Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T11:42:07.548Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

37 - Neuroimaging of Williams–Beuren syndrome

from Section VII - Developmental Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2011

Andreia Santos
Affiliation:
Central Institute of Mental Health Mannheim, Germany
Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy University of Heidelberg and Central Institute of Mental Health Mannheim, Germany
Martha E. Shenton
Affiliation:
VA Boston Healthcare System and Brigham and Women's Hospital, Harvard Medical School
Bruce I. Turetsky
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In the last decades, studying genetic neuropsychiatric syndromes at multiple levels has proven to be a powerful means for elucidating the pathways of both typical and atypical neurodevelopment. Within this context, Williams–Beuren syndrome, or Williams syndrome (WS) for short, has been established as model syndrome of special interest to investigate gene–brain–behavior relationships and a “unique window to genetic contributions to neural function” (Meyer-Lindenberg et al.,2006, p. 391).

WS is a relatively rare neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by a combination of distinctive clinical, cognitive, behavioral, genetic and neuroanatomical features. It was first described in the early 1960s by two groups of cardiologists as a condition involving a constellation of cardiovascular abnormalities, hypercalcemia, peculiar facial “elfin-like” features, and mild to moderate mental retardation (Beuren et al., 1962; Williams et al., 1961).

Insights into the nature of WS culminated in the mid 1990s with the identification of the genetic cause, a so-called microdeletion (see below, Genetic profile). Since then, several neuroimaging studies, using a wide range of new imaging techniques, have attempted to uncover the structural and functional neural substrates of WS, providing an emerging understanding of brain mechanisms mediating between genetic variation and cognitive-behavioral phenotypes in humans.

The aim of this chapter is to review imaging studies delineating the unique neuropsychiatric features of WS, as well as recent advances in investigating the neural substrates of the disorder, which have provided significant contributions to unraveling the impact of a specific genetic defect on brain structure and function.

Type
Chapter
Information
Understanding Neuropsychiatric Disorders
Insights from Neuroimaging
, pp. 537 - 554
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R. 2003. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 165–78.Google Scholar
Aguirre, G K, Zarahn, E and D'Esposito, M. 1998. Neural components of topographical representation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95, 839–46.Google Scholar
,American Academy of Pediatrics. 2001. Health care supervision for children with Williams syndrome. Pediatrics 107, 1192–204.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J. 2000. The Developing Visual Brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Atkinson, J, Anker, S, Braddick, O, Nokes, L, Mason, A and Braddick, F. 2001. Visual and visuospatial development in young children with Williams syndrome. Devptl Med Child Neurol 43, 330–7.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J, Braddick, O, Anker, S, et al. 2003. Neurobiological models of visuospatial cognition in children with Williams syndrome: Measures of dorsal-stream and frontal function. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 139–72.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J, Braddick, O, Rose, F E, Searcy, Y M, Wattam-Bell, J and Bellugi, U. 2006. Dorsal-stream motion processing deficits persist into adulthood in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 44, 828–33.Google Scholar
Atkinson, J, King, J, Braddick, O, Nokes, L, Anker, S and Braddick, F. 1997. A specific deficit of dorsal stream function in Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 8 1919–22.Google Scholar
Bello, A, Capirci, O and Volterra, V. 2004. Lexical production in children with Williams syndrome: Spontaneous use of gesture in a naming task. Neuropsychologia 42, 201–13.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U, Adolphs, R, Cassadi, C and Chiles, M. 1999. Towards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndrome. Neuroreport 10, 1653–7.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U, Lichtenberger, L, Jones, W, Lai, Z and St. George, M. 2000. I. The neurocognitve profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (Suppl 1), 7–29.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U, Sabo, H and Vaid, V. 1988. Spatial defects in children with Williams syndrome. In Stiles-Davis, J, Kritchevsky, M, Bellugi, U (Eds.), Spatial Cognition: Brain Bases and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 273–98.
Bellugi, U, Wang, P and Jernigan, T. 1994. Higher cortical functions: Evidence from specific genetically based syndromes of disorder. In Broman, S, Graffman, J (Eds.), Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Disorders: Implications for Brain Function. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 23–56.
Beuren, A J, Apitz, J and Harmjanz, D. 1962. Supravalvular aortic stenosis in association with mental retardation and a certain facial appearance. Circulation 26, 1235–40.Google Scholar
Bihrle, A M, Bellugi, U, Delis, S and Marks, S. 1989. Seeing the forest or the trees: Dissociation in visuospatial processing. Brain Cogn 11, 37–49.Google Scholar
Bishop, K M, Goudreau, G and O'Leary, D D. 2000. Regulation of area identity in the mammalian neocortex by Emx2 and Pax6. Science 288, 344–9.Google Scholar
Blomberg, S, Rosander, M and Andersson, G. 2006. Fears, hyperacusis and musicality in Williams syndrome. Res Devptl Disabil 27, 668–80.Google Scholar
Brock, J. 2007. Language abilities in Williams syndrome: A critical review. Devpt Psychopathol 19, 97–127.Google Scholar
Campbell, L E, Daly, E, Toal, F, et al. 2009. Brain structural differences associated with the behavioural phenotype in children with Williams syndrome. Brain Res 1258, 96–107.Google Scholar
Chapman, C A, Plessis, A and Pober, B R. 1996. Neurologic findings in children and adults with Williams syndrome. J Child Neurol 11, 63–5.Google Scholar
Cherniske, E M, Carpenter, T O, Klaiman, C, et al. 2004. Multisystem study of 20 older adults with Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet 131A, 255–64.Google Scholar
Chiang, M C, Reiss, A L, Lee, A D, et al. 2007. 3D pattern of brain abnormalities in Williams syndrome visualized using tensor-based morphometry. Neuroimage 36, 1096–109.Google Scholar
Danoff, S K, Taylor, H E, Blackshaw, S and Desiderio, S. 2004. TFII-I, a candidate gene for Williams syndrome cognitive profile: Parallels between regional expression in mouse brain and human phenotype. Neuroscience 123, 931–8.Google Scholar
Davachi, L, Mitchell, J P and Wagner, A D. 2003. Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 100, 2157–62.Google Scholar
Deruelle, C, Rondan, C, Livet, M O and Mancini, J. 2003. Exploring face processing in Williams syndrome. Cogn Brain Behav 7, 157–72.Google Scholar
Deruelle, C, Rondan, C, Mancini, J and Livet, M O. 2006. Do children with Williams syndrome fail to process visual configural information? Res Devptl Disabil 27, 243–53.Google Scholar
Deruelle, C, Schön, D, Rondan, C and Mancini, J. 2005. Global and local music perception in children with Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 16, 631–4.Google Scholar
Donnai, D and Karmiloff-Smith, A. 2000. Williams syndrome: From genotype through to the cognitive phenotype. Am J Med Genet Semin Med Genet 97, 164–71.Google Scholar
Dykens, E M. 2003. Anxiety, fears, and phobias in persons with Williams syndrome. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 291–316.Google Scholar
Eckert, M A, Galaburda, A M, Karchemskiy, A, et al. 2006a. Anomalous Sylvian fissure morphology in Williams syndrome. Neuroimage 33, 39–45.Google Scholar
Eckert, M A, Hu, D, Eliez, S, et al. 2005. Evidence for superior parietal impairment in Williams syndrome. Neurology 64, 152–3.Google Scholar
Eckert, M A, Tenforde, A, Galaburda, A M, et al. 2006b. To modulate or not to modulate: Differing results in uniquely shaped Williams syndrome brains. Neuroimage 32, 1001–07.Google Scholar
Einfield, S, Tonge, B and Florio, T. 1997. Behavioral and emotional disturbance in individuals with Williams syndrome. Am J Mental Retard 102, 45–53.Google Scholar
Epstein, R and Kanwisher, N. 1998. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392, 598–601.Google Scholar
Ewart, A K, Morris, C A, Atkinson, D, et al. 1993. Hemizygosity at the elastin locus in a developmental disorder, Williams syndrome. Nat Genet 5, 11–6.Google Scholar
Fagot, J and Deruelle, C. 1997. Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 23, 429–42.Google Scholar
Faravelli, F, D'Arrigo, S, Bagnasco, I, et al. 2003. Oligogyric microcephaly in a child with Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet A 117, 169–71.Google Scholar
Farran, E K and Jarrold, C. 2003. Visuospatial cognition in Williams syndrome: Reviewing and accounting for the strengths and weaknesses in performance. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 173–200.Google Scholar
Farran, E K, Jarrold, C and Gathercole, S E. 2001. Block design performance in the Williams syndrome phenotype: A problem with mental imagery? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42, 719–28.Google Scholar
Farran, E K, Jarrold, C and Gathercole, S E. 2003. Divided attention, selective attention and drawing: processing preferences in Williams syndrome are dependent on the task administered. Neuropsychologia 41, 676–87.Google Scholar
Fidler, D J, Hepburn, S L, Most, D E, Philofsky, A and Rogers, S J. 2007. Emotional responsivity in young children with Williams syndrome. Am J Mental Retard 112, 194–206.Google Scholar
Frangiskakis, J M, Ewart, A K, Morris, C A, et al. 1996. LIM-kinase1 hemizygosity implicated in impaired visuospatial constructive cognition. Cell 86, 59–69.Google Scholar
Frigerio, E, Burt, D M, Gagliardi, C, et al. 2006. Is everybody always my friend? Perception of approachability in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia 44, 254–9.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A M and Bellugi, U. 2000. V. Multi-level analysis of cortical neuroanatomy in Williams syndrome. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (Suppl 1), 74–88.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A M, Holinger, D P, Bellugi, U and Sherman, G F. 2002. Williams syndrome: neuronal size and neuronal-packing density in primary visual cortex. Arch Neurol 59, 1461–7.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A M, Schmitt, J E, Atlas, S W, Eliez, S, Bellugi, U and Reiss, A L. 2001. Dorsal forebrain anomaly in Williams syndrome. Arch Neurol 58, 1865–9.Google Scholar
Galaburda, A M, Wang, P P, Bellugi, U and Rossen, M. 1994. Cytoarchitectonic anomalies in a genetically based disorder: Williams syndrome. Neuroreport 5, 753–7.Google Scholar
Gaser, C, Luders, E, Thompson, P M, et al. 2006. Increased local gyrification mapped in Williams syndrome. Neuroimage 33, 46–54.Google Scholar
Gothelf, D, Searcy, Y M, Reilly, J, et al. 2008. Association between cerebral shape and social use of language in Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet 146, 2753–61.Google Scholar
Gosch, A and Pankau, R. 1994. Social–emotional and behavioral adjustment in children with Williams–Beuren syndrome. Am J Med Genet 52, 291–6.Google Scholar
Gosch, A and Pankau, R. 1997. Personality characteristics and behavioural problems in individuals of different ages with Williams syndrome. Devptl Med Child Neurol 39, 327–533.Google Scholar
Grill-Spector, K. 2003. The neural basis of object perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13, 159–66.Google Scholar
Gunn, A, Cory, E, Atkinson, J, et al. 2002. Dorsal and ventral stream sensitivity in normal development and hemiplegia. Neuroreport 13, 843–7.Google Scholar
Haas, B W, Mills, D, Yam, A, Hoeft, F, Bellugi, U and Reiss, A. 2009. Genetic influences on sociability: Heightened amygdala reactivity and event-related responses to positive social stimuli in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci 29, 1132–49.Google Scholar
Hammond, P, Hutton, T J, Allanson, J E, et al. 2005. Discriminating power of localized three-dimensional facial morphology. Am J Hum Genet 77, 999–1010.Google Scholar
Hansen, P C, Stein, J F, Orde, S R, Winter, J L and Talcott, J B. 2001. Are dyslexics' visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function? Neuroreport 12, 1527–30.Google Scholar
Hariri, A R, Tessitore, A, Mattay, V S, Fera, F and Weinberger, D R. 2002. The amygdala response to emotional stimuli: A comparison of faces and scenes. Neuroimage 17, 317–23.Google Scholar
Haxby, J V, Ungerleider, L G, Clark, V P, Schouten, J L, Hoffman, E A and Martin, A. 1999. The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 22, 189–99.Google Scholar
Hoeft, F, Barnea-Goraly, N, Haas, B W, et al. 2007. More is not always better: Increased fractional anisotropy of superior longitudinal fasciculus associated with poor visuospatial abilities in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci 27, 11 960–5.Google Scholar
Holinger, D P, Bellugi, U, Mills, D L, et al. 2005. Relative sparing of primary auditory cortex in Williams syndrome. Brain Res 1037, 35–42.Google Scholar
Hoogenraad, C C, Koekkoek, B, Akhmanova, A, et al. 2002. Targeted mutation of Cyln2 in the Williams syndrome critical region links CLIP-115 haploinsufficiency to neurodevelopmental abnormalities in mice. Nat Genet 32, 116–27.Google Scholar
Howlin, P, Davies, M and Udwin, O. 1998. Cognitive functioning in adults with Williams syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 39, 183–9.Google Scholar
Jackowski, A P and Schultz, R T. 2005. Foreshortened dorsal extension of the central sulcus in Williams syndrome. Cortex 41, 282–90.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C, Baddeley, A D and Hewes, A K. 1998. Verbal and nonverbal abilities in the Williams syndrome phenotype: Evidence for diverging developmental trajectories. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 39, 511–23.Google Scholar
Jernigan, T L and Bellugi, U. 1990. Anomalous brain morphology on magnetic resonance images in Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Arch Neurol 47, 529–33.Google Scholar
Jernigan, T L, Bellugi, U, Sowell, E, Doherty, S and Hesselink, J R. 1993. Cerebral morphologic distinctions between Williams and Down syndromes. Arch Neurol 50, 186–91.Google Scholar
Jones, W, Bellugi, U, Lai, Z, et al. 2000. Hypersociability in Williams syndrome. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (Suppl 1), 30–46.Google Scholar
Jones, W, Hesselink, J, Courschene, E, et al. 2002. Cerebellar abnormalities in infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome. Devptl Med Child Neurol 44, 688–94.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, N, McDermott, J and Chun, M M. 1997. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17, 4302–11.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1998. Development itself is the key to understanding developmental disorders. Trends Cogn Sci 2, 389–98.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A, Brown, J H, Grice, S and Paterson, S. 2003. Dethroning the myth: Cognitive dissociations and innate modularity in Williams syndrome. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 227–42.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A, Klima, E, Bellugi, U, Grant, J and Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Is there a social module? Language, face-processing and theory of mind in Williams syndrome. J Cogn Neurosci 7, 196–208.Google Scholar
Kimchi, R. 1990. Children's perceptual organization of hierarchical visual patterns. Eur J Cogn Psychol 2, 133–49.Google Scholar
Kippenhan, J S, Olsen, R K, Mervis, C B, et al. 2005. Genetic contributions to human gyrification: Sulcal morphometry in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci 25, 7840–6.Google Scholar
Klein-Tasman, B P and Mervis, C B. 2003. Distinctive personality characteristics of 8–9, and 10-year-olds with Williams syndrome. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 269–90.Google Scholar
Kogan, C S, Boutet, I, Cornish, K, et al. 2004. Differential impact of the FMR1 gene on visual processing in fragile X syndrome. Brain 127, 591–601.Google Scholar
Korenberg, J R, Chen, X N, Hirota, H, et al. 2000. IV. Genome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndrome. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (Suppl 1), 89–107.Google Scholar
Korenberg, J R, Dai, L, Bellugi, U, et al. 2008. Deletion of 7q11.23 Genes and Williams syndrome. In Epstein, C J, Erickson, R P, Wynshaw-Boris A (Eds.), Inborn Errors of Development (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ku, M, Sokol, S, Wu, J, Tussie-Luna, M, Roy, A and Hata, A. 2005. Positive, and negative regulation of the transforming growth factor beta/activin target gene goosecoid by the TFII-I family of transcripton factors. Mol Cell Biol 25, 7144–57.Google Scholar
Laing, E, Butterworth, G, Ansari, D, et al. 2002. Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Devptl Sci 5, 233–46.Google Scholar
Landau, B, Hoffman, J E and Kurz, N. 2006. Object recognition with severe spatial deficits in Williams syndrome: Sparing and breakdown. Cognition 100, 483–510.Google Scholar
Lawler, K A and Cowey, A. 1986. The effects of pretectal and superior collicular lesions on binocular vision. Exp Brain Res 63, 402–08.Google Scholar
Levitin, D J, Menon, V, Schmitt, J E, et al. 2003. Neural correlates of auditory perception in Williams syndrome: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 18, 74–82.Google Scholar
Levitin, D J, Cole, K, Chiles, M, Lai, Z, Lincoln, A and Bellugi, U. 2004. Characterizing the musical phenotype in individuals with Williams Syndrome. Child Neuropsychol 10, 223–47.Google Scholar
Levitin, D J, Cole, K, Lincoln, A and Bellugi, U. 2005. Aversion, awareness, and attraction: Investigating claims of hyperacusis in the Williams syndrome phenotype. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 46, 514–23.Google Scholar
Leyfer, O T, Woodruff-Borden, J, Klein-Tasman, B P, Fricke, J S and Mervis, C B. 2006. Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in 4 to 16-year-olds with Williams syndrome. Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatr Genet 141, 615–22.Google Scholar
Logothetis, N K. 1999. Vision: A window on consciousness. Sci Am 281, 69–75.Google Scholar
Lowery, M C, Morris, C A, Ewart, A, et al. 1995. Strong correlation of elastin deletions, detected by FISH, with Williams syndrome. Am J Hum Genet 57, 49–53.Google Scholar
Luders, E, Di Paola, M, Tomaiuolo, F, et al. 2007. Callosal morphology in Williams syndrome: A new evaluation of shape and thickness. Neuroreport 18, 203–07.Google Scholar
Marler, J A, Elfenbein, J L, Ryals, B M, Urban, Z and Netzloff, M L. 2005. Sensorineural hearing loss in children and adults with Williams syndrome. Am J Medl Genet 138, 318–27.Google Scholar
Marenco, S, Siuta, M A, Kippenhan, J S, et al. 2007. Genetic contributions to white matter architecture revealed by diffusion tensor imaging in Williams syndrome. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 15 117–222.Google Scholar
Mervis, C B and Klein-Tasman, B P. 2000. Williams syndrome: Cognition, personality, and adaptive behaviour. Mental Retard Devptl Disabil Res Rev 6, 148–58.Google Scholar
Mervis, C B, Morris, C A, Bertrand, J and Robinson, B F. 1999. Williams syndrome: Findings from an integrated program of research. In Tager-Flusberg, H (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Contributions to a New Framework from the Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 65–110.
Mervis, C B, Morris, C A, Klein-Tasman, B P, et al. 2003a. Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome during triadic interactions. Devptl Neuropsychol 23, 243–68.Google Scholar
Mervis, C B, Robinson, B, Bertrand, J, Morris, C A, Klein-Tasman, B and Armstrong, S. 2000. The Williams syndrome cognitive profile. Brain Cogn 44, 604–28.Google Scholar
Mervis, C B, Robinson, B F, Rowe, M L, Becerra, A M and Klein-Tasman, B P. 2003b. Language abilities of individuals with Williams syndrome. Int Rev Res Mental Retard 27, 35–81.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Hariri, A R, Munoz, K E, et al. 2005a. Neural correlates of genetically abnormal social cognition in Williams syndrome. Nat Neurosci 8, 991–3.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Kohn, P, Mervis, C B, et al. 2004. Neural basis of genetically determined visuospatial construction deficit in Williams syndrome. Neuron 43, 623–31.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Mervis, C B and Berman, K F. 2006. Neural mechanisms in Williams syndrome: A unique window to genetic influences on cognition and behaviour. Nat Neurosci 8, 991–3.Google Scholar
Meyer-Lindenberg, A, Mervis, C B, Sarpal, D, et al. 2005b. Functional, structural, and metabolic abnormalities of the hippocampal formation in Williams syndrome. J Clin Invest 115, 1888–95.Google Scholar
Milner, A D and Goodale, M A. 1995. The Visual Brain in Action. Cambridge, UK: MIT Press.
Mobbs, D, Eckert, M A, Menon, V, et al. 2007a. Reduced parietal and visual cortical activation during global processing in Williams syndrome. Devptl Med Child Neurol 49, 433–8.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D, Eckert, M A, Mills, D, et al. 2007b. Frontostriatal dysfunction during response inhibition in Williams syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 62, 256–61.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D, Garrett, A S, Menon, V, Rose, F E, Bellugi, U and Reiss, A L. 2004. Anomalous brain activations during face and gaze processing in Williams syndrome. Neurology 62, 2070–6.Google Scholar
Morris, C A. 2006. The dysmorphology, genetics, and natural history of Williams–Beuren syndrome. In Morris, C A, Lenhoff, H M, Wang, P P (Eds.), Williams–Beuren Syndrome: Research, Evaluation and Treatment. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, pp. 3–17.
Morris, C A, Demsey, S A, Leonard, C O, Dilts, C and Blackburn, B L. 1988. Natural history of Williams syndrome: Physical characteristics. J Ped 113, 318–26.Google Scholar
Morris, C A and Mervis, C B. 2000. Williams syndrome and related disorders. Annu Rev Genom Hum Genet 1, 461–84.Google Scholar
Nakamura, M, Watanabe, K, Matsumoto, A, et al. 2001. Williams syndrome and deficiency in visuospatial recognition. Devptl Med Child Neurol 43, 617–21.Google Scholar
Navon, D. 1977. Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn Psychol 9, 353–83.Google Scholar
Paul, B M, Stiles, J, Passarotti, A, Bavar, N and Bellugi, U. 2002. Face and place processing in Williams syndrome: Evidence for a dorsal–ventral dissociation. Neuroreport 13, 1115–9.Google Scholar
Pennington, B F, Filipek, P A, Lefly, D, et al. 2000. A twin MRI study of size variations in human brain. J Cogn Neurosci 12, 223–32.Google Scholar
Peoples, R, Franke, Y, Wang, Y K, et al. 2000. A physical map, including a BAC/PAC clone contig, of the Williams–Beuren syndrome – Deletion region at 7q11.23. Am J Hum Genet 66, 47–68.Google Scholar
Pober, B R and Filiano, J J. 1995. Association of Chiari I malformation and Williams syndrome. Ped Neurol 12, 84–8.Google Scholar
Prather, M D, Lavenex, P, Mauldin-Jourdain, M L, et al. 2001. Increased social fear and decreased fear of objects in monkeys with neonatal amygdala lesions. Neuroscience 106, 653–8.Google Scholar
Ranganath, C and D'Esposito, M. 2001. Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31, 865–73.Google Scholar
Reilly, J, Losh, M, Bellugi, U and Wulfeck, B. 2004. Frog, where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain Lang 88, 229–47.Google Scholar
Reiss, A L, Abrams, M T, Singer, H S, Ross, J L and Denckla, M B. 1996. Brain development, gender and IQ in children. A volumetric imaging study. Brain 119, 1763–74.Google Scholar
Reiss, A L, Eckert, M A, Rose, F E, et al. 2004. An experiment of nature: Brain anatomy parallels cognition and behaviour in Williams Syndrome. J Neurosci 24, 5009–15.Google Scholar
Reiss, A L, Eliez, S, Schmitt, J E, et al. 2000. IV. Neuroanatomy of Williams syndrome: A high-resolution MRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 12 (Suppl 1), 65–73.Google Scholar
Rondan, C, Santos, A, Mancini, J, Livet, M O and Deruelle, C. 2008. Global and local processing in Williams syndrome: Drawing versus perceiving. Child Neuropsychol 14, 237–48.Google Scholar
Santos, A, Milne, D, Rosset, D and Deruelle, C. 2007a. Challenging symmetry on mental retardation: Evidence from Williams syndrome. In Heinz, E B (Ed.), Mental Retardation Research Advances. New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers, pp. 147–74.
Santos, A, Rondan, C, Mancini, J and Deruelle, C. 2007b. Behavioural indexes of callosal functioning in Williams syndrome. J Neuropsychol 1, 189–200.Google Scholar
Sarpal, D, Buchsbaum, B R, Kohn, P D, et al. (2008). A genetic model for understanding higher order visual processing: Functional interactions of the ventral visual stream in Williams syndrome. Cerebral Cortex 18, 2402–09.Google Scholar
Scheiber, B. 2000. Fulfilling Dreams – Book 1. A Handbook for Parents of Williams Syndrome Children. Clawson, MI: Williams Syndrome Association.
Schmitt, J E, Eliez, S, Bellugi, U and Reiss, A L. 2001a. Analysis of cerebral shape in Williams syndrome. Arch Neurol 58, 283–7.Google Scholar
Schmitt, J E, Eliez, S, Warsofsky, I S, Bellugi, U and Reiss, A L. 2001b. Corpus callosum morphology of Williams syndrome: Relation to genetics and behaviour. Devptl Med Child Neurol 43, 155–9.Google Scholar
Schmitt, J E, Watts, K, Eliez, S, Bellugi, U, Galaburda, A M and Reiss, A L. 2002. Increased gyrification in Williams syndrome: Evidence using 3D MRI methods. Devptl Med Child Neurol 44, 292–5.Google Scholar
Schultz, R T. 2005. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face are. Int J Devptl Neurosci 23, 125–41.Google Scholar
Spencer, J, O'Brien, J, Riggs, K, Braddick, O, Atkinson, J and Wattam-Bell, J. 2000. Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. Neuroreport 11, 2765–7.Google Scholar
Strømme, P, Bjørnstad, P G and Ramstad, K. 2002. Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. J Child Neurol 17, 269–71.Google Scholar
Sukarova-Angelovska, E, Piperkova, K, Sredovska, A, Ilieva, G and Kocova, M. 2007. Implementation of fluorescent in situ hybridization (FISH) as a method for detecting microdeletion syndromes – Our first experiments. Prilozi 28, 87–98.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H, Plesa Skewerer, D, Faja, S and Joseph, R M. 2003. People with Williams syndrome processes faces holistically. Cognition 89, 11–24.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H and Sullivan, K. 2000. A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition 76, 59–90.Google Scholar
Thompson, P M, Cannon, T D, Narr, K L, et al. 2001. Genetic influences on brain structure. Nat Neurosci 4, 1253–8.Google Scholar
Thompson, P M, Lee, A D, Dutton, R A, et al. 2005. Abnormal cortical complexity and thickness profiles mapped in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci 25, 4146–58.Google Scholar
Tomaiuolo, F, Di Paola, M, Caravale, B, Vicari, S, Petrides, M and Caltagirone, C. 2002. Morphology and morphometry of the corpus callosum in Williams syndrome: A TI-weighted MRI study. Neuroreport 13, 2281–4.Google Scholar
Udwin, O and Yule, W. 1991. A cognitive and behavioural phenotype in Williams syndrome. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 13, 232–44.Google Scholar
Udwin, O, Yule, W and Martin, N. 1987. Cognitive abilities and behavioural characteristics of children with idiopathic infantile hypercalcaemia. J Child Psychol Psychiatry All Discipl 28, 297–309.Google Scholar
Ungerleider, L and Mishkin, M. 1982. Two cortical visual systems. In Ingle, D J, Goodale, M A, Mansfeld, R J W (Eds.), Analyses of visual behaviour. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 549–86.
Urbán, Z, Helms, C, Fekete, G, et al. 1996. 7q11.23 deletions in Williams syndrome arise as a consequence of unequal meiotic crossover. Am J Hum Genet 59, 958–62.Google Scholar
Essen, D.C, Anderson, C H and Felleman, D J. 1992. Information processing in the primate visual system: An integrated systems perspective. Science 255, 419–23.Google Scholar
Essen, D C, Dierker, D, Snyder, A Z, Raichle, M E, Reiss, A L and Korenberg, J. 2006. Symmetry of cortical folding abnormalities in Williams syndrome revealed by surface-based analyses. J Neurosci 26, 5470–83.Google Scholar
VanLieshout, C, DeMeyer, R, Curfs, L and Fryns, J. 1998. Family contexts, parental behavior, and personality profiles of children and adolescents with Prader-Willi, Fragile-X, or Williams syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry All Discipl 39, 699–710.Google Scholar
Vicari, S, Brizzolara, D, Carlesimo, G A, Pezzini, G and Volterra, V. 1996. Memory abilities in children with Williams syndrome. Cortex 32, 502–14.Google Scholar
Wang, P P and Bellugi, U. 1994. Evidence from two genetic syndromes for a dissociation between verbal and visual–spatial short-term memory. J Child Exp Neuropsychol 162, 317–22.Google Scholar
Wang, P P, Doherty, S, Hesselink, J R and Bellugi, U. 1992. Callosal morphology concurs with neurobehavioral and neuropathological findings in two neurodevelopmental disorders. Arch Neurol 49, 407–11.Google Scholar
Wang, P P, Doherty, S, Rourke, S B and Bellugi, U. 1995. Unique profile of visuo-perceptual skills in a genetic syndrome. Brain Cogn 29, 54–65.Google Scholar
Wang, P P, Hesselink, J R, Jernigan, T L, Doherty, S and Bellugi, U. 1992. Specific neurobehavioral profile of Williams' syndrome is associated with neocerebellar hemispheric preservation. Neurology 42, 1999–2002.Google Scholar
Wilke, M, Sohn, J H, Byars, A W and Holland, S K. 2003. Bright spots: Correlations of gray matter volume with IQ in a normal pediatric population. Neuroimage 20, 202–15.Google Scholar
Williams, J C P, Barret-Boyes, B G and Lowe, J B. 1961. Supravalvular aortic stenosis. Circulation 24, 1311–8.Google Scholar
Zeki, S. 2001. Localization and globalization in conscious vision. Annu Rev Neurosci 24, 57–86.Google Scholar
Zhao, C, Aviles, C, Abel, R A, Almli, C R, McQuillen, P and Pleasure, S J. 2005. Hippocampal and visuospatial learning defects in mice with a deletion of frizzled 9, a gene in the Williams syndrome deletion interval. Development 132, 2917–27.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×