond, J-C 1976b Specific recognition in the song of Bonelli's warbler (Phylloscopus bonelli). Behaviour 58: 99116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, KL, Catchpole, CK, Lewis, JW and Lodge, A 1999 Song as an indicator of parasitism in the sedge warbler. Animal Behaviour 57: 307314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buchanan, KL, Spencer, KA, Goldsmith, AR and Catchpole, CK 2003 Song as an honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 270B: 1149-1156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butchart, SHM, Stattersfield, AJ and Collar, NJ 2006 How many bird extinctions have we prevented? Oryx 40: 266278CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK 1976 Temporal and sequential organisation of song in the sedge warbler (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus). Behaviour 59: 226246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK 1980 Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among warblers of the genus Acrocephalus. Behaviour 74: 149166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK 1983 Variation in the song of the great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) in relation to mate attraction and territorial defence. Animal Behaviour 31: 12171225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK 1986 Song repertoires and reproductive success in the great reed warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 19: 439445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK and Slater, PJB 1995 Bird song: biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK, Dittami, J and Leisler, B 1984 Differential responses to male song repertoires in female songbirds implanted with estradiol. Nature 312: 563564CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catchpole, CK, Leisler, B and Dittami, J 1986 Sexual differences in the responses of captive great reed warblers (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) to variation in song structure and size. Ethology 73: 6977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, M, Böhner, J and Marler, P 1993 Song acquisition in European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris: a comparison of the songs of live-tutored, tape-tutored, untutored, and wild-caught males. Animal Behaviour 46: 10791090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaiken, M, Böhner, J and Marler, P 1994 Repertoire turnover and the timing of song acquisition in European starlings. Behaviour 128: 2539Google Scholar
Chaiken, M, Gentner, TQ and Hulse, SH 1997 Effects of social interaction on the development of starling song and the perception of these effects by conspecifics. Journal of Comparative Psychology 111: 379392CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, NS 1987 Song learning in cross-fostered zebra finches: a re-examination of the sensitive phase. Behaviour 102: 6781CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, NS 1990 Assortative mating in zebra finch subspecies Taeniopygia guttata guttata and T.g. castonotis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London 330B: 351-370Google Scholar
Clayton, N and Prove, E 1989 Song discrimination in female zebra finches and Bengalese finches. Animal Behaviour 38: 352354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabelsteen, T and Pedersen, SB 1993 Song-based species discrimination and behavior assessment by female blackbirds, Turdus merula. Animal Behaviour 45: 759771CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dabelsteen, T, McGregor, PK, Shepherd, M, Whittaker, X and Pedersen, SB 1996 Is the signal value of overlapping different from that of alternating during matched singing in great tits? Journal of Avian Biology 27: 189194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittus, WPJ and Lemon, RE 1969 Effects of song tutoring and acoustic isolation on the song repertoires of cardinals. Animal Behaviour 17: 523533CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eales, LA 1985 Song learning in zebra finches: some effects of song model availability on what is learnt and when. Animal Behaviour 33: 12931300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eales, LA 1989 The influences of visual and vocal interaction on song learning in zebra finches. Animal Behaviour 37: 507508CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eens, M 1997 Understanding the complex song of the European starling: an integrated ethological approach. Advances in the Study of Behavior 26: 355434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eens, M, Pinxten, R and Verheyen, RF 1991 Male song as a cue for mate choice in the European starling. Behaviour 116: 210238Google Scholar
Eens, M, Pinxten, R and Verheyen, RF 1992a No overlap in song repertoire size between yearling and older starlings Sturnus vulgaris. Ibis 134: 7276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eens, M, Pinxten, R and Verheyen, RF 1992b Song learning in captive European starlings, Sturnus vulgaris. Animal Behaviour 44: 11311143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, D 1991 The significance of song for species recognition and mate choice in the pied flycatcher, Ficedula hypoleuca. PhD thesis, University of Uppsala, SwedenGoogle Scholar
Ewert, DN 1979 Development of song of a rufous-sided towhee raised in acoustic isolation. Condor 81: 313316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galeotti, P, Saino, N, Perani, E and Møller, AP 2001 Age-related song variation in male barn swallows. Italian Journal of Zoology 68: 305310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geberzahn, N and Hultsch, H 2003 Long-time storage of song types in birds: evidence from interactive playbacks. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 270B: 1085-1090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gil, D and Gahr, M 2002 The honesty of bird song: multiple constraints for multiple traits. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17: 133141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guttinger, HR 1979 Integration of learnt and genetically programmed behavior - study of hierarchical organization in songs of canaries, greenfinches and their hybrids. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 49: 285303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, WD and Zuk, M 1982 Heritable true fitness and bright birds: a role for parasites? Science 218: 384387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasselquist, D, Bensch, S and Von Schantz, T 1996 Correlation between male song repertoire, extra-pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Nature 381: 229232CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinroth, O and Heinroth, M 1924-1933 Die Vögel Mitteleuropas. Bergmühler: Berlin, Germany. [Title translation: Birds of central Europe]Google Scholar
Hiebert, SM, Stoddard, PK and Arcese, P 1989 Repertoire size, territory acquisition and reproductive success in the song sparrow. Animal Behaviour 37: 266273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hindmarsh, AM 1984 Vocal mimicry in starlings. Behaviour 90: 302324CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houx, BB and ten Cate, C 1999 Do stimulus-stimulus contingencies affect song learning in zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata)? Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 235242CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houx, BB, ten Cate, C and Feuth, E 2000 Variations in zebra finch song copying: an examination of the relationship with tutor song quality and pupil behaviour. Behaviour 137: 13771389Google Scholar
Howard, RD 1974 The influence of sexual selection and interspecific competition on mocking bird song (Mimus polyglottos). Evolution 28: 428438Google Scholar
Hultsch, H 1993 Tracing the memory mechanisms in the song acquisition of nightingales. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 43: 155171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, H and Kopp, M-L 1989 Early auditory learning and song improvisation in nightingales, Luscinia megarhynchos. Animal Behaviour 37: 510512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, H and Todt, D 1989 Song acquisition and acquisition constraints in the nightingale Luscinia megarhynchos. Naturwissenschaften 76: 8385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, H and Todt, D 1992 The serial order effect in the song acquisition of birds: relevance of exposure frequency to song models. Animal Behaviour 44: 590592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hultsch, H, Schleuss, F and Todt, D 1999 Auditory-visual stimulus pairing enhances perceptual learning in a songbird. Animal Behaviour 58: 143149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Immelmann, K 1969 Song development in the zebra finch and other estrilidid finches. In: Hinde, RA (ed) Bird vocalisations pp 6174. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
King, AP, West, MJ and Eastzer, DH 1980 Song structure and song development as potential contributors to reproductive isolation in cowbirds (Molothrus ater). Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 94: 10281039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kipper, S, Mundry, R, Hultsch, H and Todt, D 2004 Long-term persistence of song performance rules in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos): a longitudinal field study on repertoire size and composition. Behaviour 141: 371390Google Scholar
Kirkwood, JK 2003 Introduction: Wildlife Casualties and the Veterinary Surgeon. In: Mullineaux, E, Best, D and Cooper, JE (eds) BSAVA Manual of Wildlife Casualties pp 15. British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Gloucester, UKGoogle Scholar
Konishi, M 1964 Effects of deafening on song development in two species of junco. Condor 66: 85102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konishi, M 1965 Effects of deafening in song development in American robins and black-headed grosbeaks. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 22: 584599Google Scholar
Konishi, M 1994 An outline of recent advances in birdsong neu-robiology. Brain Behavior and Evolution 44: 279285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, JR 1977 The significance of song repertoires: the Beau Geste hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 25: 475478CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, DE 1974 Song learning, dialects and dispersal in the Bewick's wren. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 35: 352380Google ScholarPubMed
Kroodsma, DE 1976 Effect of large song repertoires on neighbor recognition in male song sparrows. Condor 78: 9799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, DE 1977 A re-evaluation of song development in the song sparrow. Animal Behaviour 25: 390399CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroodsma, DE and Miller, EH 1983 Acoustic communication in birds volume 2: song learning and its consequences. Academic Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Kroodsma, DE and Pickert, R 1980 Environmentally dependent sensitive periods for avian vocal learning. Nature 288: 477479CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambrechts, M and Dhondt, AA 1986 Male quality, reproduction, and survival in the great tit (Parus major). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 19: 5763CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langmore, NE 1999 Song tutor choice in polyandrous dun-nocks. Ethology 105: 125136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leitner, S, Nicholson, J, Leisler, B, DeVoogd, TJ and Catchpole, CK 2002 Song and the song control pathway in the brain can develop independently of exposure to song in the sedge warbler. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 269B: 2519-2524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, W-C and Kroodsma, DE 1999 Song development by chipping sparrows and field sparrows. Animal Behaviour 57: 12751286CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llewellyn, P 2003 Rehabilitation and release. In: Mullineaux, E, Best, D and Cooper, JE (eds) BSAVA Manual of Wildlife Casualties pp 2936. British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Gloucester, UKGoogle Scholar
Margoliash, D, Staicer, C and Inoue, SA 1994 The process of syllable acquisition in adult indigo buntings (Passerina cyanea). Behaviour 131: 3964Google Scholar
Marler, P 1967 Comparative study of song development in sparrows. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 14: 231244Google Scholar
Marler, P 1970 A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 71: S125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P 1981 Birdsong: the acquisition of a learned motor skill. Trends in Neurosciences 3: 8894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P 1997 Three models of song learning: evidence from behavior. Journal of Neurobiology 33: 5015163.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marler, P 2004 Innateness and the instinct to learn. Anais da Academia Brasileira de Ciencias 76: 189200CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marler, P and Peters, S 1977 Selective vocal learning in a sparrow. Science 198: 519521CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P and Peters, S 1982 Long-term storage of learned bird-songs prior to production. Animal Behaviour 30: 479482CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P and Peters, S 1987 A sensitive period for song acquisition in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia: a case of age-limited learning. Ethology 76: 89100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P and Peters, S 1988 Sensitive periods for song acquisition from tape recordings and live tutors in the swamp sparrow, Melospiza georgiana. Ethology 77: 7684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P and Tamura, M 1964 Song ‘dialects’ in three popoulations of white-crowned sparrows. Science 146: 14831486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P, Kreith, M and Tamura, M 1962 Song development in hand-raised Oregon juncos. Auk 79: 1230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P, Mundinger, P, Waser, MS and Lutjen, A 1972 Effects of acoustical stimulation and deprivation on song development in red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus). Animal Behaviour 20: 586606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, PK 1980 Song dialects in the corn bunting (Emberiza calandra). Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 54: 285297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, PK 1985 Song dialects: what has to be explained, and with what? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, PK and Krebs, JR 1982 Song types in a population of great tits (Parus major): their distribution, abundance and acquisition by individuals. Behaviour 79: 126152CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, PK and Krebs, JR 1989 Song learning in adult great tits (Parus major) - effects of neighbors. Behaviour 108: 139159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGregor, PK, Krebs, JR and Perrins, CM 1981 Song repertoires and lifetime reproductive success in the great tit (Parus major). American Naturalist 118: 149159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messmer, E and Messmer, I 1956 Die Entwicklung der Lautausserungen und einiger Verhaltensweisen der Amsel (Turdus merula merula L.) unter natürlichen Bedingungen und nach Einzelaufzucht in schalldichten Raümen. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 13: 341441. [Title translation: Development of calls and behaviour of the blackbird under natural conditions and after acoustic isolation]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metcalfe, NB and Monaghan, P 2001 Compensation for a bad start: grow now, pay later? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 16: 254260CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooney, R 1999 Sensitive periods and circuits for learned bird-song. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 9: 121127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mountjoy, DJ and Lemon, RE 1995 Extended song learning in wild European starlings. Animal Behaviour 49: 357366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulligan, D 1966 Singing behavior and its development in the song sparrow. University of California publications in Zoology 81: 176Google Scholar
Mundinger, PC 1995 Behaviour-genetic analysis of canary song: inter-strain differences in sensory learning, and epigenetic rules. Animal Behaviour 50: 14911511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, DA 1998 External validity and experimental design: the sensitive phase for song learning. Animal Behaviour 56: 487491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, DA, Marler, P, Soha, JA and Fullerton, AL 1997 The timing of song memorization differs in males and females: a new assay for avian vocal learning. Animal Behaviour 54: 587597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nice, MM 1943 Studies in the life history of the song sparrow II. The behaviour of the song sparrow and other passerines. Transactions of the Linnean Society of New York 6: 1328Google Scholar
Nottebohm, F 1969 The ‘critical period’ for song learning in birds. Ibis 111: 386387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nottebohm, F 1993 The search for neural mechanisms that define the sensitive period for song learning in birds. Netherlands Journal of Zoology 43: 193234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nottebohm, F and Nottebohm, ME 1978 Relationship between song repertoire and age in the canary Serinus canaria. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 46: 298305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nottebohm, F, Stokes, TM and Leonard, CM 1976 Central control of song in the canary. Journal of Comparative Neurology 165: 457486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S, Peters, S and Podos, J 1998 Song learning, early nutrition and sexual selection in songbirds. American Zoologist 38: 179190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S, Hasselquist, D, Bensch, S and Peters, S 2000 Nestling growth and song repertoire size in great reed warblers: evidence for song learning as an indicator mechanism in mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 267B: 2419-2424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowicki, S, Searcy, WA and Peters, S 2002 Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. Journal of Comparative Physiology 188A: 1003-1014Google Scholar
Otter, KA, Ratcliffe, L, Njegovan, M and Fotheringham, J 2002 Importance of frequency and temporal song matching in black-capped chickadees: evidence from interactive playback. Ethology 108: 181191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, RB 1978 Microgeographic variation in songs of splendid sunbirds Nectarinia coccinigaster: population phenetics, habitats, and song dialects. Behaviour 65: 282308Google Scholar
Payne, RB 1981 Song learning and social interaction in indigo buntings. Animal Behaviour 29: 688697CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, RB, Thompson, WL, Fiala, KL and Sweany, LL 1981 Local song traditions in indigo buntings: cultural transmission of behavior patterns across generations. Behaviour 77: 199221Google Scholar
Petrinovich, L and Baptista, LF 1987 Song development in the white-crowned sparrow: modification of learned song. Animal Behaviour 35: 961974CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliffe, L and Weisman, RG 1986 Song sequence discrimination in the black-capped chickadee (Parus atricapillus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 100: 361367CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, JO and Thompson, WL 1968 Song development in the indigo bunting. Animal Behaviour 16: 462469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riebel, K and Slater, PJB 1998 Testing female chaffinch song preferences by operant conditioning. Animal Behaviour 56: 14431453Google ScholarPubMed
Searcy, WA 1984 Song repertoire size and female preferences in song sparrows. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 14: 281286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searcy, WA 1988 Dual intersexual and intrasexual functions of song in red-winged blackbirds. Proceedings of the International Ornithological Congress 19: 13721381Google Scholar
Searcy, WA 1990 Species recognition of song by female red-winged blackbirds. Animal Behaviour 40: 11191127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searcy, WA and Marler, P 1981 A test for responsiveness to song structure and programming in female sparrows. Science 213: 926928CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searcy, WA and Yasukawa, K 1996 Song and female choice. In: Kroodsma, DE and Miller, EH (eds) Ecology and evolution of acoustic communication in birds pp 454473. Cornell University Press: New York, USAGoogle Scholar
Slater, PJB and Ince, SA 1982 Song development in chaffinches: what is learnt and when? Ibis 124: 2126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, KA, Buchanan, KL, Goldsmith, AR and Catchpole, CK 2003 Song as an honest signal of developmental stress in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Hormones and Behavior 44: 132139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, KA, Buchanan, KL, Goldsmith, AR and Catchpole, CK 2004 Developmental stress, social rank and song complexity in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris). Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 271B: S121-S123Google Scholar
Stocker, L 2005 Practical wildlife care. Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturdy, CB, Phillmore, LS, Sartor, JJ and Weisman, RG 2001 Reduced social contact causes auditory perceptual deficits in zebra finches, Taeniopygia guttata. Animal Behaviour 62: 12071218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielcke, G 1970 Lernen von Gesand als möglicher Schrittmacher der Evolution. Zeitschrift für die Zoologische Systematik und Evolutionsforschung 8: 309320. [Title translation: Song learning as a possible pacemaker in evolution]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielcke, G 1973 Uniformierung des Gesangs den Tannenmeise (Parus ater) durch Lernen. Journal für Ornithologie 114: 443454. [Title translation: Universals in the song of the coal tit during learning]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thielcke-Poltz, H and Thielcke, G 1960 Akustisches Lernen verschieden alter schallsolierter Amseln (Turdus merula) und die Entwicklung erlernter Motive ohne und mit künstlichen Einfluss von Testosteron. Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 17: 211244. [Title translation: Acoustic learning of differently aged echo-located blackbirds and the development of acquired motives with and without the artificial influence of testosterone]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, WH 1954 The process of song-learning in the chaffinch as studied by means of the sound spectrograph. Nature 173: 465469CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, WH 1958 Further studies on the process of song learning in the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs gengleri). Nature 182: 554557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorpe, WH 1961 Bird song. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
Todt, D and Boehner, J 1994 Experience can modify social selectivity during song learning in the nightingale (Luscinia megarhynchos). Ethology 97: 169176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todt, D, Hultsch, H and Heike, D 1979 Conditions affecting song acquisition in nightingales (Luscinia megarhynchos L.). Zeitschrift für Tiërpsychologie 51: 2325Google Scholar
Tribe, A and Brown, PR 2000 The role of wildlife rescue groups in the care and rehabilitation of Australian fauna. Human Dimensions of Wildlife 5: 6985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yasukawa, K 1981 Song repertoires in the red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus): a test of the Beau Geste hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 29: 114125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yasukawa, K, Blank, JL and Patterson, CB 1980 Song repertoires and sexual selection in the red-winged blackbird. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 7: 233238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, RJ 2003 Environmental enrichment for captive animals Blackwell Publishing: Oxford, UKCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zann, RA 1996 The zebra finch: a synthesis of field and laboratory studies. Oxford University Press: Oxford, UKGoogle Scholar