Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T09:13:34.932Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2018

Adam Clark Arcadi
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Wild Chimpanzees
Social Behavior of an Endangered Species
, pp. 190 - 225
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, M. (1981). Individual copulatory preference and the “strange female effect” in a captive group-living male chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Primates 22: 221236.Google Scholar
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49: 227267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amsler, S. J. (2010). Energetic costs of territorial boundary patrols by wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 72: 93103.Google Scholar
Amster, G. and Sella, G. (2016). Life history effects on the molecular clock of autosomes and sex chromosomes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113: 15881593.Google Scholar
Anderson, D. P., Nordheim, E. V., Boesch, C., and Moermond, T. C. (2002). Factors influencing fission–fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Taï National Park, Cote d’Ivoire. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 90101.Google Scholar
Anderson, D. P., Nordheim, E. V., Moermond, T. C., Gone Bi, Z. B., and Boesch, C. (2005). Factors influencing tree phenology in Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. Biotropica 37: 631640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, K. (2017). Chimpanzee mind reading: Don’t stop believing. Philosophy Compass 12: e12394. doi:10.1111/phc3.12394Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark (1996). Phrase structure of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Patterns of production and interpopulation variability. American Journal of Primatology 39: 159178.Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark (2000). Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolution of language. Journal of Human Evolution 39: 205223.Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark (2005). Language evolution: What do chimpanzees have to say? Current Biology 15: R884R886.Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark and Wallauer, W. (2011). Individual-level lateralization in the asymmetrical gaits of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for hand preference and skeletal asymmetry? Behaviour 148: 14191441.Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark and Wallauer, W. (2013). They wallop like they gallop: Audiovisual analysis reveals the influence of gait on buttress drumming by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology 34: 194215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark and Wrangham, R. W. (1999). Infanticide in chimpanzees: Review of cases and a new within-group observation from the Kanyawara study group in Kibale National Park. Primates 40(2): 337351.Google Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark, Robert, D., and Boesch, C. (1998). Buttress drumming by wild chimpanzees: Temporal patterning, phrase integration into loud calls, and preliminary evidence for individual distinctiveness. Primates 39(4): 505518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arcadi, A. Clark, Robert, D., and Mugurusi, F. (2004). A comparison of buttress drumming by male chimpanzees from two populations. Primates 45(2): 135139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K. and Whiten, A. (2001). Post-conflict behaviour of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Behaviour 138: 649690.Google Scholar
Arnold, K. and Whiten, A. (2003). Grooming interactions among the chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda: Test of five explanatory models. Behaviour 140: 519552.Google Scholar
Arnold, K., Fraser, O. N., and Aureli, F. (2011). Postconflict reconciliation. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), 2nd edn., Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 608625.Google Scholar
Atsalis, S. and Videan, E. (2009). Reproductive aging in captive and wild common chimpanzees: Factors influencing the rate of follicular depletion. American Journal of Primatology 71: 271282.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. and de Waal, F. B. M., eds. (2000). Natural Conflict Resolution. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Aureli, F. and Smucny, D. (2000). The role of emotion in conflict and conflict resolution. In Natural Conflict Resolution (Aureli, F. and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, pp. 199224.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Preston, S. D., and de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): A pilot study. Journal of Comparative Psychology 113: 5965.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Cords, M., and van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: A predictive framework. Animal Behaviour 64: 325343.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Schaffner, C. M., Boesch, C., Bearder, S. K., Call, J., Chapman, C. A., Connor, R., Di Fiore, A., Dunbar, R. I. M., Henzi, S. P., Holekamp, K., Korstjens, A. H., Layton, R., Lee, P., Lehmann, J., Manson, J. H., Ramos-Fernandez, G., Strier, K. B., and van Schaik, C. P. (2008). Fission–fusion dynamics. Current Anthropology 49: 627654.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., Fraser, O. N., Schaffner, C. M., and Schino, G. (2012). The regulation of social relationships. In The Evolution of Primate Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 531551.Google Scholar
Babiszewska, M., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., and Slocombe, K. E. (2015). Social, contextual, and individual factors affecting the occurrence and acoustic structure of drumming bouts in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Physical Anthropology 156: 125134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Babweteera, F., Sheil, D., Reynolds, V., Plumptre, A.J., Zuberbühler, K., Hill, C.M., Webber, A., and Tweheyo, M. (2011). Environmental and anthropogenic changes in and around Budongo Forest Reserve. In The Ecological Impact of Long-Term Changes in Africa’s Rift Valley (Plumptre, A. J., ed.), Nova Science, New York, NY, pp. 3153.Google Scholar
Balcomb, S. R., Chapman, C. A., and Wrangham, R. W. (2000). Relationship between chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) density and large, fleshy-fruit tree density: Conservation implications. American Journal of Primatology 51: 197203.Google Scholar
Baldwin, L. A. and Teleki, G. (1973). Field research on chimpanzees and gorillas: An historical, geographical, and bibliographical listing. Primates 14: 315330.Google Scholar
Ban, S. D., Boesch, C., and Janmaat, K. R. L. (2014). Taï chimpanzees anticipate revisiting high-valued fruit trees from further distances. Animal Cognition 17: 13531364.Google Scholar
Ban, S. D., Boesch, C., N’Guessan, A., N’Goran, E. K., Tako, A., and Janmaat, K. R. L. (2016). Taï chimpanzees change their travel direction for rare feeding trees providing fatty fruits. Animal Behaviour 118: 135147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, T. Q. (2011). The Hylobatidae: Small apes of Asia. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), 2nd edn., Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 300312.Google Scholar
Bates, L. A. and Byrne, R. W. (2009). Sex differences in the movement patterns of free-ranging chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): Foraging and border checking. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64: 247255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentley-Condit, V. K. and Smith, E. O. (2010). Animal tool use: Current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog. Behaviour 147: 185221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biro, D., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., Yamakoshi, G., Sousa, C., and Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from field experiments. Animal Cognition 6: 213223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biro, D., Carvalho, S., and Matsuzawa, T. (2010a). Tools, traditions, and technologies: Interdisciplinary approaches to chimpanzee nut cracking. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee (Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 141155.Google Scholar
Biro, D., Humle, T., Koops, K., Sousa, C., Hayashi, M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2010b). Chimpanzee mothers at Bossou, Guinea carry the mummified remains of their dead infants. Current Biology 20: R351R352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjork, A., Liu, W., Wertheim, J. O., Hahn, B. H., and Worobey, M. (2011). Evolutionary history of chimpanzees inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes. Molecular Biology and Evolution 28: 615623.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991a). The effects of leopard predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees. Behaviour 117: 220241.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991b). Symbolic communication in wild chimpanzees? Human Evolution 6: 8190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991c). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 41: 530532.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1994). Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 48: 653667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1995). Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology 16: 116.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1996). Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In Great Ape Societies (McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 101113.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (2008). Why do chimpanzees die in the forest? The challenges of understanding and controlling for wild ape health. American Journal of Primatology 70: 722726.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (2013). Ecology and cognition of tool use in chimpanzees. In Tool Use in Animals: Cognition and Ecology (Sanz, C. M., Call, J., and Boesch, C., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 2147.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1983). Optimization of nut-cracking with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 83(3–4): 265285.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 78: 547573.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds. (2002). Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Kohou, G., Néné, H., and Vigilant, L. (2006). Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Taï Forest. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 130: 108115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Wittig, R., Moebius, Y., and Normand, E. (2008). Intergroup conflicts among chimpanzees in Taï National Park: Lethal violence and the female perspective. American Journal of Primatology 70: 519532.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Head, J., and Robbins, M. M. (2009). Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. Journal of Human Evolution 56: 560569.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Bolé, C., Eckhardt, N., and Boesch, H. (2010). Altruism in forest chimpanzees: The case of adoption. PLoS One 5(1): e8901.Google Scholar
Bonnie, K. E. and de Waal, F. B. M. (2006). Affiliation promotes the transmission of a social custom: Handclasp grooming among captive chimpanzees. Primates 47: 2734.Google Scholar
Bradbury, J. W. and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2011). Principles of Animal Communication. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.Google Scholar
Brand, C., Eguma, R., Zuberbühler, K., and Hobaiter, C. (2014). First report of prey capture from human laid snare-traps by wild chimpanzees. Primates 55: 437440.Google Scholar
Busse, C. D. (1977). Chimpanzee predation as a possible factor in the evolution of red colobus monkey social organization. Evolution 31: 907911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busse, C. D. (1978). Do chimpanzees hunt cooperatively? American Naturalist 112: 767770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butynski, T. M. (2003). The robust chimpanzee Pan troglodytes: Taxonomy, distribution, abundance, and conservation status. In West African Chimpanzees: Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan (Kormos, R., Boesch, C., Bakarr, M. I., and Butynski, T., eds.), IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group, IUCN, Gland, Switzerland, pp. 512.Google Scholar
Bygott, J. D. (1979). Agonistic behavior, dominance, and social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 405427.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2007). The gestural repertoire of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys (Call, J. and Tomasello, M., eds.), Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 1739.Google Scholar
Carbone, L. et al. (2014). Gibbon genome and the fast karyotype evolution of small apes. Nature 513: 195201.Google Scholar
Caro, T. M. and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in animals? Quarterly Review of Biology 67: 151174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carvalho, S., Cunha, E., Sousa, C., and Matsuzawa, T. (2008). Chaînes opératoires and resource-exploitation strategies in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) nut cracking. Journal of Human Evolution 55: 148163.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A. and Wrangham, R. W. (1993). Range use of the forest chimpanzees of Kibale: Implications for the understanding of chimpanzee social organization. American Journal of Primatology 31: 263273.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A., White, F. J., and Wrangham, R. W. (1994). Party size in chimpanzees and bonobos. In Chimpanzee Cultures (Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 4157.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A., Wrangham, R. W., Chapman, L. J., Kennard, D. K., and Zanne, A. E. (1999). Fruit and flower phenology at two sites in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Journal of Tropical Ecology 15: 189211.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J., Struhsaker, T. T., Zanne, A. E., Clark, C. J., and Poulsen, J. R. (2005). A long-term evaluation of fruiting phenology: Importance of climate change. Journal of Tropical Ecology 21: 3145.Google Scholar
Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J., Ghai, R., Hartter, J., Jacob, A. L., Lwanga, J. S., Omeja, P., Rothman, J. M., and Twinomugisha, D. (2011). Complex responses to climate and anthropogenic changes: An evaluation based on long-term data from Kibale National Park, Uganda. In The Ecological Impact of Long-term Changes in Africa’s Rift Valley (Plumptre, A. J., ed.), Nova Science, New York, NY, pp. 7394.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (2007). Baboon Metaphysics. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Clark (Arcadi), A. P. (1993). Rank differences in the production of vocalizations by wild chimpanzees as a function of social context. American Journal of Primatology 31: 159179.Google Scholar
Clark (Arcadi), A. P. and Wrangham, R. W. (1993). Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: Do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? American Journal of Primatology 31: 99109.Google Scholar
Clark (Arcadi), A. P. and Wrangham, R. W. (1994). Chimpanzee arrival pant hoots: Do they signify food or status? International Journal of Primatology 15: 185205.Google Scholar
Clark, C. B. (1977). A preliminary report on weaning among chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Primate Bio-social Development (Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. and Poirier, F. E., eds.), Garland Publishing, New York, NY, pp. 235260.Google Scholar
Connor, R. (2007). Dolphin social intelligence: Complex alliance relationships in bottlenose dolphins and a consideration of selective environments for extreme brain size evolution in mammals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362: 587602.Google Scholar
Constable, J. L., Ashley, M. V., Goodall, J., and Pusey, A. E. (2001). Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees. Molecular Ecology 10: 12791300.Google Scholar
Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2003). Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: Analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour 66: 115125.Google Scholar
Crockford, C. and Boesch, C. (2005). Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 142: 397421.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Herbinger, I., Vigilant, L., and Boesch, C. (2004). Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning? Ethology 110: 221243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2007). Baboons eavesdrop to deduce mating opportunities. Animal Behaviour 73: 885890.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2008). Social stressors and coping mechanisms in wild female baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus). Hormones and Behavior 53: 254265.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R., and Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Chimpanzees distinguish acoustically similar alert hoos from resting hoos. Folia Primatologica 84: 260261.Google Scholar
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Langergraber, K., Ziegler, T.E., Zuberbühler, K., and Deschner, T. (2013). Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 280: 18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2765Google ScholarPubMed
Cronin, K. A., van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Vreeman, V., and Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Population-level variability in the social climates of four chimpanzee societies. Evolution and Human Behavior 35: 389396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Allcock, B., Thomas, C., and Bard, K. A. (2011). Aping expressions? Chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others. Emotion 11: 10131020.Google Scholar
Davila-Ross, M., Jesus, G., Osborne, J., and Bard, K. A. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) produce same types of “laugh faces” when they emit laughter and when they are silent. PLoS ONE 10(6): e0127337. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0127337Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1982/1998). Chimpanzee Politics. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1994). Chimpanzee’s adaptive potential: A comparison of social life under captive and wild conditions. In Chimpanzee Cultures (Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 243260.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and Aureli, F. (1996). Consolation, reconciliation, and a possible cognitive difference between macaques and chimpanzees. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of the Great Apes (Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., and Parker, S. T., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 80110.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P. L., eds. (2003). Animal Social Complexity. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and van Roosmalen, A. (1979). Reconciliation and consolation among chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 5: 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. and Yoshihara, D. (1983). Reconciliation and redirected affection in rhesus monkeys. Behaviour 85: 224241.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987). The Intentional Stance. Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Deschner, T., Heistermann, M., Hodges, K., and Boesch, C. (2003). Timing and probability of ovulation in relation to sex skin swelling in wild West African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus. Animal Behaviour 66: 551560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deschner, T., Heistermann, M., Hodges, K., and Boesch, C. (2004). Female sexual swelling size, timing of ovulation, and male behavior in wild West African chimpanzees. Hormones and Behavior 46: 204215.Google Scholar
Dominy, N. J., Svenning, J., and Li, W. (2003). Historical contingency in the evolution of primate color vision. Journal of Human Evolution 44: 2545.Google Scholar
Doran, D. (1997). Influence of seasonality on activity patterns, feeding behavior, ranging, and grouping patterns in Taï chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 18: 183206.Google Scholar
Duffy, K. G., Wrangham, R. W., and Silk, J. B. (2007). Male chimpanzees exchange political support for mating opportunities. Current Biology 17: R586.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate Social Systems. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6: 178190.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. and Shultz, S. (2007). Understanding primate brain evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362: 649658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery Thompson, M. (2005). Reproductive endocrinology of wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes): Methodological considerations and the role of hormones in sex and conception. American Journal of Primatology 67: 137158.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M. (2013). Reproductive ecology of female chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 75: 222237.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M. and Wrangham, R. W. (2006). Comparison of sex differences in gregariousness in fission-fusion species. In Primates of Western Uganda (Newton-Fisher, N. E., Notman, H., Paterson, J. D., and Reynolds, V., eds.), Springer, New York, NY, pp. 209226.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M. and Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Diet and reproductive function in wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Kibale National Park, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 135: 171181.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M., Newton-Fisher, N. E., and Reynolds, V. (2006). Probable community transfer of parous adult female chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 27: 16011617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emery Thompson, M., Kahlenberg, S. M., Gilby, I. C., and Wrangham, R. W. (2007a). Core area quality is associated with variance in reproductive success among female chimpanzees at Kibale National Park. Animal Behaviour 73: 501512.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M., Jones, J. H., Pusey, A. E., Brewer-Marsden, S., Goodall, J., Marsden, D., Matsuzawa, T., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., and Wrangham, R. W. (2007b). Age and fertility patterns in wild chimpanzees provide insights into the evolution of menopause. Current Biology 17: 17.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2012). The energetics of lactation and the return to fecundity in wild chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology 23(6): 12341241.Google Scholar
Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2014). Male chimpanzees compromise the foraging success of their mates in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 19731983.Google Scholar
Estienne, V., Stephens, C., and Boesch, C. (2017). Extraction of honey from underground bee nests by central African chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in Loango National Park, Gabon: Techniques and individual differences. American Journal of Primatology. doi:10.1002/ajp.22672Google Scholar
Fallon, B.L., Neumann, C., Byrne, R. W., and Zuberbühler, K. (2016). Female chimpanzees adjust copulation calls according to reproductive status and level of female competition. Animal Behaviour 113: 8792.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P. and Slocombe, K. E. (2013). The social function of food-associated calls in male chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 75: 726739.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fedurek, P., Donnellan, E., and Slocombe, K. E. (2014). Social and ecological correlates of long-distance pant hoot calls in male chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 68: 13451355.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Schell, A. M., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013a). The acoustic structure of chimpanzee pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 17811789.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Machanda, Z. P., Schel, A. M., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013b). Pant hoot chorusing and social bonds in male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 86: 189196.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Slocombe, K. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2015a). Chimpanzees communicate to two different audiences during aggressive interactions. Animal Behaviour 110: 2128.Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Slocombe, K.E., Hartel, J.A., and Zuberbühler, K. (2015b). Chimpanzee lip-smacking facilitates cooperative behaviour. Scientific Reports 5: 13460. doi:10.1038/srep13460Google Scholar
Fedurek, P., Slocombe, K. E., Enigk, D. K., Emery Thompson, M., Wrangham, R. W., and Muller, M. N. (2016). The relationship between testosterone and long-distance calling in wild male chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 70: 659672.Google Scholar
Feldblum, J. T., Wroblewski, E. E., Rudicell, R. S., Hahn, B. H., Paiva, T., Cetinkaya-Rundel, M., Pusey, A. E., and Gilby, I. C. (2014). Sexually coercive male chimpanzees sire more offspring. Current Biology 24: 28552860.Google Scholar
Ferguson, R. B. (2011). Born to live: Challenging killer myths. In Origins of Altruism and Cooperation (Sussman, R. W. and Cloninger, C. R., eds.), Springer, pp. 249270.Google Scholar
Fischer, J. and Price, T. (2016). Meaning, intention, and inference in primate communication. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, J., Wheeler, B. C., and Higham, J. P. (2015). Is there any evidence for vocal learning in chimpanzee food calls? Current Biology 25: R1028R1029.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, C. L., Altmann, J., and Alberts, S. C. (2015). Exaggerated sexual swellings and male mate choice in primates: Testing the reliable indicator hypothesis in the Amboseli baboons. Animal Behaviour 104: 175185.Google Scholar
Fleagle, J. G. (2013). Primate Adaptation and Evolution, 3rd edn., Academic Press, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Foerster, S., McLellan, K., Schroepfer-Walker, K., Murray, C. M., Krupenye, C., Gilby, I. C., and Pusey, A. E. (2015). Social bonds in the dispersing sex: Partner preferences among adult female chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 105: 139152.Google Scholar
Foster, M. W., Gilby, I. C., Murray, C. M., Johnson, A., Wroblewski, E. E., and Pusey, A. E. (2009). Alpha male chimpanzee grooming patterns: Implications for dominance “style.” American Journal of Primatology 71: 136144.Google Scholar
Fourrier, M., Sussman, R. W., Kippen, R., and Childs, G. (2008). Demographic modeling of a predator-prey system and its implication for the Gombe population of Procolobus rufomitratus tephrosceles. International Journal of Primatology 29: 497508.Google Scholar
Fowler, A. and Sommer, V. (2007). Subsistence technology of Nigerian chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 28: 9971023.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. (2003). Making space for traditions. Evolutionary Anthropology 12: 6170.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M. and Perry, S., eds. (2003a). The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fragaszy, D.M. and Perry, S. (2003b). Towards a biology of traditions. In The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence (Fragaszy, D. M. and Perry, S., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 132.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N. and Aureli, F. (2008). Reconciliation, consolation and postconflict behavioral specificity in chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 70: 11141123.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N., Stahl, D., and Aureli, F. (2008). Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105: 85578562.Google Scholar
Fraser, O. N., Koski, S.E., Wittig, R. M., and Aureli, F. (2009). Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression? Communicative & Integrative Biology 2: 285291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fröhlich, M., Kuchenbuch, P., Müller, G., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Wittig, R. M., and Pika, S. (2016). Unpeeling the layers of language: Bonobos and chimpanzees engage in cooperative turn-taking sequences. Scientific Reports 6: 25887; doi:10.1038/srep2588CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fröhlich, M., Müller, G., Zeiträg, C., Wittig, R. M., and Pika, S. (2017). Gestural development of chimpanzees in the wild: The impact of interactional experience. Animal Behaviour, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.12.018Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (1989). Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. International Journal of Primatology 10(3): 173197.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. and Thompson, J., eds. (2008). The Bonobos: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. Springer, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Geissmann, T. (2002). Taxonomy and evolution of gibbons. Evolutionary Anthropology Supplement 1: 2831.Google Scholar
Georgiev, A. V., Russell, A. F., Emery Thompson, M., Otali, E., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2014). The foraging costs of mating effort in male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). International Journal of Primatology 35: 725745.Google Scholar
Ghiglieri, M. (1984). The Chimpanzees of Kibale Forest: A Field Study of Ecology and Social Structure. Columbia University Press, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C. (2006). Meat sharing among Gombe chimpanzees: Harassment and reciprocal exchange. Animal Behaviour 71: 953963.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C. and Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Risk-prone hunting by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) increases during periods of high diet quality. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61: 17711779.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C. and Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Association patterns among wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) reflect sex differences in cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62: 18311842.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Eberly, L. E., Pintea, L., and Pusey, A. E. (2006). Ecological and social influences on the hunting behaviour of wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 72: 169180.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Eberly, L. E., and Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Economic profitability of social predation among wild chimpanzees: Individual variation promotes cooperation. Animal Behaviour 75: 351360.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Emery Thompson, M., Ruane, J. D., and Wrangham, R. W. (2010). No evidence for short-term exchange of meat for sex among chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 59: 4453.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Wilson, M. L., and Pusey, A. E. (2013a). Ecology rather than psychology explains co-occurrence of predation and border patrols in male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 86: 6174.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Brent, L. J. N., Wroblewski, E. E., Rudicell, R. S., Hahn, B. H., Goodall, J., and Pusey, A. E. (2013b). Fitness benefits of coalitionary aggression in male chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 373381.Google Scholar
Gilby, I. C., Machanda, Z. P., Mjungu, D. C., Rosen, J., Muller, M. N., Pusey, A. E., and Wrangham, R. W. (2015). “Impact hunters” catalyse cooperative hunting in two wild chimpanzee communities. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 370: 20150005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilby, I. C., Machanda, Z. P., O’Malley, R. C., Murray, C. M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Walker, K., Mjungu, D. C., Otali, E., Muller, M. N., Emery Thompson, M., Pusey, A. E., and Wrangham, R. W. (2017). Predation by female chimpanzees: toward an understanding of sex differences in meat acquisition in the last common ancestor of Pan and Homo. Journal of Human Evolution 110: 8294.Google Scholar
Goldberg, T. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (1997). Genetic correlates of social behaviour in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from mitochondrial DNA. Animal Behaviour 54: 559570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, C. M. and Boesch, C. (2009). Wild chimpanzees exchange meat for sex on a long-term basis. PLoS ONE 4: 16 (e5116).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, C. M. and Boesch, C. (2011). Reciprocity and trades in wild West African chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 21832196.Google Scholar
Gomes, C. M., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2009). Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 276: 699706.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Gombe Stream Reserve. In Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes (DeVore, I., ed.), Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, NY, pp. 425473.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1967/2005). Mother-offspring relationships in free-ranging chimpanzees. In Primate Ethology (Morris, D., ed.), Aldine, New Brunswick, NJ, pp. 287346.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1968a). The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs 1: 163311.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1968b). A preliminary report on expressive movements and communication in the Gombe Stream chimpanzees. In Primate Patterns (Jay, P.C., ed.), Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, NY, pp. 313374.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1970). Tool-using in primates and other vertebrates. In Advances in the Study of Behavior, Vol. 3 (Hinde, R. A. and Shaw, E., eds.), Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 195249.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1971). In the Shadow of Man, 3rd edn. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt, New York, NY.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1973). Cultural elements in a chimpanzee community. In Precultural Primate Behaviour, Symposia of the 4th International Congress of Primatology, Vol. 1 (Menzel, E. W., ed.), Karger, Basel, pp. 144184.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1977). Infant-killing and cannibalism in free-living chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica 28: 259282.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Goodall, J. (1992). Unusual violence in the overthrow of an alpha male chimpanzee at Gombe. In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1: Human Origins (Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M. and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 131142.Google Scholar
Goodall, J., Bandora, A., Bergmann, E., Busse, C., Matama, H., Mpongo, E., Pierce, A., and Riss, D. (1979). Intercommunity interactions in the chimpanzee population of the Gombe National Park. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 1353.Google Scholar
Goosen, C. (1987). Social grooming in primates. In Comparative Primate Biology Vol. 2, Part B: Behavior, Cognition and Motivation (Mitchell, G. and Erwin, J., eds.), Alan R. Liss, New York, NY, pp. 107132.Google Scholar
Gradstein, F. M. (2012). A Geologic Time Scale. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Graham, C. E. (1981). Menstrual cycle of the great apes. In Reproductive Biology of the Great Apes (Graham, C. E., ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 143.Google Scholar
Groves, C. (2001). Primate Taxonomy. Smithsonian Institution Press, Washington, D.C.Google Scholar
Gruber, T. and Zuberbühler, K. (2013). Vocal recruitment for joint travel in wild chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 8(9): e76073. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0076073Google Scholar
Grueter, C. C. (2015). Home range overlap as a driver of intelligence in primates. American Journal of Primatology 77: 418424.Google Scholar
Halperin, S. D. (1979). Temporary association patterns in free ranging chimpanzees. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 491499.Google Scholar
Hamai, M., Nishida, T., Takasaki, H., and Turner, L. A. (1992). New records of within-group infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Primates 33(2): 151162.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 152.Google Scholar
Harcourt, A. H. and Stewart, K. J. (2007). Gorilla Society. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Hartwig, W. C. (2002). The Primate Fossil Record. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, T. (1989). Sexual behavior of immigrant and resident female chimpanzees at Mahale. In Understanding Chimpanzees (Heltne, P. G. and Marquardt, L. A., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 90103.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, T. (1990). Sex differences in ranging patterns. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 99114.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, T. and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1983). Opportunistic and restrictive matings among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale mountains, Tanzania. Journal of Ethology 1: 7585.Google Scholar
Hasegawa, T. and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1990). Sperm competition and mating behavior. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. (Nishida, T., ed.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 115132.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, C. and Furuichi, T. (2006). Frequent copulation by females and high promiscuity in chimpanzees in the Kalinzu Forest, Uganda. In Primates of Western Uganda (Newton-Fisher, N., Notman, H., Reynolds, V., and Paterson, J.D., eds.), Springer, New York, NY, pp. 247257.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1985). Social play of juvenile and adolescent chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 26: 343360.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1988). Association partners of young chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 29: 147161.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H. (1990). Social context of pant-grunting in young chimpanzees. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 189206.Google Scholar
Hayaki, H., Huffman, M. A., and Nishida, T. (1989). Dominance among male chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania: A preliminary study. Primates 30: 187197.Google Scholar
Henzi, S. P. and Barrett, L. (1999). The value of grooming to female primates. Primates 40: 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herbinger, I., Papworth, S., Boesch, C., and Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Vocal, gestural and locomotor responses of wild chimpanzees to familiar and unfamiliar intruders: A playback study. Animal Behaviour 78: 13891396.Google Scholar
Hey, J. (2010). The divergence of chimpanzee species and subspecies as revealed in multipopulation isolation-with-migration analysis. Molecular Biology and Evolution 27: 921933.Google Scholar
Hildebrand, M. (1977). Analysis of asymmetrical gaits. Journal of Mammalogy 58: 131156.Google Scholar
Hill, K. (2009). Animal “culture”? In The Question of Animal Culture (Laland, K. N. and Galef, B. G., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 269287.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1976). Interactions, relationships and social structure. Man 11: 117.Google Scholar
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1989). Sex differences in the behavioral development of chimpanzees at Mahale. In Understanding Chimpanzees (Heltne, P. G. and Marquardt, L. A., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 104115.Google Scholar
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1992). Cannibalism among non-human primates. In Cannibalism: Ecology and Evolution among Diverse Taxa (Elgar, M. A. and Crespi, B. J., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 323338.Google Scholar
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., and Nishida, T. (1984). Demographic study of a large-sized unit-group of chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania: A preliminary report. Primates 25: 401413.Google Scholar
Hirata, S., Yamakoshi, G., Fujita, S., Ohashi, G., and Matsuzawa, T. (2001). Capturing and toying with hyraxes (Dendrohyrax dorsalis) by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Primatology 53: 9397.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2011a). The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition 14: 745767.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C. and Byrne, R. W. (2011b). Serial gesturing by wild chimpanzees: Its nature and function for communication. Animal Cognition 14: 827838.Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C., Schel, A. M., Langergraber, K., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014a). “Adoption” by maternal siblings in wild chimpanzees. PLoS ONE 9(8): 16 (e103777).Google Scholar
Hobaiter, C., Poisot, T., Zuberbühler, K., Hoppitt, W., and Gruber, T. (2014b). Social network analysis shows direct evidence for social transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. PLoS Biology 12: 112.Google Scholar
Hockings, K. J. (2011). The crop-raiders of the sacred hill. In The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds.), Springer, Tokyo, pp. 211220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hockings, K. J., Yamakoshi, G., Kabasawa, A., and Matsuzawa, T. (2010). Attacks on local persons by chimpanzees in Bossou, Republic of Guinea: Long-term perspectives. American Journal of Primatology 72: 887896.Google Scholar
Hopkins, W. D., Taglialatela, J. P., and Leavens, D. A. (2007). Chimpanzees differentially produce novel vocalizations to capture the attention of a human. Animal Behaviour 73: 281286.Google Scholar
Hosaka, K. (2015a). Hunting and food sharing. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 274290.Google Scholar
Hosaka, K. (2015b). Intimidation display. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 435447.Google Scholar
Hosaka, K., Nishida, T., Hamai, M., Matsumoto-Oda, A., and Uehara, S. (2001). Predation of mammals by the chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In All Apes Great and Small Vol. 1: African Apes (Galdikas, B. M. F., Briggs, N. E., Sheeran, L. K., Shapiro, G. L., and Goodall, J., eds.), Kluwer Academic, NY, pp. 107130.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1974). Male–male competition and infanticide among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Folia Primatologica 22: 1958.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1977). The Langurs of Abu. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among animals: A review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology 1: 1340.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (1981). The Woman That Never Evolved. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Huchard, E., Courtiol, A., Benavides, J. A., Knapp, L. A., Raymond, M., and Cowlishaw, G. (2009). Can fertility signals lead to quality signals? Insights from the evolution of primate sexual swellings. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 276: 18891897.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. (1990). Some socio-behavioral manifestations of old age. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.). University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 236255.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. (2015). Chimpanzee self-medication: A historical perspective of the key findings. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 340353.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. and Hirata, S. (2004). An experimental study of leaf swallowing in captive chimpanzees: Insights into the origin of a self-medicative behavior and the role of social learning. Primates 45: 113118.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A. and Kalunde, M. A. (1993). Tool-assisted predation on a squirrel by a female chimpanzee in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates 34: 9398.Google Scholar
Huffman, M. A., Spiezio, C., Sgaravatti, A., and Leca, J. (2010). Leaf swallowing behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Biased learning and the emergence of group level cultural differences. Animal Cognition 13: 871880.Google Scholar
Humle, T. (2011). Location and ecology. In The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds.), Springer, Tokyo, pp. 1321.Google Scholar
Humle, T. and Fragaszy, D. M. (2011). Tool use and cognition in primates. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 637651.Google Scholar
Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T., and Matsuzawa, T. (2009a). Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Animal Cognition 12 (Suppl. 1): S37S48.Google Scholar
Humle, T., Colin, C., and Raballand, E. (2009b). Preliminary report on hand-clasp grooming in sanctuary-released chimpanzees, Haut Niger National Park, Guinea. Pan African News 16: 13.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Growing Points in Ethology (Hinde, R. and Bateson, P., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 303317.Google Scholar
Hunt, K. D. (1989). Positional behavior in Pan troglodytes at the Mahale Mountains and the Gombe Stream National Parks, Tanzania. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.Google Scholar
Hyeroba, D., Apell, P., and Otali, E. (2011). Managing a speared alpha male chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Veterinary Record 169: 658. doi:10.1136/vr.d4680Google Scholar
Inoue-Nakamura, N. and Matsuzawa, T. (1997). Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 111: 159173.Google Scholar
Inoue, E., Inoue-Murayama, M., Vigilant, L., Takenaka, O., and Nishida, T. (2008). Relatedness in wild chimpanzees: Influence of paternity, male philopatry, and demographic factors. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 137: 256262.Google Scholar
Isabirye-Basuta, G. (1989). The Ecology and Conservation Status of the Chimpanzee Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii in Kibale Forest, Uganda. Ph.D. dissertation, Makerere University.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. and Muramatsu, D. (2015). Patterns and trends in fruiting phenology: Some important implications for chimpanzee diet. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 174194.Google Scholar
Itoh, N. and Nakamura, M. (2015). Diet and feeding behavior. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 227245.Google Scholar
IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (2017). www.primate-sg.org/species.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2013). Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour 86: 11831205.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D., and Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 111: 1634316348.Google Scholar
Jenny, D. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Hunting behaviour of West African forest leopards. African Journal of Ecology 43: 197200.Google Scholar
Jensen, K. (2012). Social regard: Evolving a psychology of cooperation. In The Evolution of Primate Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 565584.Google Scholar
Jones, J. H., Wilson, M. L., Murray, C., and Pusey, A. (2010). Phenotypic quality influences fertility in Gombe chimpanzees. Journal of Animal Ecology 79: 12621269.Google Scholar
Judge, P. G. (1991). Dyadic and triadic reconciliation in pigtail macaques (Macaca nemestrina). American Journal of Primatology 23: 225237.Google Scholar
Kaas, J. H. (2005). The evolution of visual cortex in primates. In The Primate Visual System (Kremers, J., ed.), John Wiley & Sons, New York, NY, pp. 267283.Google Scholar
Kaburu, S. S. K. and Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2013). Social instability raises the stakes during social grooming among wild male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 86: 519527.Google Scholar
Kaburu, S. S. K. and Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2015a). Egalitarian despots: Hierarchy steepness, reciprocity and the grooming-trade model in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour 99: 6171.Google Scholar
Kaburu, S. S. K. and Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2015b). Trading or coercion? Variation in male mating strategies between two communities of East African chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 69: 10391052.Google Scholar
Kaburu, S. S. K., Inoue, S., and Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2013). Death of the alpha: Within-community lethal violence among chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park. American Journal of Primatology 75: 789797.Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M. and Wrangham, R. W. (2010). Sex differences in chimpanzees’ use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology 20: R10671068.Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., Emery Thompson, M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2008a). Female competition over core areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 29: 931947.Google Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2008b). Immigration costs for female chimpanzees and male protection as an immigrant counterstrategy to intrasexual aggression. Animal Behaviour 76: 14971509.Google Scholar
Kalan, A. K., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2015). Wild chimpanzees modify food call structure with respect to tree size for a particular fruit species. Animal Behaviour 101: 19.Google Scholar
Kamilar, J. M. and Marshack, J. L. (2012). Does geography or ecology best explain “cultural” variation among chimpanzee communities? Journal of Human Evolution 62: 256260.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. (2012). The behavioral ecology of strepsirrhines and tarsiers. In The Evolution of Primate Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 1942.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. and van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Evolution of primate social systems. International Journal of Primatology 23: 707740.Google Scholar
Kawanaka, K. (1989). Age differences in social interactions of young males in a chimpanzee unit-group at the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 30(3): 285305.Google Scholar
Kawanaka, K. (1990). Alpha males’ interactions and social skills. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 171187.Google Scholar
Kawanaka, K. and Nishida, T. (1974). Recent advances in the study of inter-unit-group relationships and social structure of wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. In Proceedings of the Symposia of the 5th Congress of the International Primatological Society (Kondo, S., Ehara, A., Kawai, M., and Kawamura, S., eds.), Japan Science Press, Tokyo, pp. 173186.Google Scholar
Knott, C. (2001). Female reproductive ecology of the apes: Implications for human evolution. In Reproductive Ecology and Human Evolution (Ellison, P.T., ed.), Aldine de Gruyter, New York, NY, pp. 429463.Google Scholar
Kojima, S. (2001). Early vocal development in a chimpanzee. In Primate Origins of Human Cognition and Behavior (Matsuzawa, T., ed.), Springer, Hong Kong, pp. 190196.Google Scholar
Kojima, S., Izumi, A., and Ceugniet, M. (2003). Identification of vocalizers by pant hoots, pant grunts and screams in a chimpanzee. Primates 44: 225230.Google Scholar
Köndgen, S., Kühl, H., N’Goran, P. K., Walsh, P. D., Schenk, S., Ernst, N., Biek, R., Formenty, P., Matz-Rensing, K., Schweiger, B., Junglen, S., Ellerbrok, H., Nitsche, A., Briese, T., Lipkin, W. I., Pauli, G., Boesch, C., and Leendertz, F. H. (2008). Pandemic human viruses cause decline of endangered great apes. Current Biology 18: 15.Google Scholar
Koops, K., Humle, T., Sterck, E. H. M., and Matsuzawa, T. (2007). Ground nesting by the chimpanzees of the Nimba Mountains, Guinea: Environmentally or socially determined? American Journal of Primatology 69: 407419.Google Scholar
Koops, K., McGrew, W. C., de Vries, H., and Matsuzawa, T. (2012). Nest-building by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Seringbara, Nimba Mountains: Antipredation, thermoregulation, and antivector hypotheses. International Journal of Primatology 33: 356380.Google Scholar
Koops, K., Schöning, C., Isaji, M., and Hashimoto, C. (2015). Cultural differences in ant-dipping tool length between neighbouring chimpanzee communities at Kalinzu, Uganda. Scientific Reports 5: 12456; doi:10.1038/srep12456Google Scholar
Koski, S. E. and Sterck, E. H. M. (2007). Triadic postconflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: Does consolation console? Animal Behaviour 73: 133142.Google Scholar
Koski, S. E. and Sterck, E. H. M. (2009). Post-conflict third-party affiliation in chimpanzees: What’s in it for the third party? American Journal of Primatology 71: 409418.Google Scholar
Kraft, T., Venkataraman, V. V., and Dominy, N. J. (2014). A natural history of human tree climbing. Journal of Human Evolution 71: 105118.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L. (1928). Sub-human culture beginnings. Quarterly Review of Biology 3: 325342.Google Scholar
Kühl, H., Sop, T., Williamson, E. A., Mundry, R., Brugière, D., Campbell, G., Cohen, H., Danquah, E., Ginn, L., Herbinger, I., Jones, S., Junker, J., Kormos, R., Kouakou, C. Y., N’Goran, P. K., Normand, E., Shutt-Phillips, K., Tickle, A., Vendras, E., Welsh, A., Wessling, E. G., and Boesch, C. (2017). The critically endangered western chimpanzee declines by 80%. American Journal of Primatology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22681Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1968). Tripartite relations in hamadryas baboons. In Social Communication among Primates (Altmann, S. A., ed.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 6372.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1978). On the value of social relationships to nonhuman primates: A heuristic scheme. Social Science Information 17: 687705.Google Scholar
Kummer, H. (1995). In Quest of the Sacred Baboon. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Kutsukake, N. and Castles, D. L. (2004). Reconciliation and post-conflict third-party affiliation among wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Primates 45: 157165.Google Scholar
Kutsukake, N. and Matsusaka, T. (2002). Incident of intense aggression by chimpanzees against an infant from another group in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 58: 175180.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. and Galef, B. G., eds. (2009). The Question of Animal Culture. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Lamon, N., Neumann, C., Gruber, T., and Zuberbühler, K. (2017). Kin-based cultural transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees. Science Advances 3: e1602750.Google Scholar
Langergraber, K. E. and Vigilant, L. (2011). Genetic differences cannot be excluded from generating behavioural differences among chimpanzee groups. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 278: 20942095. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0391Google Scholar
Langergraber, K., Mitani, J., and Vigilant, L. (2007). The limited impact of kinship on cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 104(19): 77867790.Google Scholar
Langergraber, K., Mitani, J., and Vigilant, L. (2009). Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 71: 840851.Google Scholar
Langergraber, K. E., Boesch, C., Inoue, E., Inoue-Murayama, M., Mitani, J. C., Nishida, T., Pusey, A., Reynolds, V., Schubert, G., Wrangham, R. W., Wroblewski, E., and Vigilant, L. (2011). Genetic and “cultural” similarity in wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 278: 408416. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1112Google Scholar
Langergraber, K., Mitani, J., Watts, D. P., and Vigilant, L. (2013). Male–female socio-spatial relationships and reproduction in wild chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 861873.Google Scholar
Langergraber, K. E., Rowney, C., Crockford, C., Wittig, R., Zuberbühler, K., and Vigilant, L. (2014). Genetic analyses suggest no immigration of adult females and their offspring into the Sonso community of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology 76: 640648.Google Scholar
LaPorte, M. N. C. and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Animal Behaviour 80: 467473.Google Scholar
Lee, P. C. (1987). Nutrition, fertility and maternal investment in primates. Journal of Zoology 213: 409422.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. and Boesch, C. (2004). To fission or to fusion: Effects of community size on wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) social organization. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56: 207216.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. and Boesch, C. (2005). Bisexually bonded ranging in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57: 525535.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. and Boesch, C. (2008). Sexual differences in chimpanzee sociality. International Journal of Primatology 29: 6581.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J. and Boesch, C. (2009). Sociality of the dispersing sex: The nature of social bonds in West African female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Animal Behaviour 77: 377387.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J., Fickenscher, G., and Boesch, C. (2006). Kin biased investment in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 143: 931955.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Animal Behaviour 74: 16171629.Google Scholar
Levréro, F. and Mathevon, N. (2013). Vocal signature in wild infant chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 75: 324332.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour 70: 673683.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Animal Cognition 9: 3646.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E., and Pusey, A. E. (2004). Sex differences in learning in chimpanzees. Nature 428: 715716.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds. (2010). The Mind of the Chimpanzee. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Anderson, K. E., Stanton, M. A., Shender, M., Heintz, M. R., Goodall, J., and Murray, C. M. (2014a). Boys will be boys: Sex differences in wild chimpanzee social interactions. Animal Behaviour 88: 7983.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Markham, A. C., Heintz, M. R., Anderson, K. E., Ciuk, D. J., Goodall, J., and Murray, C. M. (2014b). Sex differences in wild chimpanzee behavior emerge during infancy. PLoS ONE 9: e99099. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0099099Google Scholar
Luncz, L. V. and Boesch, C. (2014). Tradition over trend: Neighboring chimpanzee communities maintain differences in cultural behavior despite frequent immigration of adult females. American Journal of Primatology 76: 649657.Google Scholar
Luncz, L. V. and Boesch, C. (2015). The extent of cultural variation between adjacent chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) communities: A microecological approach. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 156: 6775.Google Scholar
Luncz, L. V., Mundry, R., and Boesch, C. (2012). Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Current Biology 22: 922926.Google Scholar
Lwanga, J. S., Struhsaker, T. T., Struhsaker, P. J., Butynski, T. M., and Mitani, J. C. (2011). Primate population dynamics over 32.9 years at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. American Journal of Primatology 73: 9971011.Google Scholar
Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., and McGrew, W. C. (2007). Phylogenetic analyses of behavior support existence of culture among wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 104: 1758817592.Google Scholar
Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Cladistic analyses of behavioural variation in wild Pan troglodytes: Exploring the chimpanzee culture hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution 142: 337349.Google Scholar
Lycett, S. J., Collard, M., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Are behavioral differences among wild chimpanzee communities genetic or cultural? An assessment using tool-use data and phylogenetic methods. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 142: 461467.Google Scholar
Macedonia, J. M. and Evans, C. S. (1993). Variation among mammalian alarm call systems and the problem of meaning in animal signals. Ethology 93: 177197.Google Scholar
Machanda, Z. P., Gilby, I. C., and Wrangham, R. W. (2013). Male–female association patterns among free-ranging chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). International Journal of Primatology 34: 917938.Google Scholar
Machanda, Z. P., Gilby, I. C., and Wrangham, R. W. (2014). Mutual grooming among adult male chimpanzees: The immediate investment hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 87: 165174.Google Scholar
Manson, J. and Wrangham, R. W. (1991). Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and humans. Current Anthropology 32(4): 369390.Google Scholar
Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., and Boesch, C. (1995). Census and distribution of chimpanzees in Côte d’Ivoire. Primates 36(4): 591607.Google Scholar
Markham, A. C., Santymire, R. M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Heintz, M. R., Lipende, I., and Murray, C. M. (2014). Rank effects on social stress in lactating chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 87: 195202.Google Scholar
Marks, J. (2002). What It Means to Be 98% Chimpanzee. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1969). Vocalizations of wild chimpanzees: An introduction. Proceedings of the 2nd International Congress of Primatology, Atlanta, 1968, Vol. 1, Karger, Basel, pp. 94100.Google Scholar
Marler, P. (1976). Social organization, communication, and graded signals: The chimpanzee and the gorilla. In Growing Points in Ethology (Hinde, R. and Bateson, P., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 239280.Google Scholar
Marler, P. and Hobbett, L. (1975). Individuality in a long-range vocalization of wild chimpanzees. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 38: 97109.Google Scholar
Marler, P. and Tenaza, R. (1977). Signalling behavior of apes with special reference to vocalization. In How Animals Communicate (Sebeok, T. A., ed.), Indiana University Press, Bloomington, pp. 9651032.Google Scholar
Marler, P., Evans, C. S., and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals: Motivational, referential, or both? In Nonverbal Vocal Communication (Papousek, H., Jürgens, U., and Papousek, M., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 6686.Google Scholar
Marshall, A., Wrangham, R. W., and Arcadi, A. Clark (1999). Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour 58: 825830.Google Scholar
Martin, P. and Bateson, P. (1995). Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A. (1999a). Mahale chimpanzees: Grouping patterns and cycling females. American Journal of Primatology 47: 197207.Google Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A. (1999b). Female choice in the opportunistic mating of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Mahale. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 46: 258266.Google Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A. and Ihara, Y. (2011). Estrous asynchrony causes low birth rates in wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 73: 180188.Google Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A. and Oda, R. (1998). Changes in the activity budget of cycling female chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology 46: 157166.Google Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A., Hosaka, K., Huffman, M. A., and Kawanaka, K. (1998). Factors affecting party size in chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. International Journal of Primatology 19(6): 9991011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto-Oda, A., Kutsukaka, N., Hosaka, K., and Matsusaka, T. (2007). Sniffing behaviors of Mahale chimpanzees. Primates 48: 8185.Google Scholar
Matsusaka, T. (2004). When does play panting occur during social play in wild chimpanzees? Primates 45: 221229.Google Scholar
Matsusaka, T., Shimada, M., and Nakamura, M. (2015). Diversity of play. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 544555.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1994). Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. In Chimpanzee Cultures (Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 351370.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1996). Chimpanzee intelligence in nature and captivity: Isomorphism of symbol use and tool use. In Great Ape Societies (McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 196209.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Log doll: Pretence in wild chimpanzees. In The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds.), Springer, Tokyo, pp. 131135.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds. (2011). The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba. Springer, Tokyo.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1977). Socialization and object manipulation of wild chimpanzees. In Primate Bio-Social Development: Biological, Social and Ecological Determinants (Poirer, F. E. and Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S., eds.), Garland Press, New York, NY, pp. 261288.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1979). Evolutionary implications of sex differences in chimpanzee predation and tool use. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 440463.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2004). The Cultured Chimpanzee: Reflections on Cultural Primatology. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2017). Field studies of Pan troglodytes reviewed and comprehensively mapped, focussing on Japan’s contribution to cultural primatology. Primates 58: 237258.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. and Tutin, C. E. G. (1978). Evidence for a social custom in wild chimpanzees? Man (New Series) 13: 234251.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Baldwin, P. J., and Tutin, C. E. G. (1988). Diet of Wild Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Mt. Assirik, Senegal: I. Composition. American Journal of Primatology 16: 213226.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T., eds. (1996). Great Ape Societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., Scott, S. E., and Tutin, C. E. G. (2001). Intergroup differences in a social custom of wild chimpanzees: The grooming hand-clasp of the Mahale Mountains. Current Anthropology 42: 148153.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Hunt, K. D. (2007). Etho-archaeology of manual laterality: Well digging by wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica 78: 240244.Google Scholar
McLennan, M. R. (2011). Tool-use to obtain honey by chimpanzees at Bulindi: New record from Uganda. Primates 52: 315322.Google Scholar
Mech, L. D., Adams, L. G., Meier, T. J., Burch, J. W., and Dale, B. W. (1998). The Wolves of Denali. University of Minnesota Press, Minneapolis, MN.Google Scholar
Miller, J. A., Pusey, A. E., Gilby, I. C., Schroepfer-Walker, K. Markham, A. C., and Murray, C. M. (2014). Competing for space: Female chimpanzees are more aggressive inside than outside their core areas. Animal Behaviour 87: 147152.Google Scholar
Milton, K. (1993). Diet and primate evolution. Scientific American August: 8693.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2006a). Demographic influences on the behavior of chimpanzees. Primates 47: 613.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2006b). Reciprocal exchange in chimpanzees and other primates. In Cooperation in Primates and Humans (Kappeler, P. M. and van Schaik, C. P., eds.), Springer, Berlin, pp. 107119.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2009a). Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal Behaviour 77: 633640.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2009b). Cooperation and competition in chimpanzees: Current understanding and future challenges. Evolutionary Anthropology 18: 215227.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Amsler, S. J. (2003). Social and spatial aspects of male subgrouping in a community of wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 140: 869884.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Brandt, K. L. (1994). Social factors influence the acoustic variability in the long-distance calls of male chimpanzees. Ethology 96: 233252.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. (1995). Species and sex differences in the screams of chimpanzees and bonobos. International Journal of Primatology 16: 393411.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Gros-Louis, J. (1998). Chorusing and call convergence in chimpanzees: tests of three hypotheses. Behaviour 135: 10411064.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Nishida, T. (1993). Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 45: 735746.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Watts, D. P. (1999). Demographic influences on the hunting behavior of chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109: 439454.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Watts, D. P. (2001). Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour 61: 915924.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C. and Watts, D. P. (2005). Correlates of territorial boundary patrol behaviour in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 70: 10791086.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hasegawa, T., Gros-Louis, J., Marler, P., and Byrne, R. (1992). Dialects in wild chimpanzees? American Journal of Primatology 27: 233243.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Gros-Louis, J., and Macedonia, J. M. (1996). Selection for acoustic variability within the vocal repertoire of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 17: 569583.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Hunley, K. L., and Murdoch, M. E. (1999). Geographic variation in the calls of wild chimpanzees: A reassessment. American Journal of Primatology 47: 133151.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Merriwether, D. A., and Zhang, C. (2000a). Male affiliation, cooperation and kinship in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 59: 885893.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Struhsaker, T. T., and Lwanga, J. S. (2000b). Primate community dynamics in old growth forest over 23.5 years at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda: Implications for conservation and census methods. International Journal of Primatology 21: 269286.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., and Lwanga, J. S. (2002a). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 102111.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., Pepper, J. W., and Merriwether, A. (2002b). Demographic and social constraints on male chimpanzee behaviour. Animal Behaviour 64: 727737.Google Scholar
Mitani, J. C., Watts, D. P., and Amsler, S. J. (2010). Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology 20: R507R508.Google Scholar
Mitra Setia, T., Delgado, R. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Singleton, I., and van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Social organization and male–female relationships. In Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., and van Schaik, C. P., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 245253.Google Scholar
Mjungu, D. C. (2010). Dynamics of Intergroup Competition in Two Neighboring Chimpanzee Communities. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Minnesota, http://conservancy.umn.edu/bitstream/95275/1/MJUNGU_umn_0130E_11279.pdfGoogle Scholar
Möbius, Y., Boesch, C., Koops, K., Matsuzawa, T., and Humle, T. (2008). Cultural differences in army ant predation by West African chimpanzees? A comparative study of microecological variables. Animal Behaviour 76: 3745.Google Scholar
Moorjani, P., Amorim, C. E. G., Arndt, P. F., and Przeworski, M. (2016). Variation in the molecular clock of primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113: 1060710612.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N. (2002). Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 112124.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N. and Wrangham, R. W. (2004a). Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: A test of the “challenge hypothesis.” Animal Behaviour 67: 113123.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N. and Wrangham, R. W. (2014). Mortality rates among Kanyawara chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 67: 107114.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N., Emery Thompson, M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2006). Male chimpanzees prefer mating with older females. Current Biology 16: 22342238.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N., Kahlenberg, S. M., Emery Thompson, M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Male coercion and the costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 274: 10091014.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N., Kahlenberg, S. M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2009). Male aggression against females and sexual coercion in chimpanzees. In Sexual Coercion in Primates and Humans (Muller, M. N. and Wrangham, R. W., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 184217.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N., Emery Thompson, M., Kahlenberg, S. M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Sexual coercion by male chimpanzees shows that female choice may be more apparent than real. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65: 921933.Google Scholar
Muller, M. N., Wrangham, R. W., and Pilbeam, D. R., eds. (2017). Chimpanzees and Human Evolution. Belknap Press of Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M. (2007). Method for assigning categorical rank in female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii via the frequency of approaches. International Journal of Primatology 28: 853864.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M., Eberly, L. E., and Pusey, A. E. (2006). Foraging strategies as a function of season and rank among wild female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Ecology 17(6): 10201028.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M., Mane, S. V., and Pusey, A. E. (2007a). Dominance rank influences female space use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Towards an ideal despotic distribution. Animal Behaviour 74: 17951804.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M., Wroblewski, E., and Pusey, A. E. (2007b). New case of intragroup infanticide in the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. International Journal of Primatology 28: 2337.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M., Gilby, I. C., Mane, S. V., and Pusey, A. E. (2008). Adult male chimpanzees inherit maternal ranging patterns. Current Biology 18: 2024.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Eberly, L. E., and Pusey, A. E. (2009). Reproductive energetics in free-living female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behavioral Ecology 20: 12111216.Google Scholar
Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R., and Sanz, C. (2016). Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Scientific Reports 6: 34783.Google Scholar
Nadler, R. D., Graham, C. E., Gosselin, R. E., and Collins, D. C. (1985). Serum levels of gonadotropins and gonadal steroids, including testosterone, during the menstrual cycle of the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology 9: 273284.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2002). Grooming-hand-clasp in Mahale M group chimpanzees: Implications for culture in social behaviors. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 7183.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2010). Poke-type social scratching persists at Mahale. Pan Africa News 17: 1517.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. (2011). Comparison of social behaviors. In The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds.), Springer, New York, NY, pp. 251263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Nishida, T. (2013). Ontogeny of a social custom in wild chimpanzees: Age changes in grooming hand-clasp at Mahale. American Journal of Primatology 75: 186196.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. and Uehara, S. (2004). Proximate factors of different types of grooming hand-clasp in Mahale chimpanzees: Implications for chimpanzee social customs. Current Anthropology 45: 108114.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T. (2000). Social scratch: Another social custom in wild chimpanzees? Primates 41: 237248.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M. et al. (2013). Ranging behavior of Mahale chimpanzees: A 16 year study. Primates 54: 171182.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds. (2015). Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Nakazawa, N., Hanamura, S., Inoue, E., Nakatsukasa, M., and Nakamura, M. (2013). A leopard ate a chimpanzee: First evidence from East Africa. Journal of Human Evolution 65: 334337.Google Scholar
Nekaris, K. A. I. and Bearder, S. K. (2011). The lorisiform primates of Asia and mainland Africa. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), 2nd edn., Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 3454.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (1999a). Association by male chimpanzees: A social tactic? Behaviour 136(6): 705730.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (1999b). Infant killers of Budongo. Folia Primatologica 70: 167169.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2002). Relationships of male chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 125137.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2003). The home range of the Sonso community of chimpanzees from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. African Journal of Ecology 41: 150156.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2006). Female coalitions against male aggression in wild chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. International Journal of Primatology 27: 15891599.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. and Lee, P. C. (2011). Grooming reciprocity in wild male chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 81: 439446.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E., Reynolds, V., and Plumptre, A. J. (2000). Food supply and chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) party size in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 21: 613628.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E., Emery Thompson, M., Reynolds, V., Boesch, C., and Vigilant, L. (2010). Paternity and social rank in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 142: 417428.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates 9: 167224.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1970). Social behavior and relationship among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Primates 11: 4787.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1979). The social structure of chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 73121.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1980). The leaf-clipping display: A newly discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution 9: 117128.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1983). Alpha status and agonistic alliance in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Primates 24(3): 318336.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1989). Social interactions between resident and immigrant chimpanzees. In Understanding Chimpanzees (Heltne, P. G. and Marquardt, L. A., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 6889.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., ed. (1990). The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (1997). Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 38(4): 379398.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (2010). Chimpanzee behavior in the wild [electronic resource]: An audiovisual encyclopedia. Springer, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. (2012). Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore: Natural History and Culture at Mahale. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (1985). Responses to a stranger mother-son pair in the wild chimpanzee: A case report. Primates 26: 113.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Hosaka, K. (1996). Coalition strategies among adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In Great Ape Societies (McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F., and Nishida, T., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 114134.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Kawanaka, K. (1972). Inter-unit-group relationships among wild chimpanzees of the Mahali Mountains. Kyoto University Africa Studies 7: 131169.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Kawanaka, K. (1985). Within-group cannibalism by adult male chimpanzees. Primates 26(3): 274284.Google Scholar
Nishida, T. and Turner, L. A. (1996). Food transfer between mother and infant chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology 17: 947968.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Uehara, S., and Nyundo, R. (1979). Predatory behavior among wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. Primates 20: 120.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., and Takahata, Y. (1985). Group extinction and female transfer in wild chimpanzees in the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 67: 284301.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Takasaki, H., and Takahata, Y. (1990). Demography and reproductive profiles. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 6397.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Hasegawa, T., Hayaki, H., Takahata, Y., and Uehara, S. (1992). Meat-sharing as a coalition strategy by an alpha male chimpanzee? In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1: Human Origins (Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M., and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 159174.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Hosaka, K., and Hamai, M. (1995). A within-group gang attack on a young adult male chimpanzee: Ostracism of an ill-mannered member? Primates 36: 207211.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Corp, N., Hamai, M., Hasegawa, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hosaka, K., Hunt, K. D., Itoh, N., Kawanaka, K., Matsumoto-Oda, A., Mitani, J. C., Nakamura, M., Norikoshi, K., Sakamaki, T., Turner, L., Uehara, S., and Zamma, K. (2003). Demography, female life history, and reproductive profiles among the chimpanzees of Mahale. American Journal of Primatology 59: 99121. doi:10.1002/ajp.10068Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D. P. (2004). Variable grooming behaviours in wild chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica 75: 3136.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Matsusaka, T., and McGrew, W. C. (2009). Emergence, propagation or disappearance of novel behavioral patterns in the habituated chimpanzees of Mahale: A review. Primates 50: 2336.Google Scholar
Nishida, T., Zamma, K., Matsusaka, T., Inaba, A., and McGrew, W. C. (2010). Chimpanzee Behavior in the Wild: An Audio-visual Encyclopedia. Springer, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Nishie, H. (2015). Use of tools and other objects. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 568582.Google Scholar
Notman, H. and Rendall, D. (2005). Contextual variation in chimpanzee pant hoots and its implications for referential communication. Animal Behaviour 70: 177190.Google Scholar
Nunn, C. L. (1999). The evolution of exaggerated sexual swellings in primates and the graded-signal hypothesis. Animal Behaviour 58: 229246.Google Scholar
O’Malley, R. C., Wallauer, W., Murray, C. M., and Goodall, J. (2012). The appearance and spread of ant fishing among the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe: A possible case of intercommunity cultural transmission. Current Anthropology 53: 650663.Google Scholar
Ohashi, G. and Matsuzawa, T. (2011). Deactivation of snares by wild chimpanzees. Primates 52: 15.Google Scholar
Otali, E. and Gilchrist, J. S. (2006). Why chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) mothers are less gregarious than nonmothers and males: The infant safety hypothesis. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 59: 561570.Google Scholar
Packer, C. and Ruttan, L. (1988). The evolution of cooperative hunting. American Naturalist 132: 159198.Google Scholar
Parish, A. R. (1996). Female relationships in bonobos (Pan paniscus). Human Nature 7: 6196.Google Scholar
Parr, L. A. and Waller, B. M. (2006). Understanding chimpanzee facial expression: Insights into the evolution of communication. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1: 221228. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl031Google Scholar
Parr, L. A., Waller, B. M., Vick, S. J., and Bard, K. A. (2007). Classifying chimpanzee facial expressions using muscle action. Emotion 7: 172181.Google Scholar
Pepper, J. W., Mitani, J. C., and Watts, D. P. (1999). General gregariousness and specific social preferences among wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 20: 613632.Google Scholar
Perry, S. (2009). Are nonhuman primates likely to exhibit cultural capacities like those of humans? In The Question of Animal Culture (Laland, K. N. and Galef, B. G., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 247268.Google Scholar
Pieta, K. (2008). Female mate preferences among Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii of Kanyawara, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 29: 845864.Google Scholar
Pika, S. and Mitani, J. C. (2006). Referential gestural communication in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Current Biology 16(6): R191R192.Google Scholar
Pintea, L., Pusey, A., Wilson, M., Gilby, I., Collins, A., Kamenya, S., and Goodall, J. (2011). Long-term ecological changes affecting the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In The Ecological Impact of Long-Term Changes in Africa’s Rift Valley (Plumptre, A. J., ed.), Nova Science, New York, NY, pp. 227247.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1978a). Tool use during chimpanzees’ bushpig hunt. Carnivore 1: 103106.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1978b). Some basic traits of language in wild chimpanzees? In Action, Gesture and Symbol (Lock, A., ed.), Academic Press, London, pp. 111131.Google Scholar
Plooij, F. X. (1980). The Behavioural Development of Free-living Chimpanzee Babies and Infants. Ph.D. dissertation, Rijksuniversiteit te Groningen.Google Scholar
Plumptre, A. J. and Reynolds, V. (1994). The effect of selective logging on the primate populations in the Budongo Forest Reserve, Uganda. Journal of Applied Ecology 31: 631641.Google Scholar
Plumptre, A. J. et al. (2010). Eastern Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan 2010–2020. IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group, Gland.Google Scholar
Polansky, L. and Boesch, C. (2013). Long-term changes in fruit phenology in a West African lowland tropical rain forest are not explained by rainfall. Biotropica 45: 434440.Google Scholar
Pontzer, H. and Wrangham, R. W. (2006). Ontogeny of ranging in wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 27: 295309.Google Scholar
Potts, K. B., Watts, D. P., and Wrangham, R. W. (2011). Comparative feeding ecology of two communities of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 32: 669690.Google Scholar
Power, M. (1991). The Egalitarians – Human and Chimpanzee. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Prasetyo, D., Ancrenaz, M., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Wich, S. A., and van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Nest building in orangutans. In Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Setia, T. M., and van Schaik, C. P., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 269277.Google Scholar
Price, T., Wadewitz, P., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Hammerschmidt, K., and Fischer, J. (2015). Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Scientific Reports 5: 111, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/srep13220Google Scholar
Pruetz, J. D. (2006). Feeding ecology of savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Fongoli, Senegal. In Feeding Ecology in Apes and Other Primates (Hohmann, G., Robbins, M. M., and Boesch, C., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 161182.Google Scholar
Pruetz, J. D. and Bertolani, P. (2007). Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with tools. Current Biology 17: 412417.Google Scholar
Pruetz, J. D., Bertolani, P., Ontl, K. B., Lindshield, S., Shelley, M., and Wessling, E. G. (2015). New evidence on the tool-assisted hunting exhibited by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in a savannah habitat at Fongoli, Sénégal. Royal Society Open Science 2: 140507. doi:10.1098/rsos.140507Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. (1980). Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 28: 543552.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. (1983). Mother–offspring relationships in chimpanzees after weaning. Animal Behaviour 31: 363377.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. (1990). Behavioural changes at adolescence in chimpanzees. Behaviour 115: 203246.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. and Packer, C. (1987). Dispersal and philopatry. In Primate Societies (Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 250266.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. and Schroepfer-Walker, K. (2013). Female competition in chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 368: 20130077; http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0077Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E, Williams, J., and Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science 277: 828831.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E. and Wolf, M. (1996). Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 11: 201206.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E., Oehlert, G. W., Williams, J. M., and Goodall, J. (2005). Influence of ecological and social factors on body mass of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 26(1): 331.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E., Pintea, L., Wilson, M. L., Kamenya, S., and Goodall, J. (2007). The contribution of long-term research at Gombe National Park to chimpanzee conservation. Conservation Biology 21: 623634.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E., Murray, C., Wallauer, W., Wilson, M., Wroblewski, E., and Goodall, J. (2008a). Severe aggression among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology 29: 949973.Google Scholar
Pusey, A. E., Wilson, M. L., and Collins, D. A. (2008b). Human impacts, disease risk, and population dynamics in the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 70: 738744.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. and Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 99: 436441.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., and Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general intelligence and cultural intelligence. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 366: 10171027.Google Scholar
Reichard, U. H. and Barelli, C. (2008). Life history and reproductive strategies of Khao Yai Hylobates lar: Implications for social evolution in apes. International Journal of Primatology 29: 823844.Google Scholar
Reynolds, V. (2005). The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: Ecology, Behaviour, and Conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Reynolds, V. and Reynolds, F. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest. In Primate Behavior: Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes (DeVore, I., ed.), Holt, Rinehart & Winston, New York, NY, pp. 368424.Google Scholar
Riede, T., Owren, M. J., and Arcadi, A. Clark (2004). Nonlinear acoustics in pant hoots of common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Frequency jumps, subharmonics, biphonation, and deterministic chaos. American Journal of Primatology 64: 277291.Google Scholar
Riede, T., Arcadi, A. Clark, and Owren, M. J. (2007). Nonlinear acoustics in pant hoots of common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Vocalizing at the edge. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121: 17581767.Google Scholar
Riedel, J., Franz, M., and Boesch, C. (2011). How feeding competition determines female gregariousness and ranging in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. American Journal of Primatology 73: 305313.Google Scholar
Robbins, M. M. (2011). Gorillas: Diversity in ecology and behavior. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), 2nd edn., Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 326339.Google Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Vick, S., and Buchanan-Smith, H. M. (2013). Communicative intentions in wild chimpanzees: persistence and elaboration in gestural signalling. Animal Cognition 16: 187196.Google Scholar
Roberts, A. I., Roberts, S. G. B., and Vick, S. (2014). The repertoire and intentionality of gestural communication in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition 17: 317336.Google Scholar
Russon, A. E. and Begun, D. R., eds. (2004). The Evolution of Thought. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T. (2011). Submissive pant-grunt greetings of female chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. African Study Monographs 32: 2541.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T. and Hayaki, H. (2015). Greetings and dominance. In Mahale Chimpanzees: 50 Years of Research (Nakamura, M., Hosaka, K., Itoh, N., and Zamma, K., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 459471.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T., Itoh, N., and Nishida, T. (2001). An attempted within-group infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Primates 42: 359366.Google Scholar
Sakura, O. and Matsuzawa, T. (1991). Flexibility of nut-cracking behavior using stone hammers and anvils of wild chimpanzees: An experimental analysis. Ethology 87: 237248.Google Scholar
Samuni, L., Mundry, R., Terkel, J., Zuberbühler, K., and Hobaiter, C. (2014). Socially learned habituation to human observers in wild chimpanzees. Animal Cognition 17: 9971005.Google Scholar
Sandel, A. A., Reddy, R. B., and Mitani, J. C. (2017). Adolescent male chimpanzees do not form a dominance hierarchy with their peers. Primates 58: 3949.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M. and Morgan, D. B. (2009). Flexible and persistent tool-using strategies in honey-gathering by wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 30: 411427.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M. and Morgan, D. B. (2010). The complexity of chimpanzee tool-use behaviors. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee (Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 127140.Google Scholar
Sanz, C. M., Deblauwe, I., Tagg, N., and Morgan, D. B. (2014). Insect prey characteristics affecting regional variation in chimpanzee tool use. Journal of Human Evolution 71: 2837.Google Scholar
Scally, A. et al. (2012). Insights into hominid evolution from the gorilla genome sequence. Nature 483: 169175.Google Scholar
Scarantino, A. and Clay, Z. (2015). Contextually variable signals can be functionally referential. Animal Behaviour 100: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.08.017Google Scholar
Schel, A. M., Machanda, Z., Townsend, S. W., Zuberbühler, K., and Slocombe, K. E. (2013). Chimpanzee food calls are directed at specific individuals. Animal Behaviour 86: 955965.Google Scholar
Schel, A. M., Townsend, S. W., Machanda, Z., Zuberbühler, K., and Slocombe, K. E. (2014). Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS One 8(10): e76674. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0076674Google Scholar
Schino, G., Scucchi, S., Maestripieri, D., and Turillazzi, P. G. (1988). Allogrooming as a tension-reduction mechanism: A behavioral approach. American Journal of Primatology 16: 4350.Google Scholar
Schöning, C., Humle, T., Möbius, Y., and McGrew, W. C. (2008). The nature of culture: Technological variation in chimpanzee predation on army ants revisited. Journal of Human Evolution 55: 4859.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. and Fredericson, E. (1951). The causes of fighting in mice and rats. Physiological Zoölogy 24: 273309.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (2003). The structure of social knowledge in monkeys. In Animal Social Intelligence (de Waal, F. B. M. and Tyack, P. L., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 207229.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., and Marler, P. (1980). Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Animal Behaviour 28: 10701094.Google Scholar
Sherrow, H. M. (2012). Adolescent male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda have decided dominance relationships. Folia Primatologica 83: 6775.Google Scholar
Shimada, M. K. (2002). Social scratch among the chimpanzees of Gombe. Pan Africa News 9: 2123.Google Scholar
Shimada, M. K., Hayakawa, S., Fujita, S., Sugiyama, Y., and Saitou, N. (2009). Skewed matrilineal genetic composition in a small wild chimpanzee community. Folia Primatologica 80: 1932.Google Scholar
Short, R. V. (1979). Sexual selection and its component parts, somatic and genital selection, as illustrated by man and the great apes. Advances in the Study of Behavior 9: 131158.Google Scholar
Silk, J. B. (2012). The adaptive value of sociality. In The Evolution of Primate Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 552564.Google Scholar
Simpson, M. J. A. (1973). The social grooming of male chimpanzees. In Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates (Michael, R. P. and Crook, J. H., eds.), Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 4150.Google Scholar
Singleton, I., Knott, C. D., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Wich, S. A., and van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Ranging behavior of orangutan females and social organization. In Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Setia, T. M., and van Schaik, C. P., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 205213.Google Scholar
Slater, K., Cameron, E., Turner, T., and du Toit, J. T. (2008). The influence of oestrous swellings on the grooming behaviour of chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Behaviour 145: 12351246.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2005). Food sharing between wild adult chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): A socially significant event? American Journal of Primatology 65: 385391.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005a). Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. Journal of Comparative Psychology 119: 6777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005b). Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Current Biology 15: 17791784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). Food-associated calls in chimpanzees: responses to food types or food preferences? Animal Behaviour 72: 989999.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 104: 1722817233.Google Scholar
Slocombe, K. E., Kaller, T., Turman, L., Papworth, S., Townsend, S., and Zuberbühler, K. (2010). Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64: 19591966.Google Scholar
Smaers, J. B. and Soligo, C. (2013). Brain reorganization, not relative brain size, primarily characterizes anthropoid brain evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 280: 20130269. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0269Google Scholar
Smith, W. J. (1965). Message, meaning, and context in ethology. American Naturalist 99: 405409.Google Scholar
Smith, W. J. (1969). Messages in vertebrate communication. Science 165: 145150.Google Scholar
Smith, W. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Smuts, B. B. (1986). Gender, aggression, and influence. In Primate Societies (Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W., and Struhsaker, T. T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 400412.Google Scholar
Sobolewski, M. E., Brown, J. L., and Mitani, J. C. (2013). Female parity, male aggression, and the Challenge Hypothesis. Primates 54: 8188.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. (1995). The influence of chimpanzee predation on group size and anti-predator behaviour in red colobus monkeys. Animal Behaviour 49: 577587.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. (1996). The hunting ecology of wild chimpanzees: Implications for the evolutionary ecology of Pliocene hominids. American Anthropologist 98: 96113.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. (1998). Chimpanzee and Red Colobus. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., Wallis, J., Mpongo, E., and Goodall, J. (1994a). Hunting decisions in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 131: 118.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. B., Wallis, J., Matama, H., and Goodall, J. (1994b). Patterns of predation by chimpanzees on red colobus monkeys in Gombe National Park, 1982–1991. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 94: 213228.Google Scholar
Steklis, H. D. and Kling, A. (1985). Neurobiology of affiliative behavior in nonhuman primates. In The Psychobiology of Attachment and Separation (Reite, M. and Field, T., eds.), Academic Press, Orlando, FL, pp. 93134.Google Scholar
Strier, K. B. (2011). Primate Behavioral Ecology, 4th edn., Allyn & Bacon, Needham Heights, MA.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1975). The Red Colobus Monkey. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1999). Primate communities in Africa: The consequence of long-term evolution or the artifact of recent human hunting? In Primate Communities (Fleagle, J. G., Janson, C., and Reed, K. E., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 289294.Google Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (2010). The Red Colobus Monkeys: Variation in Demography, Behavior, and Ecology of Endangered Species. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. M. (2011). Chimpanzees and bonobos: Inter- and intraspecies diversity. In Primates in Perspective (Campbell, C. J., Fuentes, A., MacKinnon, K. C., Bearder, S. K., and Stumpf, R. M., eds.), 2nd edn., Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 340356.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. M. and Boesch, C. (2005). Does promiscuous mating preclude female choice? Female sexual strategies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57: 511524.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. M. and Boesch, C. (2006). The efficacy of female choice in chimpanzees of the Taï Forest, Côte d’Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60: 749765.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. M. and Boesch, C. (2010). Male aggression and sexual coercion in wild West African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus. Animal Behaviour 79: 333342.Google Scholar
Stumpf, R. M., Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M. N., and Wrangham, R. W. (2009). The context of female dispersal in Kanyawara chimpanzees. Behaviour 146: 629656.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1969). Social behavior of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Primates 10: 197225.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1988). Grooming interactions among adult chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, with special reference to social structure. International Journal of Primatology 9: 393407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1999). Socioecological factors of male migration at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 40: 6168.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (2004). Demographic parameters and life history of chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 124: 154165.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. and Fujita, S. (2011). The demography and reproductive parameters of Bossou chimpanzees. In The Chimpanzees of Bossou and Nimba (Matsuzawa, T., Humle, T., and Sugiyama, Y., eds.), Springer, Tokyo, pp. 2334.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. and Koman, J. (1979). Social structure and dynamics of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 20: 323339.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. and Koman, J. (1987). A preliminary list of chimpanzees’ alimentation at Bossou, Guinea. Primates 28: 133147.Google Scholar
Suzuki, A. (1971). Carnivority and cannibalism observed among forest-living chimpanzees. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Nippon 79: 3048.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1985). Adult male chimpanzees kill and eat a male newborn infant: Newly observed intragroup infanticide and cannibalism in Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica 44: 161170.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1990a). Adult males’ social relations with adult females. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.). University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 133148.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y. (1990b). Social relationships among adult males. In The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains (Nishida, T., ed.). University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 149170.Google Scholar
Takahata, Y., Hasegawa, T., and Nishida, T. (1984). Chimpanzee predation in the Mahale Mountains from August 1979 to May 1982. International Journal of Primatology 5: 213233.Google Scholar
Takasaki, H. (1985). Female life history and mating patterns among M group chimpanzees of the Mahale National Park, Tanzania. Primates 26(2): 121129.Google Scholar
Teelen, S. (2008). Influence of chimpanzee predation on the red colobus population at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 49: 4149.Google Scholar
Teleki, G. (1973). The Predatory Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees. Bucknell University Press, Lewisburg, PA.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Gilby, I.C., and Mundry, R. (2009a). The meat-scrap hypothesis: Small quantities of meat may promote cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63: 421431.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2009b). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364: 24052415.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2012). Untrained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) fail to imitate novel actions. PLoS ONE 7: e41548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1994). The question of chimpanzee culture. In Chimpanzee Cultures (Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B. M., and Heltne, P. G., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 301317.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). The question of chimpanzee culture, plus postscript (chimpanzee culture, 2009). In The Question of Animal Culture (Laland, K. N. and Galef, B. G., eds.), Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 198221.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (2007). Introduction: Intentional communication in nonhuman primates. In The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys (Call, J. and Tomasello, M., eds.), Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 115.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Call, J. (2010). Chimpanzee social cognition. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee (Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 235250.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., and Carpenter, M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: A trans-generational study. Primates 35: 137154.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W. and Manser, M. B. (2013). Functionally referential communication in mammals: The past, present and the future. Ethology 119: 111.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Slocombe, K. E., Emery Thompson, M., and Zuberbühler, K. (2007). Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology 17: 355356.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Deschner, T., and Zuberbühler, K. (2008). Female chimpanzees use copulation calls flexibly to prevent social competition. PloS ONE 3(6), e2431.Google Scholar
Townsend, S. W., Deschner, T., and Zuberbühler, K. (2011). Copulation calls in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) convey identity but do not accurately reflect fertility. International Journal of Primatology 32: 914923.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G. (1979). Mating patterns and reproductive strategies in a community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 6: 2938.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G. and McGinnis, P. R. (1981). Chimpanzee reproduction in the wild. In Reproductive Biology of the Great Apes (Graham, C. E., ed.), Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 239264.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. E. G. and McGrew, W. C. (1973). Sexual behaviour of group-living adolescent chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 38: 195199.Google Scholar
Uehara, S. (1986). Sex and group differences in feeding on animals by wild chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates 27: 113.Google Scholar
Uehara, S. (1997). Predation on mammals by the chimpanzee. Primates 38: 193214.Google Scholar
Uehara, S., Nishida, T., Hamai, M., Hayaki, H., Huffman, M., Kawanaka, K., Kobayashi, S., Mitani, J., Takahata, Y., Takasaki, H., and Tsukahara, T. (1992). Characteristics of predation by the chimpanzees in the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1: Human Origins (Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M., and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 143158.Google Scholar
Utami Atmoko, S. S., Singleton, I., van Noordwijk, M. A., van Schaik, C. P., and Setia, T. M. (2009). Male–male relationships in orangutans. In Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioral Ecology and Conservation (Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Setia, T. M., and van Schaik, C. P., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 225233.Google Scholar
Vahed, K. and Parker, D. J. (2012). The evolution of large testes: Sperm competition or male mating rate? Ethology 118: 107117.Google Scholar
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. (1967). The facial displays of the catarrhine monkeys and apes. In Primate Ethology (Morris, D., ed.), Weidenfeld and Nicolson, London, pp. 768.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C. and Haun, D. B. M. (2014). Conformity without majority? The case for demarcating social from majority influences. Animal Behaviour 96: 187194.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Haun, D. B. M., Mundry, R., and Bodamer, M. D. (2012). Neighbouring chimpanzee communities show different preferences in social grooming behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 279: 43624367.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Schütte, S., Call, J., and Haun, D. B. M. (2013). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) flexibly adjust their behaviour in order to maximize payoffs, not to conform to majorities. PLoS One 8: e80945.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Cronin, K. A., Mundry, R., Bodamer, M., and Haun, D. B. M. (2017). Chimpanzee culture extends beyond matrilineal family units. Current Biology 27: R588R590.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., and Nunn, C. L. (1999). Sex and social evolution in primates. In Comparative Primate Socioecology (Lee, P. C., ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 204231.Google Scholar
Vasey, N. (2006). Impact of seasonality and reproduction on social structure, ranging patterns, and fission-fusion social organization in red ruffed lemurs. In Lemurs: Ecology and Adaptation (Gould, L. and Sauther, M., eds.), Springer, New York, NY, pp. 275303.Google Scholar
Visalberghi, E. and Fragaszy, D. (2006). What is challenging about tool use? The capuchin’s perspective. In Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence (Wasserman, E. A. and Zentall, T. R., eds.), Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 529552.Google Scholar
Wakefield, M. L. (2008). Grouping patterns and competition among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 29: 907929.Google Scholar
Wakefield, M. L. (2013). Social dynamics among females and their influence on social structure in an East African chimpanzee community. Animal Behaviour 85: 13031313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waller, B. M. and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2005). Differential behavioural effects of silent bared teeth display and relaxed open mouth display in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 111: 129142.Google Scholar
Wallis, J. (1992). Chimpanzee genital swelling and its role in the pattern of sociosexual behavior. American Journal of Primatology 28: 101113.Google Scholar
Wallis, J. (1997). A survey of reproductive parameters in the free-ranging chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 109: 297307.Google Scholar
Watson, S. K., Townsend, S. W., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E. K., Cheng, L., West, V., and Slocombe, K. E. (2015a). Vocal learning in the functionally referential food grunts of chimpanzees. Current Biology 25: 495499.Google Scholar
Watson, S. K., Townsend, S. W., Schel, A. M., Wilke, C., Wallace, E. K., Cheng, L., West, V., and Slocombe, K. E. (2015b). Reply to Fisher et al. Current Biology 25: R1030R1031.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (1989). Infanticide in mountain gorillas: New cases and a reconsideration of the evidence. Ethology 81: 118.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (1998). Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 44: 4355.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2000a). Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. I. Partner number and diversity and grooming reciprocity. International Journal of Primatology 21: 189210.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2000b). Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park. II. Influence of male rank and possible competition for partners. International Journal of Primatology 21: 211238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2002). Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour 139: 343370.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2006). Conflict resolution in chimpanzees and the valuable-relationships hypothesis. International Journal of Primatology 27: 13371364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2007). Effects of male group size, parity, and cycle stage on female chimpanzee copulation rates at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 48: 222231.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2008). Scavenging by chimpanzees at Ngogo and the relevance of chimpanzee scavenging to early hominin behavioral ecology. Journal of Human Evolution 54: 125133.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2012). Long-term research on chimpanzee behavioral ecology in Kibale National Park, Uganda. In Long-Term Field Studies of Primates (Kappeler, P. M. and Watts, D.P., eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 313338.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. (2015). Mating behavior of adolescent male chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 56: 163172.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. and Amsler, S. J. (2013). Chimpanzee-red colobus encounter rates show a red colobus population decline associated with predation by chimpanzees at Ngogo. American Journal of Primatology 75: 927937.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. and Mitani, J. C. (2001). Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour 138: 299327.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. and Mitani, J. C. (2002a). Hunting behavior of chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology 23: 128.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. and Mitani, J. C. (2002b). Hunting and meat sharing by chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 244255.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P. and Mitani, J. C. (2015). Hunting and prey switching by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo. International Journal of Primatology 36: 728748.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P., Colmenares, F., and Arnold, K. (2000). Redirection, consolation, and male policing. In Natural Conflict Resolution (Aureli, F. and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of California Press, Berkeley, CA, pp. 281301.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P., Mitani, J. C., and Sherrow, H. M. (2002). New cases of inter-community infanticide by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates 43: 263270.Google Scholar
Watts, D. P., Potts, K. B., Lwanga, J. S., and Mitani, J. C. (2012a). Diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, 1. Diet composition and diversity. American Journal of Primatology 74: 114129. doi:10.1002/ajp.21016Google Scholar
Watts, D. P., Potts, K. B., Lwanga, J. S., and Mitani, J. C. (2012b). Diet of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, 2. Temporal variation and fallback foods. American Journal of Primatology 74: 130144.Google Scholar
Webster, T.H., Hodson, P. R., and Hunt, K. D. (2009). Grooming hand-clasp by chimpanzees of the Mugiri community, Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, Uganda. Pan African News 16: 57.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C. and Fischer, J. (2012). Functionally referential signals: A promising paradigm whose time has passed. Evolutionary Anthropology 21: 195205.Google Scholar
Wheeler, B. C. and Fischer, J. (2015). The blurred boundaries of functional reference: A response to Scarantino & Clay. Animal Behaviour 100, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.11.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, F. J. and Wood, K. D. (2007). Female feeding priority in bonobos, Pan paniscus, and the question of female dominance. American Journal of Primatology 69: 837850.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2010). A coming of age for cultural panthropology. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee (Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 87100.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2012). Social learning, traditions, and culture. In The Evolution of Primate Societies (Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., and Silk, J. B., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 682700.Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2015). Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technologies in Homo and Pan. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 370: 20140359.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C., Wrangham, R., and Boesch, C. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399: 682685.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C., Wrangham, R., and Boesch, C. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour 138(11): 14811516.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437: 737740.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Spiteri, A., Horner, V., Bonnie, K. E., Lambeth, S. P., Schapiro, S. J., and de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). Transmission of multiple traditions within and between chimpanzee groups. Current Biology 17: 10381043.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Caldwell, C. A., and Mesoudi, A. (2016). Cultural diffusion in humans and other animals. Current Opinion in Psychology 8: 1521.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Pusey, A. E., Carlis, J. V., Farm, B. P., and Goodall, J. (2002a). Female competition and male territorial behavior influence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Animal Behaviour 63: 347360.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Liu, H., and Pusey, A. E. (2002b). Costs and benefits of grouping for female chimpanzees at Gombe. In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 192203.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Oehlert, G. W., Carlis, J. V., and Pusey, A. E. (2004). Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range? Animal Behaviour 68: 523532.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Wilson, M. L., Schumacher-Stankey, J., Goodall, J., and Pusey, A. E. (2008). Causes of death in the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology 70: 766777.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L. (2012). Long-term studies of the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Long-Term Field Studies of Primates (Kappeler, P. M. and Watts, D. P., eds.), Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp. 357384.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L. and Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology 32: 363392.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., and Wrangham, R.W. (2001). Does participation in intergroup conflict depend on numerical assessment, range location, or rank for wild chimpanzees? Animal Behaviour 61: 12031216.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Wallauer, W. R., and Pusey, A. E. (2004). New cases of intergroup violence among chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology 25: 523549.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Hauser, M. D., and Wrangham, R. W. (2007). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour 144: 16211653.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L., Kahlenberg, S. M., Wells, M., and Wrangham, R. W. (2012). Ecological and social factors affect the occurrence and outcomes of intergroup encounters in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 83: 277291.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L. et al. (2014a). Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature 513: 414417Google Scholar
Wilson, M. L. et al. (2014b). Human impacts are neither necessary nor sufficient to explain chimpanzee violence (or bonobo non-violence). Response to Ferguson (2014) at https://blogs.scientificamerican.com/cross-check/chimp-violence-researchers-respond-to-criticism-on-cross-check/Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M. (2010). The function and cognitive underpinnings of post-conflict affiliation in wild chimpanzees. In The Mind of the Chimpanzee (Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R., and Matsuzawa, T., eds.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 208219.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M. and Boesch, C. (2003). Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Taï National Park. International Journal of Primatology 24: 847867.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittig, R. M. and Boesch, C. (2005). How to repair relationships – Reconciliation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Ethology 111: 736763.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Lehmann, J., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2008). Focused grooming networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Hormones and Behavior 54: 170177.Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Deschner, T., Langergraber, K. E., Ziegler, T. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014a). Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 281: 20133096. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3096Google Scholar
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Langergraber, K. E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2014b). Triadic social interactions operate across time: A field experiment with wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 281: 2013155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3155Google Scholar
Wittiger, L. and Boesch, C. (2013). Female gregariousnesss in western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) is influenced by resource aggregation and the number of estrous females. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 10971111.Google Scholar
Wood, B. (2010). Reconstructing human evolution: Achievements, challenges, and opportunities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107: 89028909.Google Scholar
Wood, B. M., Watts, D. P., Mitani, J. C., and Langergraber, K. E. (2017). Favorable ecological circumstances promote life expectancy in chimpanzees similar to that of human hunter-gatherers. Journal of Human Evolution 105: 4156.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1974). Artificial feeding of chimpanzees and baboons in their natural habitat. Animal Behaviour 22: 8393.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1975). The Behavioural Ecology of Chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1977). Feeding behaviour of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In Primate Ecology (Clutton-Brock, T. H., ed.), Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 503538.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1979). Sex differences in chimpanzee dispersion. In The Great Apes (Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R., eds.), Benjamin/Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 481489.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1993). The evolution of sexuality in chimpanzees and bonobos. Human Nature 4: 4779.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1999). Evolution of coalitionary killing. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 42: 130.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (2000). Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition (Kappeler, P. M., ed.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 248258.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (2002). The cost of sexual attraction: Is there a trade-off in female Pan between sex appeal and received coercion? In Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., and Marchant, L. F., L.F., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 204215.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (2008). Why the link between long-term research and conservation is a case worth making. In Science and Conservation in African Forests (Wrangham, R. and Ross, E., eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 18.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. and Bergmann Riss, E. vZ. (1990). Rates of predation on mammals by Gombe chimpanzees, 1972–1975. Primates 31: 157170.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. and Nishida, T. (1983). Aspilia spp. leaves: A puzzle in the feeding behavior of wild chimpanzees. Primates 24: 276282.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. and Ross, E., eds. (2008). Science and Conservation in African Forests: The Benefits of Longterm Research. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. and Smuts, B. B. (1980). Sex differences in the behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, Supplement 28: 1331.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Clark (Arcadi), A. P., and Isabirye-Basuta, G. (1992). Female social relationships and social organization of Kibale Forest chimpanzees. In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1: Human Origins (Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M., and de Waal, F. B. M., eds.), University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, pp. 8198.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., McGrew, W. C., de Waal, F. B., and Heltne, P. G., eds. (1994). Chimpanzee Cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Wilson, M. L., and Muller, M. N. (2006). Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans. Primates 47: 1426.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Koops, K., Machanda, Z. P., Worthington, S., Bernard, A. B., Brazeau, N. F., Donovan, R., Rosen, J., Wilke, C., Otali, E., and Muller, M. N. (2016). Distribution of a chimpanzee social custom is explained by matrilineal relationship rather than conformity. Current Biology 26: 30333037.Google Scholar
Wroblewski, E. E., Murray, C. M., Keele, B. F., Schumacher-Stankey, J. C., Hahn, B. H., and Pusey, A. E. (2009). Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour 77: 873885.Google Scholar
Zamma, K. (2002). Leaf-grooming by a wild chimpanzee in Mahale. Primates 43: 8790.Google Scholar
Zamma, K. (2005). Rejecting a bit of meat to get more. Pan Africa News 12: 810.Google Scholar
Zamma, K. (2014). What makes wild chimpanzees wake up at night? Primates 55: 5157.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×